ABSTRACT
Insects are well known for their ability to keep track of their heading direction based on a combination of skylight cues and visual landmarks. This allows them to navigate back to their nest, disperse throughout unfamiliar environments, as well as migrate over large distances between their breeding and non-breeding habitats. The monarch butterfly (Danaus plexippus), for instance, is known for its annual southward migration from North America to certain trees in Central Mexico. To maintain a constant flight route, these butterflies use a time-compensated sun compass, which is processed in a region in the brain, termed the central complex. However, to successfully complete their journey, the butterflies’ brain must generate a multitude of orientation strategies, allowing them to dynamically switch from sun-compass orientation to a tactic behavior toward a certain target. To study whether monarch butterflies exhibit different orientation modes and if they can switch between them, we observed the orientation behavior of tethered flying butterflies in a flight simulator while presenting different visual cues to them. We found that the butterflies' behavior depended on the presented visual stimulus. Thus, while a dark stripe was used for flight stabilization, a bright stripe was fixated by the butterflies in their frontal visual field. If we replaced a bright stripe with a simulated sun stimulus, the butterflies switched their behavior and exhibited compass orientation. Taken together, our data show that monarch butterflies rely on and switch between different orientation modes, allowing the animal to adjust orientation to its actual behavioral demands.
INTRODUCTION
Orientation in space is an essential ability for animals to find food, escape from predators or return to their nest. To achieve this, insects exhibit a number of different orientation mechanisms, ranging from the simple straight-line orientation of dung beetles (Dacke et al., 2021; el Jundi et al., 2019) to more complex behaviors such as path integration of ants and bees (Collett and Collett, 2000; Heinze et al., 2018) or long-distance migration of lepidopterans (Grob et al., 2021; Hu et al., 2021; Merlin and Liedvogel, 2019; Warrant et al., 2016). One striking example of a migrating insect is the monarch butterfly (Danaus plexippus) (Reppert and de Roode, 2018; Reppert et al., 2016). Each autumn, millions of these butterflies migrate from the northern USA and Canada over more than 4000 km to their overwintering habitat in Central Mexico. To keep a constant direction over this enormous distance, these animals rely on the sun for orientation (Froy et al., 2003; Heinze and Reppert, 2011; Mouritsen and Frost, 2002; Reppert, 2006). In combination with time-of-day information from circadian clocks in the brain (Sauman et al., 2005) and/or the antennae (Guerra et al., 2012; Merlin et al., 2009), this allows the butterflies to maintain a directed course throughout the day. Besides the sun, additional cues, such as the celestial polarization pattern (Reppert et al., 2004) or the Earth's magnetic field (Guerra et al., 2014; Wan et al., 2021) seem to play a role during the migration, but their relevance for the butterfly's compass is still not fully understood (Stalleicken et al., 2005).
As in other insects, the central complex of monarch butterflies serves as an internal compass during spatial orientation (el Jundi et al., 2014; Heinze and Reppert, 2011; Heinze et al., 2013; Pfeiffer and Homberg, 2014). Compass neurons in this brain region are sensitive to multiple simulated skylight cues (Heinze and Reppert, 2011; Nguyen et al., 2021) and encode the animal's heading with respect to a sun stimulus (Beetz et al., 2022). As shown previously, a sun stimulus – represented by a green light spot – can be employed in behavioral laboratory experiments in monarch butterflies (Franzke et al., 2020). Similar experiments in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster demonstrated that these insects exhibit a menotactic behavior with respect to a simulated sun. This means that the fruit fly maintains any arbitrary heading relative to the sun (Giraldo et al., 2018). Interestingly, closed-loop experiments showed that as soon as the activity of central-complex neurons was genetically deactivated, the flies kept the simulated sun in their frontal visual field, resembling vertical stripe fixation behavior (Giraldo et al., 2018). This attraction behavior does not depend on whether the flies are confronted with a bright stripe on a dark background or the inverted visual scene (Maimon et al., 2008). Although the biological function of the fly's attraction behavior is not fully understood, it is speculated that the flies interpret this cue as a landing or feeding site (Maimon et al., 2008). Whether monarch butterflies adjust their orientation strategy depending on the visual stimulus is not known. However, to successfully display a large repertoire of behaviors, the orientation network in the butterfly's brain needs to possess the capacity to flexibly switch between different orientation circuitries that may operate in parallel in the brain. This would, for instance, allow a flying butterfly to change from compass orientation based on skylight cues to attraction based on a visual landmark or an odor plume, similar to what has been found for homing desert ants (Buehlmann et al., 2013).
To study the monarch butterflies' behavioral repertoire, we recorded the orientation behavior of flying butterflies, tethered at the center of an LED flight simulator, while we provided different visual cues (dark stripe, bright stripe, and sun stimulus) to the animals. We found that the butterflies used the dark stripe for flight stabilization based on optic-flow information. A bright stripe, on the other hand, evoked a simple attraction behavior towards the stimulus. In contrast, a simulated sun was used by the butterflies to maintain a constant angle with respect to the stimulus. We furthermore found that the butterflies switched between compass orientation and attraction behavior during flight. Taken together, our results show that monarch butterflies display different orientation modes that allow them to dynamically switch between different behaviors while navigating through their environment.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Experimental animals
Pupae of the monarch butterfly Danaus plexippus (Linnaeus 1758) were ordered from Costa Rica Entomology Supply (butterflyfarm.co.cr) and kept in an incubator (HPP 110 and HPP 749, Memmert GmbH+Co. KG, Schwabach, Germany) at 25°C and 80% relative humidity and under a 12 h:12 h light:dark cycle. After the animals eclosed, the adult butterflies were transferred to a flight cage inside a separate incubator (I-30VL, Percival Scientific, Perry, IA, USA) with a 12 h:12 h light:dark cycle. While the relative humidity was constant at about 50%, the temperature was set to 25°C during light phases and 23°C during dark phases. Feeders inside the flight cage were filled with 15% sucrose solution and provided ad libitum food to the butterflies.
Preparation
We used female and male adult butterflies (2–3 weeks after eclosion) and prepared them in the morning prior to each experiment. We removed the scales of the butterflies' thorax and glued (multi-purpose impact instant contact adhesive, EVO-STIK, Bostik Ltd, Stafford, UK) a tungsten stalk (0.508×152.4 mm, Science Products GmbH, Hofheim, Germany) to the dorsal side. After preparation, the animals were individually kept in clear plastic containers with access to 15% sucrose solution and transferred to a dark chamber for at least 3 h. For each experiment, a new group of butterflies was used, except for the ‘dark stripe’, ‘no cue’ and ‘bright stripe’ experiments, where 20 animals experienced at least two stimulus conditions.
Flight simulator
We used a flight simulator similar to the ones described previously (Dreyer et al., 2018a,b, 2021). To record the heading directions of individual butterflies, the tungsten wire on the animals' thorax was connected to an optical encoder (E4T miniature Optical Kit Encoder, US Digital, Vancouver, WA, USA). This allowed the butterflies to rotate at the center of the flight simulator and freely choose any heading. Butterflies that stopped flying more than four times during an experiment were excluded from the study. The heading direction of the animals was recorded with an angular resolution of 3 deg and a temporal resolution of 200 ms using a data acquisition device (USB4 Encoder Data Acquisition USB Device, US Digital, Vancouver, WA, USA) and a computer with the corresponding software (USB1, USB4: US Digital, Vancouver, WA, USA). To present visual stimuli to the butterflies, the inner surface of the flight simulator was equipped with a circular array of 2048 RGB LEDs (128*16 APA102C LED Matrix, iPixel LED Light Co., Ltd, Baoan Shenzhen, China) or green high-power LEDs (LZ1-00G102, OSRAM, San Jose, CA, USA). A custom-written python script was used to control the color and intensity of all LEDs of the LED arena via a Raspberry Pi 3 Model B (Raspberry Pi Foundation, UK).
Stimuli
In all experiments, we presented the butterflies with one or multiple different visual stimuli. To produce them, the intensity and color of each LED of the arena was adjusted as summarized in Table 1.
Orientation with respect to one cue
In the first set of experiments, we presented one cue (stripe or simulated sun) as an orientation reference to the butterflies. In all experiments, the heading direction of a single animal was recorded over 8 min. To ensure that the butterflies used the displayed cue for orientation, we turned the visual scenery by 180 deg every 2 min and studied if the animals followed the relocation of the cue. We alternated the start position of the stimulus between 0 deg and 180 deg, starting at 0 deg for half of the animals and at 180 deg for the other half.
First, we investigated whether monarch butterflies can use a landmark for orientation by setting all LEDs of the arena to blue while three LED columns were turned off which generated a dark stripe on a bright background (experiment: ‘dark stripe’). 28 butterflies were then individually connected via the tungsten wire to the encoder at the arena's center and were allowed to orient by changing their heading direction with respect to the landmark. As a control, we also performed an experiment with 22 individuals, in which the animals did not perceive any visual cue for orientation. Therefore, all LEDs were set to blue (experiment: ‘no cue’; same data as in Franzke et al., 2020). Next, we inverted the visual scenery by turning all LEDs off with the exception of three LED columns which were set to blue. We recorded the headings of 22 butterflies presented with this bright stripe (experiment: ‘bright stripe’).
Finally, we investigated which orientation strategy monarch butterflies display when they were flying with respect to a simulated sun. Previous studies revealed that a green light cue is interpreted as the direction towards the sun by several insects (Edrich et al., 1979; el Jundi et al., 2015; Rossel and Wehner, 1984). Therefore, we presented a simulated sun in the form of one bright green LED to 20 animals (experiment: ‘sun stimulus’). To test if the spectral content of our stimuli (green sun stimulus vs. blue stripe) had an impact on the orientation behavior of the butterflies, we repeated the experiments with the bright stripe with 20 butterflies. This time, the stripe changed its color every 2 min of flight from green to blue and vice versa (Fig. 3G,H). Again, the position and color of the stimulus was alternated between each butterfly. This means a quarter of the animals first experienced a green stripe at 0 deg (green stripe 0/blue stripe 180/green stripe 0/blue stripe 180 deg) while a quarter of the butterflies started with a green stripe at 180 deg first (green stripe 180/blue stripe 0/green stripe 180/blue stripe 0 deg). The remaining animals perceived the blue stripe at either 0 deg (blue stripe 0/green stripe 180/blue stripe 0/green stripe 180 deg) or 180 deg first (blue stripe 180/green stripe 0/blue stripe 180/green stripe 0 deg).
In a final step, we investigated if the butterflies changed their orientation behavior when we changed the stimulus during a butterfly's directed flight. Therefore, we repeated the experiment with the blue versus green stripe with 33 animals. However, instead of changing the color, this time we changed the appearance of the stimulus presenting a bright, green stripe for 1 min followed by a sun stimulus for another 1 min phase (or vice versa). The animals' orientation was recorded over 2 min and the position and stimulus order (sun stimulus to stripe, stripe to sun stimulus) was alternated for each butterfly.
Orientation within an ambiguous scenery
In the second set of experiments, we presented two identical stimuli to the butterflies, that were set 180 deg apart from each other. In all experiments, the position of the stimuli was relocated, this time by 90 deg, every 2 min for a total of 8 min. We alternated the start position of the stimulus between 0 and 180 deg and 90 and 270 deg, starting at 0 and 180 deg for half of the animals and at 90 and 270 deg for the other half. In the first experiment, we tested the orientation behavior of 18 butterflies with respect to two dark vertical stripes. All LEDs of the arena were turned blue and to generate the stripes, two sets of three LED columns were turned off (experiment: ‘two dark stripes’). Next, we recorded the headings of 18 butterflies presented with two bright stripes on a dark background. For this experiment, all LEDs were turned off and the stripes were generated by turning two sets of three LED columns blue (experiment: ‘two bright stripes’). In addition, we tested 19 butterflies with respect to two artificial sun stimuli. Therefore, two LEDs at an elevation of about 23 deg were turned green (experiment: ‘two sun stimuli’).
Data analysis
Heading directions were calculated by importing the data into the software MATLAB (v. R2017b, MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) and analyzing it using the CircStat toolbox (Berens, 2009). As we changed the position of the visual stimulus every 2 min, we divided the 8 min flights of the butterflies into four equal 2 min sections and the 4 min flights (stripe vs. sun stimulus) into two sections. The flight trace of each butterfly, the mean vector within each 10 s bin and within a section (2 min or 1 min bins) was calculated. As the animals' directedness can increase over the first 4 min of an experiment (Franzke et al., 2020), we focused on the butterfly's flight performance in the last two flight sections (i.e. the last 4 min) of each experiment. Thus, the change of direction was measured as the angular difference between the mean heading directions taken during the last two flight sections. We then related all recorded heading angles relative to the stimulus position (stimulus position=0 deg) and calculated the mean heading vector over the last 4 min. To analyze if the animals maintain a directed flight course over a shorter time period, we counted the number of 10 s bins that exceeded a directedness of r=0.249 (which is the mean vector strength+95% confidence interval for 10 s bins in the last 4 min of the no cue experiment). For another detailed analysis of the heading distribution, we counted how often each animal kept every angle (in 3 deg bins) relative to the stimulus position. We defined the heading direction with the most counts as the animals' preferred angle and normalized the number of counts of all other headings to this value. The normalized heading counts were plotted in relation to the stimulus (stimulus position=0 deg) to generate a heatmap. For a better visualization of a bimodal or unimodal distribution of headings, we plotted the normalized heading counts in relation to the animals preferred heading. To test the butterfly's performance in the presence of the ambiguous stimuli, we analyzed the flight trajectories over the last 4 min with a temporal resolution of 2 s. We then selected all flight sections in which the animals maintained a straight flight (<45 deg change in heading) over at least 4 s. We then categorized the subsequent change in flight direction according to the angular change in heading: if a butterfly changed its heading direction between 140 and 220 deg before returning to a straight flight course, this was categorized as a ‘half turn’. In contrast, a ‘half turn’ was defined when an animal changed its heading by more than 90 deg and the next phase of oriented flight deviated by less than 40 deg from the original direction. Based on these data, we calculated a turning index by subtracting the number of ‘half turns’ from the number of ‘full turns’ and then dividing this value by the sum of all turns. Thus, animals with a positive turning index performed more ‘full turns’ while a negative turning index represents more ‘half turns’. All butterflies that performed neither ‘full turns’ nor ‘half turns’ were excluded from this analysis.
Statistics
The non-parametric Moore's Modified Rayleigh test (Moore, 1980) was used to test for a bias of heading directions within a flight sector. Furthermore, we compared the heading directions of different butterfly groups with the Mardia–Watson–Wheeler test. In the experiment where we changed the color of the bright stripe, we used the v-test to test whether the butterflies kept the same heading after the stimulus manipulation. To compare the performance of the butterflies, we first calculated the mean vector strength within the last 4 min and statistically compared them using a Kruskal–Wallis test for samples of different groups, the Wilcoxon signed-rank test for samples of the same individuals, or a linear mixed model ANOVA when the same butterflies participated in more than one condition. To test whether the mean vector strength in ten-second bins increased over time, we used the Wilcoxon signed-rank test. Additionally, we compared the number of 10 s bins with a vector strength above 0.249 of different animals using a Kruskal–Wallis test or a linear mixed model ANOVA. To test whether butterflies presented with two bright stripes, or two sun stimuli differed in their amount of ‘full turns’ versus ‘half turns’, we compared the turning indices of both experimental groups with the Kruskal–Wallis test.
RESULTS
Landmark orientation to a vertical stripe
To investigate how monarch butterflies use a local landmark for orientation, we performed flight-simulator experiments in which individual animals were tethered at the center of an LED arena (Fig. 1A). We first presented a dark vertical stripe on a blue background to the butterflies. Although the animals were only weakly oriented, we observed sequences in which they kept a certain heading over a short time (Fig. 1B, top panel; Movie 1). This was different from the butterflies' behavior in a scene without any cue (Fig. 1B, middle panel). To quantify if the butterflies more often maintained constant heading directions when they had the vertical stripe as a reference, we calculated the vector strength of the mean orientation vector for each 10 s segment of the entire flight for each animal (Fig. 1C). This value ranges from 0 to 1 and indicates how well a butterfly maintained its flight course (with 0 being completely disoriented and 1 being perfectly directed). When the butterflies had the vertical stripe for orientation, they showed a vector strength of about 0.2±0.1 in the first 10 s of their flight that increased significantly to a vector strength of 0.5±0.2 in the last 10 s of the flight (P=0.007, Z=−2.710, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; N=28; Fig. 1C, top panel). Without any orientation cue, the butterflies showed a vector strength of 0.2±0.1 (bin size: 10 s) that did not increase throughout the experiment (Fig. 1C, N=22, middle panel). This performance was significantly worse than when the vertical stripe was available as an orientation cue comparing the vector strength over the last two phases (P<0.001, F=22.788, linear mixed model ANOVA; bin size: 4 min; Fig. 1E). The higher vector strength with the vertical stripe was a result of significantly more oriented phases with r>0.249 (see Materials and Methods; P<0.001, F=13.450, linear mixed model ANOVA; bin size: 10 s; Nno cue=22, Ndark stripe=28; Fig. 1F).
We also analyzed if the butterflies changed their heading when the position of the dark vertical stripe was moved by 180 deg and found that 18 out of 28 butterflies followed the stimulus relocation (directional change>90 deg; Fig. 1D, top panel). In contrast, most of the butterflies (18 of 22) tested without an orientation reference did not change their heading in a meaningful way (Fig. 1D, middle panel). In summary, our data suggest that the butterflies can use a dark stripe to maintain a directed flight course.
We next tested how the butterflies use a bright stripe for orientation by inverting the visual scenery (i.e. a bright vertical stripe on a dark background). In contrast to the flight behavior with the dark stripe, many butterflies kept a constant heading over a longer time window or even over the entire 8-min flight (Fig. 1B, bottom panel; Movie 2). This higher orientation performance was also reflected in the animals' vector strength which significantly increased from about 0.3±0.1 at the beginning to a maximum of 0.65±0.3 at the end of the experiment (P<0.001, Z=−3.230, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; bin size: 10 s; N=22; Fig. 1C, bottom panel). The vector strength of the last 4 min of flight was significantly higher than when the butterflies had the dark vertical stripe for orientation (P<0.001, F=13.440, linear mixed model ANOVA; bin size: 4 min; Fig. 1E). Similarly, the number of oriented phases was significantly higher with the bright stripe as an orientation reference (P=0.012, F=6.844, linear mixed model ANOVA; bin size: 10 s; Fig. 1F). As expected, most of the animals (20 of 22) followed the relocation of the bright stripe (Fig. 1D, bottom panel) when we changed the position by 180 deg.
To gain insight into why the butterflies' performance was different between the two experiments (bright vs. dark stripe), we next analyzed the heading directions of butterflies within the two sceneries (Fig. 2). Interestingly, animals that were tested either without a cue (Fig. 2A) or with a dark stripe (Fig. 2B) headed in all possible directions. Calculating the mean direction for each butterfly within the last 4 min relative to the dark stripe showed that the butterflies maintained arbitrary heading directions (P=0.996, R*=0.038, non-parametric Moore's modified Rayleigh test; Ndark stripe=28; Fig. 2D). However, as no butterfly maintained its direction over a longer flight sequence with the dark stripe, we studied the heading directions of the animals on a finer scale and selected only flight sequences in which the butterflies maintained a stable heading over a time window of 10 s. Even when we studied this, we found that the butterflies' short-term headings were randomly distributed (P=0.506, Z=0.681, Rayleigh test; bin size: 10 s; Fig. 2F), suggesting that they did not keep headings towards the stimulus. This was different from the butterflies' behavior when a bright stripe was presented (Fig. 2C). Here, we found that most of the well-oriented animals flew in the direction of the bright stripe (P=0.006, R*=1.296, non-parametric Moore's modified Rayleigh test; Nbright stripe=22; Fig. 2C,E), suggesting that the animals were attracted by the stimulus. This stripe attraction was also observable when we analyzed stable heading directions over short flight sections (P<0.001, Z=43.542, Rayleigh test; bin size: 10 s; Fig. 2G). Taken together, these results suggest that monarch butterflies display different behavioral strategies depending on the contrast between a vertical stripe and its background. While a dark stripe leads to several short phases of constant headings in arbitrary directions, a bright stripe allows the butterflies to maintain constant headings towards the stripe over long phases.
Compass orientation with respect to a sun stimulus
We next wondered how monarch butterflies use a simulated sun for orientation. We therefore conducted an experiment with a green light spot as a simulated sun stimulus. Animals tested with respect to this stimulus kept constant headings over the entire experiment (Fig. 3A; Movie 3), although the butterflies directedness (as measured by the vector strength) significantly increased over time, from about 0.3±0.1 at the beginning of the flight up to a maximum of 0.7±0.3 at the end of the experiment (P=0.005, Z=−2.800, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; N=20; bin size: 10 s; Fig. 3B). The vector strengths over the last 4 min of the butterflies that oriented with the sun stimulus were in the same range as the ones that had the bright vertical stripe for orientation (Kruskal–Wallis test: P=0.801, χ2=0.06; bin size: 4 min; Fig. 3C). Similarly, the number of oriented phases were not significantly different between the sun-stimulus and the bright-stripe experiment (P=0.129, χ2=2.31, Kruskal–Wallis test; bin size: 10 s; Fig. 3D). Although most of the individuals (13 of 20) changed their heading by more than 90 deg when we changed the position of the sun stimulus by 180 deg (Fig. 1E), they did not keep this stimulus in their frontal visual field. Thus, the butterflies' heading directions were uniformly distributed (P=0.130, R*=0.825, non-parametric Moore's modified Rayleigh test; N=20; Fig. 3F). This suggests that monarch butterflies can maintain any desired compass direction with respect to a sun stimulus. This difference between how butterflies treated the sun stimulus and the bright stripe was not a consequence of a difference in the spectral content (blue stripe vs. green sun): when we changed the stripe color (from green to blue and vice versa) every 2 min, the butterflies showed well-oriented flights, irrespective of the stripe color (Fig. 3G,I) and did not change their heading relative to the bright stripe (P=0.001, u=3.047, v-test, expectation: 0 deg; Fig. 3H). In contrast, the mean direction of butterflies tested with a green stripe differed significantly from the sun-stimulus heading distribution (P=0.02, W=7.823, Mardia–Watson–Wheeler test). This indicates that the butterflies ignore the spectral content of the cue and are attracted by the brightness of the stripe whereas a sun stimulus is used for compass orientation.
Orientation in an ambiguous scenery
Our previous experiments suggest that monarch butterflies may exhibit different orientation strategies depending on the appearance of a visual stimulus: they likely use a dark vertical stripe to maintain constant courses over short flight periods, whereas a bright stripe evokes an attraction behavior. In contrast, a simulated sun is used for a menotactic behavior, i.e. for compass orientation. Interestingly, compass orientation requires the activity of the central-complex network in fruit flies, which is not necessary for an attraction towards a stripe (Giraldo et al., 2018). To investigate in more detail whether the butterflies use different visual orientation strategies and if the central complex is likely involved in coding them, we next performed experiments within ambiguous visual scenes (two dark stripes, two bright stripes, two sun stimuli; Fig. 4). We expected that the butterflies would maintain a distinct compass heading within such ambiguous sceneries if the heading-direction network of the central complex controls the orientation behavior (Beetz et al., 2022). When we provided two dark stripes as landmarks to the butterflies, their performance resembled the performance with one dark stripe. They showed short sections of straight flights in all possible heading directions that were interrupted by rapid rotations (Fig. 4A; N=18). Again, these findings support our observation that monarch butterflies use the dark stripe/s for flight stabilization rather than for compass orientation. When the butterflies oriented with two bright stripes, they maintained a constant heading towards one of the stripes. However, they frequently switched their fixation between the stripes by changing their heading by ∼180 deg (example highlighted in dark gray in Fig. 4B, left panel). Consequently, the flight bearings were clustered around 0 and around 180 deg (Fig. 4B, right panel) which resulted in a bimodal distribution of heading directions relative to the positions of the stripes (Fig. 4D, second panel; N=18). When the butterflies were provided with the two simulated suns, they maintained arbitrary headings similar to the situation with one sun stimulus (Fig. 4C, left panel). This confirms our observation that they employ compass orientation with respect to light spots (Fig. 4C, right panel). However, we also noticed that the butterflies returned to their original bearing or headed into the opposite direction when they deviated from their course. This led to a bimodal distribution of heading directions with the second peak being less pronounced than in the two-bright-stripe experiment (Fig. 4D, third panel; N=19). To quantify if the butterflies more often returned to their original bearing when they viewed the two suns, we calculated a turning index for every butterfly. A negative turning index indicated a higher amount of 180 deg (half) turns while a positive turning index marked a higher ratio of returns to the original bearing (full turns). We found that the turning index was significantly higher with the two suns than with the two bright stripes (P=0.004, χ2=8.48, Kruskal–Wallis test; Ntwo bright stripes=17, Ntwo sun stimuli=15; Fig. 4E). This suggests that the butterflies return to their original bearing more often when they had the two suns for orientation, a behavior that is expected if the heading-direction network of the butterfly's central complex controls the flight direction.
Compass orientation vs stripe attraction
As our previous experiment suggests that the butterflies' orientation modes depended on the stimulus properties, we next wondered if the butterflies rapidly switch their orientation behavior if we changed the visual scene from a sun to a stripe stimulus (and vice versa). Again, when we presented a bright stripe to the butterflies, they fixated the stimulus in their frontal visual field (Fig. 5A, left panel). Interestingly, when we changed the stimulus to a simulated sun instead, the butterflies changed their heading direction and adopted arbitrary bearings with respect to the sun stimulus (Fig. 5A, right panel). The headings taken with respect to the sun stimulus were significantly different from the headings with respect to the bright stripe (P=0.002, W=12.63, Mardia–Watson–Wheeler test). Taken together, this shows that the butterflies can flexibly change their orientation strategy from compass orientation to stripe attraction during flight.
DISCUSSION
We here tested the ability of monarch butterflies to use different visual stimuli to maintain a directed flight course and found that they exhibit different orientation modes that depend on the stimulus identity. While the butterflies used the dark stripe to stabilize their flight, they exhibited a strong attraction to the bright stripe. In contrast to these rather simple strategies, a simulated sun evoked compass orientation. This suggests that different strategies operate in parallel in the brain (Fig. 5B) which allows monarch butterflies to effectively adapt their orientation strategy to a certain behavior by dynamically switching to the most appropriate strategy during flight.
Orientation to local cues
A bright stripe triggered an attraction behavior in monarch butterflies. We interpret this behavior as a brightness-based flight approach with the intention to leave the current setting and access a new environment similarly to what has been found in navigating orchid bees (Baird and Dacke, 2016). This would also be in line with our observation that the behavior does not seem to be affected by the stripe's spectral information. However, instead of centering the bright stripe accurately in their frontal visual field, many butterflies kept the stripe slightly to their left–right vertical body axis. This indicates that the butterflies rely on the edge between the stripe and the background to sustain a constant heading, as observed in walking Lucilia flies (Osorio et al., 1990). The stripe fixation of the butterflies that we described corresponds with results reported for tethered flying Drosophila, which are also attracted by a bright stripe (Giraldo et al., 2018; Maimon et al., 2008). However, the fly's positive taxis seem to be dependent on the behavioral or locomotory state. Thus, walking fruit flies can also adopt arbitrary headings with respect to a bright stripe (Green et al., 2019). Interestingly, in flying and walking fruit flies (Götz, 1987; Horn and Wehner, 1975; Strauss and Pichler, 1998) and other insects, such as flying locusts (Baker, 1979; Robert, 1988) or naïve walking ants (Buehlmann et al., 2020), a dark stripe also elicits stripe fixation. In contrast, monarch butterflies used the dark stripe to occasionally maintain a bearing over short phases in a random direction. This result is similar to what has been reported for monarch butterflies in a more complex visual scene, where they had the panoramic skyline for orientation (Franzke et al., 2020). Interestingly, in the same study, the butterflies showed a similar behavior when they experienced a grating pattern – providing rotational optic flow – as the only visual input in a flight simulator (Franzke et al., 2020). Such rotational optic flow can provide an animal with directional information relative to a visual cue to perform compensatory steering and to keep a certain bearing (Wolf and Heisenberg, 1990; Zeil, 1996; Zeil et al., 2008). Thus, instead of using the vertical dark stripe to maintain a desired heading over an entire flight, our data suggest that the butterflies use optic-flow information to stabilize their heading over short flight sequences. In summary, a dark stripe evokes a different behavior in monarch butterflies than a bright stripe, which stands in contrast to Drosophila. It will be interesting to observe in the future at which visual angle of the bright stripe the butterflies will switch to an attraction behavior, and at which stripe width flight control will dominate the orientation behavior in monarch butterflies.
Sun compass orientation
When we presented a simulated sun to the butterflies, they kept arbitrary headings relative to the stimulus. This menotactic behavior is in line with what has been reported for other insects such as fruit flies (Giraldo et al., 2018) or dung beetles (Byrne et al., 2003). In theory, menotaxis can be carried out by a simple, vision-based retina matching of the current and remembered sun position similar to how many insects can use the profile of a panoramic skyline for orientation (Cartwright and Collett, 1983; Collett, 1992; Junger, 1991; Lent et al., 2010; Wehner and Räber, 1979). However, when we provided the butterflies with two simulated suns set 180 deg apart as orientation references, they returned to their original bearing during flight, which shows that they compute a distinct heading direction with respect to the ambiguous visual scene. This observation suggests that they not only rely on the azimuth of the sun stimulus for orientation but that their orientation mechanism also requires the involvement of the activity of a multisensory heading-direction network. This raises the question of what exactly defines the green light spot as a compass cue. In a recent paper, the butterflies' flight headings were directed towards the sun stimulus when the elevation of the sun stimulus was set to a low elevation of about 5 deg (Franzke et al., 2020). Even though the contrast between the background and the sun stimulus in Franzke et al. (2020) might have led to these heading choices, it opens up the possibility that the elevation of the sun stimulus is a critical parameter to induce compass orientation. In addition, for maintaining a certain heading direction, compass orientation also requires the network to memorize the desired direction (Grob et al., 2021; Honkanen et al., 2019). Whether the monarch butterfly can develop a long-term memory for a direction relative to the sun stimulus, as shown in the fruit fly (Giraldo et al., 2018) awaits to be investigated. Similarly, our future studies will focus on the use of the sun stimulus in the context of migration. Rather than adopting arbitrary headings, we expect that migratory monarch butterflies keep directed courses with respect to the sun stimulus that would guide them to the migratory destination. Moreover, as the butterflies employ a time-compensated sun compass during their migration (Merlin et al., 2009; Mouritsen and Frost, 2002), we will next study if the heading to the sun stimulus will be adjusted according to the time of day. Taken together, our findings show, that monarch butterflies use a sun stimulus for compass orientation, a strategy that allows them to maintain any arbitrary heading with respect to the sun during dispersal or in a distinct southward direction when they are in their migratory stage.
Neuronal network behind orientation
Our experiments suggest that the butterfly brain generates different orientation strategies but how is this accomplished at the neuronal level? As the butterflies used the dark stripe for flight control, the neuronal basis for it likely lies in the motion vision center, the lobula plate of the optic lobe (Meier and Borst, 2019; Ullrich et al., 2015). Although some optic-flow information is integrated into the central complex in locusts and bees (Rosner et al., 2019; Stone et al., 2017), the relevant information for flight control is directly transferred to the thoracic ganglia via descending pathways (Suver et al., 2016). In fruit flies, attraction does not require the activity of the central complex (Giraldo et al., 2018). This is well in line with our results from the two bright stripes experiment, which points towards a coding of directional information without the association of a multisensory heading-direction network in monarch butterflies. Thus, the basis for the attraction to a bright stripe might also be based on the motion-vision network that is directly connected to descending neurons, as suggested in a recent model (Fenk et al., 2014) (Fig. 5B). In contrast, the butterflies resolved the ambiguity of the visual scene, when we instead presented two suns as stimuli for orientation. This matches recordings from the heading-direction network in the butterfly central complex that encodes an explicit heading based on multisensory inputs if confronted with a similar two sun stimulus (Beetz et al., 2022). Thus, our behavioral data suggest that the central complex encodes the sun stimulus, which is also in line with the sensitivity of central complex neurons to a green light spot (Heinze and Reppert, 2011; Nguyen et al., 2021). Recent results suggest that the central complex compares the actual heading direction with the desired direction (Green et al., 2019; Stone et al., 2017). By encoding the desired migratory direction, the butterfly's central complex is likely taking a central role in the migration and is the region in the brain where time-of-day information becomes relevant for sustaining the migratory southward direction. We therefore propose that compass orientation is processed by the central complex, whereas stripe attraction and flight control seem to rely on reflexive pathways without the involvement of a higher brain center (Fig. 5B). Our results here show that the butterflies can switch between compass orientation and attraction. Information from the central complex is sent to the lateral accessory lobe and further to the posterior protocerebrum in monarch butterflies (Heinze et al., 2013) where it might converge with the attraction and flight control pathways (Fig. 5B). Interestingly, recent results in the fruit fly suggest that descending neurons can generate different steering commands based on different input pathways (Rayshubskiy et al., 2020 preprint). This suggests that the reliance on different orientation strategies might also be weighted and governed by descending neurons in monarch butterflies, which allows butterflies to rapidly switch their orientation strategy during flight. This enables them to flexibly switch from a long-distance system during dispersal or migration to a short-distance orientation strategy such as the attraction to their host plant.
Acknowledgements
We thank Jerome Beetz, James Foster, Tu Anh Thi Nguyen and Anna Stöckl for fruitful discussions on the manuscript. We are grateful to Konrad Öchsner for his help in designing the LED arena as well as the LED band for simulating the sun. We also thank the mechanics workshop of the Biocenter (University of Würzburg) for building important pieces of the flight simulator. In addition, we would like to thank Sergio Siles (butterflyfarm.co.cr) and Marie Gerlinde Blaese for providing us with monarch butterfly pupae.
Footnotes
Author contributions
Study design: M.F., K.P., B.e.J.; Conducting experiments: M.F., M.G., C.K.; Analysis of data: M.F., D.D., B.e.J.; Interpretation of data: M.F., K.P., B.e.J.; Drafting of the manuscript: M.F., B.e.J.; Critical review of the manuscript: C.K., M.G., D.D., K.P.; Acquired Funding: B.e.J. All authors approved of the final version of the manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by the Emmy Noether program of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft granted to B.e.J. (EL784/1-1) and an individual research grant by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft to K.P. (PF 714/5-1). Open Access funding provided by Julius-Maximilians-Universität Würzburg. Deposited in PMC for immediate release.
Data availability
Raw data can be downloaded from https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.17082701. Analysis scripts can be obtained from the corresponding author upon request.
References
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing or financial interests.