Dominance-based social hierarchies are common among teleost fishes. The rank of an animal greatly affects its behaviour, physiology and development. The outcome of fights for social dominance is affected by heritable factors and previous social experience. Divergent stress-coping styles have been demonstrated in a large number of teleosts, and fish displaying a proactive coping style have an advantage in fights for social dominance. Coping style has heritable components, but it appears to be largely determined by environmental factors, especially social experience. Agonistic behaviour is controlled by the brain's social decision-making network, and its monoaminergic systems play important roles in modifying the activity of this neuronal network. In this Review, we discuss the development of dominance hierarchies, how social rank is signalled through visual and chemical cues, and the neurobiological mechanisms controlling or correlating with agonistic behaviour. We also consider the effects of social interactions on the welfare of fish reared in captivity.

A dominance hierarchy describes a social situation in which animals are physically or chemically (e.g. urinary signals) dominant over other animals in their social group (Breed and Moore, 2012; Tibbetts et al., 2022). The prevalence of dominance hierarchies in nature suggests they are an evolutionary solution to overt fighting once the hierarchy is stable. This allows group-living animals to coexist with one another harmoniously if individuals know their place and do not engage higher-ranked individuals in aggressive interactions. Therefore, social animals often develop dominance-based social hierarchies and an individual's social rank greatly affects behaviour, development and physiology (Cubitt et al., 2008). Living within a group presents many advantages as there are more individuals to locate food, identify predators and dilute the predation risk, but social rank can influence the differential distribution of resources dependent upon dominance status (Culbert et al., 2019). Being subject to aggressive interactions which the individual does not win may lead to a chronic stress response where the animal perceives the constant threat from a higher ranked or dominant individual as a danger disrupting homeostasis. Therefore, low social rank may result in chronic stress and lower-ranking animals (i.e. not dominant) typically show submissive behaviour and a general behavioural inhibition. Dominant animals, on the other hand, show the opposite behavioural profile: they are aggressive, active, highly competitive and monopolize limited resources such as food, mates and shelter. The hierarchy is often established by pairwise agonistic interactions and the outcome tends to be based upon the relative competitive ability of the contestants. Competitive ability is determined by an interaction between genetic factors, social environment and previous experience. The behavioural inhibition displayed by subordinate animals could serve as a strategy to avoid aggression from the more dominant, whereby they sneakily obtain resources while waiting for an opportunity to climb up the hierarchy. Thus, when given the opportunity to increase in rank, individuals of low rank will have to shift from a subordinate to dominant behavioural state. This behavioural switch includes modifications in endocrine signalling, transcriptome profiles and changes in the activity of various neurotransmitter systems. In this Review, we will focus on teleost fish since there is a wealth of empirical data we can draw upon, and on the interaction of genetic and environmental effects on competitive ability. Teleost fishes are becoming increasingly more important as vertebrate models in a wide range of scientific research and thus it is important to understand how their social structure could potentially affect physiological studies and introduce high variation within data sets.

Fishes provide highly tractable models for the study of dominance behaviour since they often form easily measured linear dominance hierarchies (Holekamp and Strauss, 2016; Wallace et al., 2022). Typically, the dominant or top-ranked individual displays more aggressive behaviour, is relatively larger or has some other intrinsic property greater/better than that of others within the hierarchy. This provides the dominant individual with exclusive or priority access to resources, which may include mating opportunities (Rodriguez-Santiago et al., 2020). Size can often be used as a predictor of dominance in fishes (e.g. Reddon et al., 2011). Lower-ranked (or middle ranked) individuals are termed subdominants, and they may challenge the dominant for the top position. Beneath subdominants, subordinate individuals occupy the very lowest rank positions. Sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) held in groups of four individuals form a linear hierarchy with aggression being highest in the dominant individual, who obtains most food (Sneddon et al., 2006; Fig. 1). The subdominants in rank positions 2 and 3 obtain less food; however, the subordinate in rank 4 is much less aggressive and adopts a sneaky foraging strategy, which allows it to obtain food when the other fish are engaging in agonistic interactions. These rank positions also influence growth rates: the rank 1 dominant sticklebacks grow more than the subdominants, who display negative growth; this may be due to them engaging in unsuccessful fights with the dominant fish, leading them to expend energy to fuel the costs of aggression (Sneddon et al., 2006). In contrast, the sneaky foraging strategy of the subordinate fish pays off and these individuals exhibit positive growth rates. Thus, rank position can have profound effects on biological function and physiology.

Fig. 1.

Resource acquisition, dominance behaviour and pay-off in terms of growth in stickleback dominance hierarchies. (A) Dominant fish eat the most food. Mean number (±s.d.) of food items eaten per minute by each of the four social ranks [1 (dominant) to 4 (subordinate)] within the dominance hierarchies of sticklebacks (H=97.06, d.f.=3, P<0.001). (B) Dominant fish are the most aggressive in the hierarchy. Mean number (±s.d.) of aggressive minus defensive acts per minute performed by each rank member within dominance hierarchies (H=404.34, d.f.=3, P<0.001). (C) Dominant and subordinate sticklebacks gained weight whereas the middle-ranked subdominants (ranks 2 and 3) lost weight. Mean mass (±s.d.) change of each rank member during the experimental period (H=15.21, d.f.=3, P=0.002); n=24 for each rank; data reproduced with permission from Sneddon et al. (2006).

Fig. 1.

Resource acquisition, dominance behaviour and pay-off in terms of growth in stickleback dominance hierarchies. (A) Dominant fish eat the most food. Mean number (±s.d.) of food items eaten per minute by each of the four social ranks [1 (dominant) to 4 (subordinate)] within the dominance hierarchies of sticklebacks (H=97.06, d.f.=3, P<0.001). (B) Dominant fish are the most aggressive in the hierarchy. Mean number (±s.d.) of aggressive minus defensive acts per minute performed by each rank member within dominance hierarchies (H=404.34, d.f.=3, P<0.001). (C) Dominant and subordinate sticklebacks gained weight whereas the middle-ranked subdominants (ranks 2 and 3) lost weight. Mean mass (±s.d.) change of each rank member during the experimental period (H=15.21, d.f.=3, P=0.002); n=24 for each rank; data reproduced with permission from Sneddon et al. (2006).

Generally, the dominant individual controls the behaviour of the group, and the subdominants and subordinates are constrained by the use of aggression by the dominant. In the cichlid fish, Astatotilapia burtoni, dominant males are aggressive and exert influence over normal movement of the group. In contrast, subordinate males are non-aggressive, socially peripheral and do not direct group movement (Rodriguez-Santiago et al., 2020). This behavioural suppression and lack of access to resources may result in chronic stress in lower-ranked individuals, thus eliciting tertiary or long-term responses to stress, including impaired immune function, growth and reproduction (Rodriguez-Santiago et al., 2020). These biobehavioural differences between animals of differing rank position may increase energetic demands and consequently affect both energy intake and metabolism (Milewski et al., 2022). Further changes are known to take place in a variety of physiological systems, such as the hypothalamic–pituitary–interrenal (HPI) and hypothalamic–pituitary–gonadal (HPG) axes, and include altered gene expression and responsiveness of neural circuits that regulate dominance-related behaviours (Maruska and Fernald, 2011; 2012; Øverli et al., 2004; Almeida et al., 2019). In contrast, low-ranking animals inhibit aggression, and this may also include reproductive behaviours. Indeed, subordinates exhibit physiological changes to allow them to adapt to the stress of their low rank within the hierarchy (Backström and Winberg, 2017; Höglund et al., 2019). Chronic social stress, where low-ranked members of a hierarchy experience a long-term stress response, can lead to the occurrence of health issues since this impairs immune function (Dara et al., 2022). Yet, studies have demonstrated that rapid changes in dominance status occur as fish socially increase or decrease their rank and these changes are associated with dynamic changes in the brain (Sneddon et al., 2011; Simões et al., 2015). Gene expression profiles obtained from rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) brains with different rank positions demonstrate that there is a specific gene expression signature for dominants versus subdominants and subordinates (Sneddon et al., 2011). Upon removal of the dominant fish, subdominants display a rapid shift in brain gene expression to that of a dominant profile within 48 h (Sneddon et al., 2011). Therefore, caution should be applied to the measurement of responses from groups of fish, since it appears that social rank influences global gene expression and physiology, through to behaviour, and thus knowledge of each individual's rank position within a group may provide new insights into intraspecific variation. Transcript profiles were obtained from whole brains, and the genes expressed have functions in protein turnover, metabolism, cell structure, transport and stress; these were upregulated in low-ranked trout compared with dominant fish. This differential expression profile may reflect the compromising nature of low social status in addition to the elevated stress response in lower-ranked fish compared with dominant individuals (Sneddon et al., 2011). Behaviourally relevant candidates were identified and included gamma aminobutyric acid-receptor associated protein (GABA-RAP) with roles in stress and aggression. Here GABA-RAP was upregulated in subordinates compared with dominant fish and this correlated with elevated cortisol in subordinates and thus this may be a consequence of increased stress in subdominants (Sneddon et al., 2011). Knowledge of the life history of the specific species being investigated is crucial to meaningfully understand the impact of dominance on physiology since this is likely to vary across species. Dominant A. burtoni, for example, form breeding territories and also divert energy to gonadal growth so dominant males lose weight compared with subordinates, possibly because of the costs of territory defence and reproductive investment (Hofmann et al., 1999).

Winning or losing fights for social dominance has a dramatic effect on future chances of winning fights (Hsu and Wolf, 1999; Oliveira et al., 2011). The winner increases its chance to win future contests, whereas the loser is more likely to continue losing; a phenomenon which has been referred to as the winner/loser effect. Even though body size is usually a strong predictor of the outcome of fights for social dominance (Reddon et al., 2011), Abbott et al. (1985) showed that the effect of previous social experience may outweigh the effect of size in dyadic fights in steelhead trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). In their experiment, the outcome of initial fights was clearly related to body size, with larger fish having greater fighting ability than smaller conspecifics. However, surplus feeding of subordinates did not reverse the dominance relationship, even though subordinates ended up being 14–50% larger than their dominant contestants (Abbott et al., 1985). Apparently, fighting experience alters a contestant's estimated and/or actual fighting ability (Hsu and Wolf, 1999). Long-term social interaction, as in the experiment by Abbott et al. (1985), may cause injuries in subordinates such as fin damage or loss of weight and physical strength, and this may reduce their actual fighting ability. However, losing fights appears to inhibit aggression and motivation or willingness to initiate agonistic interactions, even with opponents that are naïve and considerably smaller in body size and thus have less resource-holding power (RHP). Using a resident/intruder test, where an isolated animal is challenged by the introduction of a conspecific intruder, Höglund et al. (2001) quantified aggressive behaviour, in isolated juvenile Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus) prior to and following 2 days of dyadic interaction with a size-matched opponent. Following dyadic interaction, losers showed a significant decline in the number of aggressive acts performed towards the small intruder, whereas the opposite was observed in winners from the dyadic interaction. Moreover, losers showed a profound increase in the latency to attack the intruder (Höglund et al., 2001).

The inhibition of aggressive behaviour observed in subordinates is likely to be mediated by factors involved in the neuroendocrine stress response (Summers and Winberg, 2006). In juvenile Arctic charr kept in groups of four, the most subordinate fish, social rank 4, displayed a longer-lasting behavioural inhibition compared with the subdominant fish (Winberg et al., 1992; Fig. 2). In a stable dominance hierarchy, the most subordinate animal is subjected to more intense stress than individuals of higher social rank. It has been shown repeatedly that there is an inverse correlation between social rank and plasma levels of cortisol (Winberg and Lepage, 1998; Bessa et al., 2021) and a similar negative relationship has also been reported between social rank and brain serotonergic activity (Winberg et al., 1991). Thus, subdominant animals are less inhibited in their behaviour and appear ready to increase their aggressive behaviour and gain dominance as soon as an opportunity arises. In fact, Øverli et al. (2004) found that in rainbow trout, fish of intermediate social rank increase their aggression towards lower-ranking fish in response to short-term defeat (Fig. 3). However, it is also obvious that there is a negative correlation between the number of aggressive acts received during this short-term defeat and aggression shown towards the subordinate partner (Fig. 3). Thus, being on the receiving end of intense aggression results in behavioural inhibition, whereas being in contact with a dominant, highly aggressive fish, but receiving only low levels of aggression stimulates aggressive behaviour in the smaller subdominant trout (Fig. 3). Interestingly, in the study by Winberg et al. (1992), the subdominant fish (social rank 2) regained aggressive behaviour faster than the subordinates (social rank 3 and 4), but they showed lower levels of aggressive behaviour than these subordinates throughout the experiment (Winberg et al., 1992; Fig. 2).

Fig. 2.

Inhibition of aggressive behaviour in subordinate fish. Groups of juvenile Arctic charr consisting of four fish with previous experience of being (A) dominant (social rank 1), (B,C) subdominant (social rank 2 or 3) or (D) subordinate (social rank 4). Bar graphs show mean (±s.e.m.) number of aggressive acts performed during week 1 (n=4) and weeks 2–6 (n=12). In addition, aggressive acts per minute during all 15 min observation sessions is also shown. *P<0.05 ; **P<0.01, Mann–Whitney U-test (two tailed) (adapted from Winberg et al., 1992).

Fig. 2.

Inhibition of aggressive behaviour in subordinate fish. Groups of juvenile Arctic charr consisting of four fish with previous experience of being (A) dominant (social rank 1), (B,C) subdominant (social rank 2 or 3) or (D) subordinate (social rank 4). Bar graphs show mean (±s.e.m.) number of aggressive acts performed during week 1 (n=4) and weeks 2–6 (n=12). In addition, aggressive acts per minute during all 15 min observation sessions is also shown. *P<0.05 ; **P<0.01, Mann–Whitney U-test (two tailed) (adapted from Winberg et al., 1992).

Fig. 3.

Displaced aggression in juvenile rainbow trout. (A) Change in aggressive behaviour in subdominant trout following repeated interaction with a subordinate fish, with or without a brief encounter with a dominant partner (mean+s.e.m., **P<0.01, two-tailed t-test). ‘No defeat’ means that the fish were held with a subordinate partner without encountering a dominant aggressive trout, then in isolation and finally with a subordinate partner. ‘Defeat+re-establishment’ means that the fish were held with a subordinate partner, then with a dominant partner and finally with a subordinate partner again. (B) Relationship between change in aggressive behaviour and number of aggressive acts received during the encounter with the dominant fish (Pearson R2 and P-values). The solid line is the least-squares fitted trendline whereas the dotted line indicates the 100% level, equalling no change in aggression. Change in aggression for the ‘no defeat’ group, i.e. fish receiving no aggressive acts, is shown on the y-axis (individual data points; adapted from Øverli et al., 2004).

Fig. 3.

Displaced aggression in juvenile rainbow trout. (A) Change in aggressive behaviour in subdominant trout following repeated interaction with a subordinate fish, with or without a brief encounter with a dominant partner (mean+s.e.m., **P<0.01, two-tailed t-test). ‘No defeat’ means that the fish were held with a subordinate partner without encountering a dominant aggressive trout, then in isolation and finally with a subordinate partner. ‘Defeat+re-establishment’ means that the fish were held with a subordinate partner, then with a dominant partner and finally with a subordinate partner again. (B) Relationship between change in aggressive behaviour and number of aggressive acts received during the encounter with the dominant fish (Pearson R2 and P-values). The solid line is the least-squares fitted trendline whereas the dotted line indicates the 100% level, equalling no change in aggression. Change in aggression for the ‘no defeat’ group, i.e. fish receiving no aggressive acts, is shown on the y-axis (individual data points; adapted from Øverli et al., 2004).

Winning or losing fights for social dominance sometimes also affects individual visual, acoustic and chemical cues, thereby signalling social experience to conspecifics. These social signals, which may work in tandem, are important in reducing agonistic interactions in established dominance hierarchies. The cichlid Metriaclima zebra uses visual signalling or displays during aggressive interactions and when these are combined with acoustic signals, increased modulation of aggressive behavioural decision making was observed: acoustic signals were used to signal decreased escalation of fights (Bertucci et al., 2010). In salmonids, social subordination results in a darkening of the sclera of the eye, followed by darkening of body coloration (O'Connor et al., 1999; Höglund et al., 2000, 2002). Höglund et al. (2000, 2002) showed that social subordination induces skin darkening in Arctic charr. This skin darkening is slow to develop and seems to represent a morphological colour change induced by chronic stress, especially stress-induced elevation in plasma levels of α-melanocyte-stimulating hormone. The darkening of the sclera that is observed in juvenile Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) in response to losing fights for social dominance occurs much faster and is most likely under neural control (O'Connor et al., 1999). Similarly, the eye-bar present in many cichlids appears to be under neuronal control since it can be turned on or off very rapidly (Price et al., 2008); however, in this case, the presence of a dark eye-bar is associated with dominance and high levels of aggression. Similarly, in zebrafish (Danio rerio), dominant fish tend to become darker with more obvious stripes, whereas subordinates turn pale (Price et al., 2008). Thus, fishes can quickly signal their social status, which may halt further aggressive interactions.

Chemical communication is common in fish, and chemical signals related to social rank have been demonstrated in cichlids (Keller-Costa et al., 2015). In the Mozambique tilapia (Oreochromis mossambicus), urinary odorants are actively released during agonistic interactions and appear to signal social status (Barata et al., 2007). A similar system has been described in the Tanganyika cichlid Astotilapia burtoni (Maruska and Fernald, 2012). Genomic analysis of the olfactory bulbs and olfactory cortex of the brain in Mozambique tilapia demonstrated specific changes in response to the urine of either dominant or subordinate males (Simões et al., 2015). Olfactory stimuli from individuals with differing dominance status had a major impact on the brain transcriptome, with different urinary social cues prompting specific patterns of transcript expression in the brain. These results illustrate that rapid changes in social status can be signalled to another conspecific, which responds at the level of gene expression in the brain and underpins behavioural plasticity in social status. An interesting point to note is that A. burtoni can undergo dynamic and fast transitions from dominant to subordinate and back again; their behaviour, colouration, transcriptome and stress responses also show a dramatic alteration when social status is altered (Maruska, 2015).

Huntingford (1976a,b) found that there is a positive relationship between boldness and aggressive behaviour in the three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Moreover, using zebrafish, Dahlbom et al. (2011) showed that bold males have an advantage in dyadic fights for social dominance. In their experiment, adult zebrafish males were screened for boldness prior to dyadic interaction using a series of behavioural assays. In all cases, boldness, as determined by these behavioural assays, predicted the outcome of fights for dominance. Thus, in addition to size and previous experience, factors such as personality traits appear to be important for the outcome of fights for dominance. Such a relationship between consistent behavioural traits and competitive ability in fights for social dominance is further supported by Schjolden et al. (2005), who showed that in juvenile rainbow trout, there is a relationship between how fast the fish resume feeding after transfer to isolation in a non-familiar environment and aggression. Fish acclimating faster start feeding sooner and display consistently higher attack frequencies and shorter attack latencies when tested in resident/intruder tests. Moreover, rapidly acclimating fish also have lower post-stress cortisol levels and become socially dominant in dyadic fights more often than fish displaying high post-stress cortisol (Schjolden et al., 2005). Fast acclimation to a novel environment, being aggressive and competitive in fights for social dominance along with low post-stress plasma cortisol is typical of a ‘proactive’ stress-coping style. Animals displaying a reactive stress-coping style show the opposite profile: high post-stress cortisol levels, neophobia with slow acclimation to novel environments and low aggression (Koolhaas et al., 2007). These divergent stress-coping styles were originally described in rodents (Koolhaas et al., 2007), but recent studies have shown that teleost fish show similar coping methods (Castanheira et al., 2017; Øverli et al., 2007; Winberg et al., 2007).

In fish, divergent stress-coping styles have been extensively studied in rainbow trout strains selectively bred for high (HR trout) and low (LR trout) post-stress plasma cortisol levels (Pottinger and Carrick, 1999). The LR and HR trout differed in behaviour in a way that could be interpreted as LR trout displaying a proactive coping style, with HR trout being reactive. In line with this, Pottinger and Carrick (2001) showed that LR trout have an advantage over HR trout in staged fights for social dominance in size-matched pairs.

The results from studies on the HR and LR trout clearly show that stress-coping styles are at least in part a heritable characteristic. However, environmental factors are likely to affect the expression of divergent stress-coping styles. For instance, transporting the selected HR and LR trout strains from Windermere to Oslo strongly affected their behavioural profile (Ruiz-Gomez et al., 2008). When tested prior to transport, LR fish showed a more rapid acclimation to a novel environment with low post-stress plasma cortisol and were more likely to become dominant in dyadic fights with size-matched HR trout, whereas HR trout took longer to acclimate, had higher post-stress cortisol and were more likely to end up subordinate in fights with LR trout. Following transport, the behaviour was reversed: HR trout acclimated faster than LR trout and also became dominant in dyadic fights more often than LR trout. However, HPI axis reactivity appeared to be stable since even after transport, LR trout showed lower post-stress cortisol than HR trout (Ruiz-Gomez et al., 2008).

Early studies suggested that boldness/personality and aggressiveness are fixed traits, meaning that individuals would have a permanent phenotype (review in Dingemanse and Réale, 2005). Genetic studies of boldness have shown it to be moderately heritable, although heritability can decline over the lifetime of offspring as a result of exposure to abiotic and biotic factors (e.g. Chervet et al., 2011; Ferrari et al., 2016). However, studies in rainbow trout investigating plasticity in boldness have provided insights into the dynamic nature of bold and shy traits. Shy trout given the experience of winning dyadic contests increase their boldness in subsequent risk-taking tests; thus, success has an emboldening effect (Frost et al., 2007). In contrast, losing fights results in bold trout becoming much shyer. Therefore, the social environment can also influence boldness. Trout which were classified as bold became shyer when placed in groups containing either 100% bold or 100% shy individuals, whereas shy trout remained shy (Thomson et al., 2016). When subject to a stressor, plasma cortisol concentrations reflect the original personality of these trout. Therefore, initially shy individuals had higher plasma cortisol than initially bold trout. These results are indicative of behavioural and physiological parameters of coping styles becoming uncoupled, whereby behavioural changes in boldness are not correlated with stress responses (see Sadoul et al., 2021). In a further study, the impact of predation threat was investigated when rainbow trout were under no, low or high predation risk conditions (Thomson et al., 2012). Under no risk, bold and shy fish retained their original behavioural responses to a novel challenge during the experimental period. However, under low and high risk, bold individuals became shyer, demonstrating their personality was labile and able to respond to predation. Shy individuals remain shy despite the risk; of course, being shy and cautious in nature in the face of the risk of predation is adaptive. Although bold fish become shyer under low risk, they still had a much lower cortisol response compared with shy fish, demonstrating again a decoupling of stress reactivity and risk-taking behaviour (Thomson et al., 2012). The dynamic nature of risk taking and behavioural flexibility in bold rainbow trout contrasts with studies conducted on the same species in other geographical locations. Several of these studies have shown that bold phenotypes are inflexible and that it is the shy personality that exhibits flexibility in behaviour (Moreira et al., 2004; Ruiz-Gomez et al., 2011; Johansen et al., 2012; Höglund et al., 2017). These disparate findings between laboratories in different countries may be due to genetic differences or it is possible that the previous experience of the fish also differed substantially. To dissect this, it would be useful to use the same animals from one location in studies conducted in geographically distant laboratories.

Neuronal networks

A social decision-making network (SDMN) has been identified in the vertebrate brain and in teleosts. The SDMN includes brain areas such as ventral pallium (Vs, the homologue of the mammalian extended medial amygdala), ventral nuclei of ventral telencephalic area (Vv, homologue of mammalian lateral septum), pre-optic area (POA, homologue of mammalian POA/paraventricular nucleus), ventral tuberal nucleus (vTn, homologue of mammalian anterior hypothalamus), anterior tuberal nucleus (aTn, homologue of mammalian ventro medial hypothalamus) and griseum centrale (GC, homologue of mammalian periaqueductal grey). These brain areas are interconnected and to some degree overlap with the mesolimbic reward system, for example, the Vv and Vs in teleosts (O'Connel and Hoffman, 2011a, b). The SDMN expresses sex steroid hormone receptors and controls multiple social behaviours, such as aggression, reproduction and parental care. However, it appears that an animal's behavioural profile is determined by the overall activity of this network and not by specific individual brain areas (Teles et al., 2016a, b). The activity of the network is affected by developmental factors, hormone levels, reproductive status, but also by the social information received, i.e. by social experience (O'Connel and Hoffman, 2011a, b). For instance, in zebrafish, winning or losing fights for social dominance is reflected in divergent expression patterns of immediate early genes in the SDMN, which in turn are paralleled by changes in plasma sex steroids and brain levels of arginine vasotocin (AVT) and isotocin, the teleost homologues of arginine vasopressin and oxytocin, respectively (Teles et al., 2016b; Almeida, et al., 2019).

Brain monoaminergic systems

The brain monoamines, dopamine (DA), noradrenaline (norepinephrine) and serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT), appear to have modulatory effects on the SDMN (Soares et al., 2018). Agonistic interactions result in a rapid activation of these neurotransmitter systems (Backström and Winberg, 2017). A brain area that interacts with the brainstem monoaminergic systems and highly important in controlling agonistic behaviour is the habenula (Hb). This diencephalic brain area, which is conserved across vertebrates, seems to be a satellite of the SDMN (Ogawa et al., 2021). In zebrafish, the medial subregion of the dorsal habenula (dHbM) projects to the median raphe (MR) via the ventral interpendicular nuclei (IPN), whereas the lateral subregion of the dorsal habenula (dHbL) projects to the GC via the dorsal IPN (Okamoto et al., 2020; Nakajo et al., 2020). These two pathways of the habenula act like a switch. Socially dominant zebrafish display a more intense activation of the dHbL–GC projection, whereas fish losing fights for social dominance show elevated activity of the dHbM–MR pathway (Okamoto et al., 2020). MR is one of the major serotonergic brain areas and 5-HT has been found to play an important role in coordinating behavioural, autonomic and neuroendocrine stress responses, as well as mediating the behavioural inhibition characterizing socially subordinate animals (Backström and Winberg, 2017). The GC, on the other hand, is rich in dopaminergic neurones. To some degree, dopamine DA and 5-HT have opposite effects on agonistic behaviour.

When establishing a dominance hierarchy, both winners (i.e. animals becoming dominant) and losers (i.e. animals becoming subordinate) show a rapid activation of the brain 5-HT system (Øverli et al., 1999). In this initial phase of agonistic interaction, both winners and losers are actively performing agonistic behaviour, showing no signs of behavioural inhibition. However, brain serotonergic activity gradually returns to control levels in animals becoming dominant, whereas it remains elevated in subordinate animals even after long-term social interaction (Winberg and Nilsson, 1993; Øverli et al., 1999). Subordinates also show elevated plasma cortisol levels (Winberg and Lepage, 1998; Øverli et al., 1999) and a pronounced behavioural inhibition (Winberg et al., 1992). The chronic elevation of brain serotonergic activity is likely to be part of the mechanism mediating this behavioural inhibition (Winberg and Thörnqvist, 2016; Backström and Winberg, 2017). Interestingly, only long-term activation of the serotonergic system results in behavioural inhibition. This is also obvious when brain serotonergic activity is stimulated by elevated dietary intake of L-tryptophan (TRP), the amino acid precursor of 5-HT (Winberg et al., 2001; Lepage et al., 2003; Höglund et al., 2019). In rainbow trout, fish fed elevated TRP show an inhibition of aggressive behaviour but only after being fed this TRP-supplemented feed for 7 days, even though elevated dietary TRP has rapid effects on brain serotonergic activity. Thus, chronic elevation of brain serotonergic activity most likely results in changes in brain 5-HT functions related to altered expression of 5-HT receptors, re-uptake transporters and the balance between pre-synaptic auto-receptors and post-synaptic receptors. Such plastic changes may result in elevated 5-HT tone in specific brain regions (Winberg and Thörnqvist, 2016; Backström and Winberg, 2017; Höglund et al., 2019). Moreover, behavioural effects of 5-HT may also be mediated in concert with other neuromodulators, e.g. neuropeptides.

Neuropeptides

AVT and corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) are involved in the control of the HPI axis and have been suggested to modulate agonistic behaviour in teleosts (reviewed by Backström and Winberg, 2017). Larson et al. (2006) observed that dominant zebrafish have higher numbers of AVT immunoreactive (AVTir) cells in the magnocellular POA (mPOA) than subordinates, and that magnocellular AVTir cells were also larger in dominant than in subordinate zebrafish. However, in the parvocellular POA (pPOA), the reverse relationship was observed, with subordinate zebrafish having larger and higher numbers of AVTir cells than dominant fish (Larson et al., 2006). By contrast, in Mozambique tilapia, subordinate (non-territorial) males showed larger AVTir cells in the mPOA than territorial males, and even though the number of pPOA or mPOA AVTir cells did not differ between non-territorial and territorial males, non-territorials displayed a higher number of gigantocellular AVTir cells (Almeida and Oliveira, 2015). The significance of this observation is unclear, even though Almeida and Oliveira (2015) suggested that it could be related to an association between the HPI axis and the gigantocellular AVT nucleus. Interestingly, in Astotilapia burtoni, another closely related African cichlid species with a mating system similar to that of the Mozambique tilapia, territorial males showed higher expression of AVT mRNA in the gigantocellular nucleus than non-territorial males, and AVT mRNA levels correlated with aggressive behaviour (Greenwood et al., 2008). Thus, even though AVT appears related to agonistic behaviour, the role of this peptide in controlling behaviour is likely to vary between species and contexts (Teles et al., 2016a,b). For example, dominant Neolamprologus pulcher have higher brain AVT gene expression than do subordinates (Aubin-Horth et al., 2007), but subordinates have higher levels of free AVT peptide, the biologically active form (Reddon et al., 2015). An interaction between 5-HT and AVT has been suggested, and Semsar et al. (2004) showed that fluoxetine, which reduces aggression in male bluehead wrasse (Thalassoma bifasciatum) (Perreault et al., 2003), also reduces the expression of AVT mRNA in this species. However, in rainbow trout, intracerebroventricular (ICV) injections of AVT had no effect on brainstem 5-HT (Backström and Winberg, 2009), even though Gesto et al. (2014) reported that ICV injections of AVT increased serotonergic activity in the telencephalon and hypothalamus of rainbow trout 8 h following injection. In the study by Backström and Winberg (2009), the fish were sampled following 60 min of dyadic agonistic interactions and since aggressive encounters result in an immediate activation of the brain 5-HT system, this socially induced activation of the 5-HT system may have concealed potential effects of AVT.

Interactions between 5-HT and CRF have been studied in more detail and it is clear that 5-HT interacts with CRF in the control of several behaviours (Backström and Winberg, 2017). Both stimulating and inhibitory effects of CRF on aggressive behaviour have been reported in mammals (see reviews by Backström and Winberg, 2013, 2017; Hostetler and Ryabinin, 2013). In rainbow trout, ICV injections of CRF have been shown to either increase or decrease dominance. For instance, Carpenter et al. (2009) found that ICV injections of CRF suppressed attack behaviour but increased the ratio of attacks to retreats, which led to dominance (Carpenter et al., 2009). Backström et al. (2011), on the other hand, reported that rainbow trout receiving ICV injections of CRF lost dyadic fights. This discrepancy could be due to differences in interaction times and experimental design. Carpenter et al. (2009) used higher doses of CRF and allowed the fish to interact for 15 min, whereas in the study by Backström et al. (2011) they interacted in pairs for 60 min. Both studies quantified effects on the brain monoaminergic systems. As in the studies of AVT discussed above, the fish were interacting in pairs, which most likely made a stimulatory effect of CRF on 5-HT activity difficult to detect. In agreement with this suggestion, neither Carpenter et al. (2009) nor Backström et al. (2011) detected any effects of CRF on brain 5-HT activity. However, ICV injections of the CRF antagonist α-helical-CRH1-41 decreased 5-HIAA and 5-HT in the brainstem (Backström et al., 2011).

Spexin (SPX), a novel neuropeptide also known as neuropeptide Q, which is highly conserved across the vertebrate subphylum, is another neuromodulator that appears to interact with the 5-HT system in the control of stress and agonistic behaviour (Lim et al., 2019). In teleost fishes, SPX occurs as two orthologues, SPX1 and SPX2, with divergent brain expression patterns. In zebrafish, SPX1 is expressed in ventral pallium, dHB and hindbrain, whereas SPX2 is expressed in the POA. SPX acts on the galanin receptors and, in zebrafish, SPX1 and SPX2 activate galanin receptor 2a and 2b. Chronic social stress results in an upregulation in the expression of SPX1a and SPX1b in the brain of Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus) (Lim et al., 2020). As discussed above, the dHb appears to be important in the control of agonistic behaviour via its projections to MR via the IPN (Okamoto et al., 2020). Transgenic zebrafish overexpressing SPX1 in the dHB show anxiolytic behaviour compared with wild-type siblings (Jeong et al., 2019). Moreover, overexpression of SPX1 also results in an upregulation of the expression of 5HT-related genes in the MR (Jeong et al., 2019).

Aggressive behaviour can differ over a fish's lifetime. For example, marine yellowtail, Seriola quinqueradiata (Carangidae), do not engage in agonistic interactions during the larval period [until 10 mm in total length (TL)] ∼20 days after hatching (Sakakura and Tsukamoto, 1998). Aggressive behaviour is first observed just after metamorphosis to the juvenile period, which is accompanied by an elevation of tissue cortisol levels. The onset of schooling behaviour is observed at 12 mm TL. Within the juvenile schools, three categories of social rank were characterised, including dominants (10–20%), subdominants (10–20%) and subordinates (60–80%) (Sakakura and Tsukamoto, 1998). Similarly, in Japanese flounder (Paralichthys olivaceus), aggression was not recorded during the larval stage [13.2 mm in standard length (SL)] (Sakakura and Tsukamoto, 2002). Aggressive behaviour was first observed on day 39 after hatching, coinciding with completion of metamorphosis from the larval to the juvenile stage, and increased until day 46. Therefore, there are clear age-related changes in aggression that need to be explored.

Early rearing conditions can also influence adult aggressive behaviour in many animals (Holekamp and Strauss, 2016). Adverse conditions during development can mean individuals become more aggressive, and this persists into adult life. Studies in mammals show that early life adversity, including repeated maternal separation and neglect, conflict between parents, post-weaning social isolation and neglect, can influence later life aggression. In Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens), individuals were subjected to social isolation as juveniles and sub-adults, with fish reared in social groups becoming much less aggressive (Iwata et al., 2021). Threatening behaviour was greater in adult fish isolated as sub-adults, whereas fighting behaviour was significantly higher in adults isolated as juveniles. The influence of rearing conditions on behaviour was greater in females than in males, with female aggression more affected by a lack of enrichment (i.e. held in barren tanks) in the environment. Adults isolated as sub-adults had higher cortisol levels after aggression tests and, additionally, males had higher plasma concentrations of the androgen 11-ketotestosterone than those males reared in a group or isolated at the juvenile stage (Iwata et al., 2021). Social isolation in males also elicits intense fighting and elevated plasma cortisol concentrations after contests in the territorial convict cichlid Archocentrus nigrofasciatus (Earley et al., 2006). Thus, rearing environment can influence aggression, and affects the HPI and HPG axes underlying stress and reproduction. Many other factors can influence aggression in later life. Increased population density and the availability of shelter increase aggression in gopher rockfish, Sebastes carnatus (Hoelzer, 1987). Predation risk influences the aggressiveness of sticklebacks, with those reared in a risky environment being less aggressive (Herczeg et al., 2016).

Maternal behaviour and maternal rank can also significantly influence offspring aggressiveness in fish (D'Amore et al., 2015). Sons of well-fed swordtail fish, Xiphophorus multilineatus, form a positive correlation between aggression and risk taking or boldness, but levels of aggression and boldness are decoupled in offspring from poorly fed females (D'Amore et al., 2015). When offspring from Atlantic salmon mothers treated with cortisol implants were compared with untreated offspring for dominance behaviour, there were differences in both aggressiveness and stress (Eriksen et al., 2011). Offspring from mothers treated with cortisol gained a high dominance rank and were more aggressive after dominance–subordinate relationships were established when compared with offspring from mothers not treated with cortisol (Eriksen et al., 2011). Female guppies, Poecilia reticulata, exposed to mild stress gave birth to offspring that are more aggressive than those from unstressed mothers (Eaton et al., 2015). Thus, the state of the mother has a profound influence on offspring behaviour and physiology, with stressed mothers producing more aggressive and possibly competitive offspring, which would be adaptive in a harsh or challenging environment.

Aggressive and agonistic interactions and the formation of dominance hierarchies occur naturally in wild populations. However, individuals can choose whether to engage in these potentially stressful and energetically costly behaviours and decide when to retreat from an opponent. This is not entirely possible in captive situations where fish are held in a relatively small volume of water and cannot necessarily retreat or escape from an aggressor. Thus, dominance behaviour and aggressive interactions can present a significant problem for low-ranking individuals owing to their low social status, potentially resulting in chronic stress. Many examples of dominance hierarchies described above are from species held in aquaculture (e.g. salmonids), used in the ornamental fish trade and held in home tanks, ponds or public aquaria, or used as laboratory models. Within these contexts, confined environments do not allow escape and researchers have speculated that the high aggression seen in captivity is an artifact (Sloman and Armstrong, 2002). Consequently, although aggression and dominance hierarchies are seen in nature, these may not result in the profound stress responses and elevated cortisol or in the neurobiological differences measured in laboratory-based studies (Sloman and Armstrong, 2002). However, in the natural environment, escape may be difficult in relatively small environments such as a pond or where predation risk is high, making it impossible for individuals to leave the group. Thus, housing aggressive species in close confinement or at high stock densities where lower-ranking individuals cannot seek shelter for respite or escape from the attentions of dominants is likely to be a significant challenge to animal welfare.

Aggression is a significant problem in the aquaculture of fishes. For example, juvenile salmonids aggressively defend feeding territories in nature, which becomes problematic in the close confines of the aquaculture environment, particularly during periods of feed restriction (Brännäs and Alanärä, 1994). Farmed salmonids and other species have observable dominance hierarchies (e.g. Atlantic salmon, rainbow trout), in which subordinates experience chronic stress measured in HPI and serotonergic activity, but HPG activity is reduced (Cubitt et al., 2008). When food is provided in a predictable manner, these relationships can lead to aggressive behaviour, injury and differential feed acquisition, with dominant animals monopolising food, resulting in growth disparity between fish (e.g. Cubitt et al., 2008, Montero et al., 2009). High levels of aggression result in more severe fin damage (e.g. Cañon Jones et al., 2010), with the dorsal and caudal fins mainly affected (Turnbull et al., 2008; Persson and Alanärä, 2014). To overcome differential growth, size grading is often used in fish farms to remove larger individuals; however, this results in subdominants taking the place of dominants so that size grading must be repeated. Handling and size grading have been shown to be a stressor in numerous species, for example, in the red sea bream Pagrus major (Biswas et al., 2006), Atlantic salmon (McCormick et al., 1998) and common carp (Saeji et al., 2003). An additional issue to consider is the variable reactions of individuals with differing stress-coping styles such that proactive, bold individuals with a low cortisol response could potentially tolerate or cope with stress in the aquaculture environment much better than reactive, shy fish.

Increased competition over food, increased boldness or even an individual being unable to withdraw from a contest through lack of space may elevate levels of aggression (Huntingford, 2004). Solutions to this problem may be challenging, particularly in fish farms focusing on high productivity. Reducing density and increasing space may limit aggression, although proposed densities to minimise aggressive behaviour varies between studies (Gronquist and Berges, 2013), suggesting that other issues may also be important. Provision of shelter or refuge for fish may also minimise the impact of aggressive encounters, as well as ensuring that space within tanks or sea cages is fully utilised (Huntingford and Kadri, 2014). For example, sea bream have impaired learning ability, brain development and spatial orientation in barren tanks, which are all improved by the provision of environmental enrichment (increasing the heterogeneity or complexity of the living space; Arechavala-Lopez et al., 2019, 2020). Provision of ropes also reduces aggression in sea bream held in sea cages (Arechavala-Lopez et al., 2019, 2020). Future studies should focus on the use of enrichment tools to allow lower-ranking individuals to escape or hide from more aggressive, dominant individuals. This has the potential to reduce stress and improve the welfare of fish with low social status.

Ornamental fish are kept or reared for their attractiveness as companion animals or as exhibits in public aquaria for educational or conservation purposes. It is estimated that 1 in 10 households possess pet fish in the UK (https://www.pfma.org.uk/statistics) and in the USA (Davenport, 1996), with an estimated 100 million housed in tanks and outdoor ponds in the UK alone (https://ornamentalfish.org/aquatic-shops-face-animal-welfare-nightmare-as-energy-prices-rocket/). Approximately 2000 freshwater and marine fish species are kept as companion animals or held in public exhibits such as aquaria or zoos (https://www.liveaquaria.com/article/159/?aid=159). The global trade in ornamental fish is estimated to amount to approximately $5.4 billion (https://www.grandviewresearch.com/industry-analysis/ornamental-fish-market) and is therefore a lucrative industry. Fishes can be readily purchased from numerous retailers and so the likelihood of purchasing aggressive or territorial species to keep in a home tank or pond is high. Aggressive or territorial fishes can harass other fish within a tank community, and this could represent chronic stress and may result in ill health or mortality. Sneddon and Wolfenden (2019) make a variety of suggestions with regards to the housing of aggressive and territorial species. The recommendations include keeping territorial species individually to avoid territories overlapping thereby reducing aggressive behaviour and considering a variety of species that can live together as a ‘community’: for example, the presence of compatible species such as freshwater angelfish, neon tetras, white cloud mountain minnows and tiger barbs leads to no or low aggression between and within these species (Sloman et al., 2011). Social isolation can be stressful for some species, so housing territorial fish completely alone might not be good for welfare and including non-aggressive smaller fishes may be beneficial and provide non-threatening companionship. Knowledge of the social or aggressive behaviour of potential ‘pet’ fish is essential to ensure good welfare.

In the laboratory environment, similar problems to those outlined above exist for aquaculture and ornamental fish-keeping (Sloman et al., 2019). Limited space, inappropriate stocking densities, mixing of unfamiliar individuals and lack of environmental enrichment may all lead to problems with aggression. Again, environmental enrichment has been shown to reduce aggression in some cases. For example, high-level enrichment (8 structures within a tank) in both rockfish (Sebastes schlegelii) and greenling (Hexagrammos otakii) reduced aggression and stabilized the social hierarchy after 3 days (Zhang et al., 2021). Batzina and Karakatsouli (2012) found that gilthead seabream held in tanks with blue or red/brown glass gravel compared with a control (no gravel) or green gravel grew better and were less aggressive. In zebrafish, contradictory results have been obtained on whether enrichment reduces aggression. Some studies have shown a clear reduction in aggressive behaviour, whereas others have shown no change or an increase (reviewed by Stevens et al., 2021). Provision of a refuge during individual testing reduced aggression in zebrafish (Weber and Ghorai, 2013). Moreover, anchored and floating artificial plants and gravel reduced aggression in group-held zebrafish (Carfagnini et al., 2009), as did 12 strips of plastic arranged 3×4 to simulate vegetation (Basquill and Grant, 1998). Zebrafish form highly aggressive dominance relationships in pairs and provision of a floating plastic plant reduced aggressiveness and the occurrence of wounds (Keck et al., 2015). Caution should be applied to using enrichment in that no one size will fit all and enriching the environment may encourage territoriality and aggression in some species of fish. Each species may have different preferences and this may be further affected by age and sex.

Social rank greatly affects behaviour, physiology and performance of fish. High-ranking individuals are active, aggressive and more likely to win future fights for social dominance. Heritable factors, in part, control the stress-coping style of individual fish and thereby also affect competitive ability and the chance to obtain dominance. However, coping styles appear more plastic than previously realised, and environmental factors, especially social experience, largely affect the coping style. Thus, intraspecific behavioural and physiological traits appear to be controlled by a complex interaction between genetic and environmental factors (Fig. 4). In nature, the development of dominance hierarchies may act to reduce overt aggression, but in fish kept in captivity, it may result in severe chronic stress, compromising fish welfare. The impact of dominance and stress-coping style needs further investigation to truly understand how the captive environment affects the welfare of individuals within groups and to inform better husbandry and housing of these animals.

Fig. 4.

Factors affecting dominance status in fish. Intrinsic factors such as development, individual characteristics and phenotype influence the dominance rank a fish can attain. Extrinsic factors also modulate social position. Curved and straight arrows show the interplay that the four main contributory factors may have in shaping dominance status of any individual fish. Fish drawing by Johanna Axling.

Fig. 4.

Factors affecting dominance status in fish. Intrinsic factors such as development, individual characteristics and phenotype influence the dominance rank a fish can attain. Extrinsic factors also modulate social position. Curved and straight arrows show the interplay that the four main contributory factors may have in shaping dominance status of any individual fish. Fish drawing by Johanna Axling.

We would like to thank three anonymous referees for valuable comments on the manuscript.

Abbott
,
J. C.
,
Dunbrack
,
R. L.
and
Orr
,
C. D.
(
1985
).
The interaction of size and experience in dominance relationships of juvenile steelhead trout (Salmo gairdneri)
.
Behaviour
92
,
241
-
253
.
Almeida
,
O.
and
Oliveira
,
R. F.
(
2015
).
Social status and arginine vasotocin neuronal phenotypes in a cichlid fish
.
Brain Behav. Evol
85
,
203
-
213
.
Almeida
,
O.
,
Felix
,
A. S.
,
Oliveira
,
G. A.
,
Lopes
,
J. S.
and
Oliveira
,
R. F.
(
2019
).
Fighting assessment triggers rapid changes in activity of the brain social decision-making network of cichlid fish
.
Front. Behav. Neurosci
13
,
742483
.
Arechavala-Lopez
,
P.
,
Diaz-Gil
,
C.
,
Saraiva
,
J. L.
,
Moranta
,
D.
,
Castanheira
,
M. F.
,
Nuñez-Velázquez
,
S.
,
Ledesma-Corvi
,
S.
,
Mora-Ruiz
,
M. R.
and
Grau
,
A.
and (
2019
).
Effects of structural environmental enrichment on welfare of juvenile seabream (Sparus aurata)
.
Aquaculture Reports
15
,
100224
.
Arechavala-Lopez
,
P.
,
Caballero-Froilán
,
J. C.
,
Jiménez-García
,
M.
,
Capó
,
X.
,
Tejada
,
S.
,
Saraiva
,
J. L.
,
Sureda
,
A.
and
Moranta
,
D.
(
2020
).
Enriched environments enhance cognition, exploratory behaviour and brain physiological functions of Sparus aurata
.
Sci. Rep.
10
,
11252
.
Aubin-Horth
,
N.
,
Desjardins
,
J.
,
Martei
,
Y.
,
Balshine
,
S.
and
Hofmann
,
H.
(
2007
).
Masculinized dominant females in a cooperatively breeding species
.
Mol. Ecol.
16
,
1349
-
1358
.
Backström
,
T.
and
Winberg
,
S.
(
2009
).
Arginine-vasotocin influence on aggressive behavior and dominance in rainbow trout
.
Physiol. Behav
96
,
470
-
475
.
Backström
,
T.
and
Winberg
,
S.
(
2013
).
Central corticotropin releasing factor and social stress
.
Front. Neurosci
.
7
,
117
.
Backström
,
T.
and
Winberg
,
S.
(
2017
).
Serotonin coordinates responses to social stress—what we can learn from fish
.
Front. Neurosci
25
,
595
.
Backström
,
T.
,
Pettersson
,
A.
,
Johansson
,
V.
and
Winberg
,
S.
(
2011
).
CRF and urotensin I effects on aggression and anxiety-like behavior in rainbow trout
.
J. Exp. Biol.
214
,
907
-
14
.
Barata
,
E. N.
,
Hubbard
,
P. C.
,
Almeida
,
O. G.
and
Miranda A Canario
,
A. V. M.
and
Canário
,
A. V. M.
(
2007
).
Male urine signals social rank in the Mozambique tilapia (Oreochromis mossambicus)
.
BMC Biol.
5
,
54
.
Basquill
,
S. P.
and
Grant
,
J. W.
(
1998
).
An increase in habitat complexity reduces aggression and monopolization of food by zebra fish (Danio rerio)
.
Can. J. Zool
76
,
770
-
772
.
Batzina
,
A.
and
Karakatsouli
,
N.
(
2012
).
The presence of substrate as a means of environmental enrichment in intensively reared gilthead seabream Sparus aurata: growth and behavioral effects
.
Aquaculture
370-371
,
54
-
60
.
Bertucci
,
F.
,
Beauchaud
,
M.
,
Attia
,
J.
and
Mathevon
,
N.
(
2010
).
Sounds modulate males’ aggressiveness in a cichlid fish
.
Ethology
116
,
1179
-
1188
.
Bessa
,
E.
,
Sadoul
,
B.
,
Mckenzie
,
D. J.
and
Geffroy
,
B.
(
2021
).
Group size, temperature and body size modulate the effects of social hierarchy on basal cortisol levels in fishes
.
Horm. Behav.
136
,
105077
.
Biswas
,
A. K.
,
Seoka
,
M.
,
Takii
,
K.
,
Maita
,
M.
and
Kumai
,
H.
(
2006
).
Stress response of red sea bream Pagrus major to acute handling and chronic photoperiod manipulation
.
Aquaculture
252
,
566
-
572
.
Brännäs
,
E.
and
Alanärä
,
A.
(
1994
).
Effect of reward level on individual variability in demand feeding activity and growth rate in Arctic charr and rainbow trout
.
J. Fish Biol.
45
,
423
-
434
.
Breed
,
M. D.
and
Moore
,
J
. (
2012
).
Social behavior, cooperation, and kinship
. In
Animal Behaviour
, pp.
367
-
397
.
London
:
Academic Press
.
Cañon Jones
,
H. A.
,
Hansen
,
L.
,
Noble
,
C.
,
Damsgård
,
B.
,
Broom
,
D. M.
and
Pearce
,
G. P.
(
2010
).
Social network analysis of behavioural interactions influencing fin damage development in Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) during feed-restriction
.
Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci
127
,
139
-
151
.
Carfagnini
,
A. G.
,
Rodd
,
F. H.
,
Jeffers
,
K. B.
and
Bruce
,
A. E. E.
(
2009
).
The effects of habitat complexity on aggression and fecundity in zebrafish (Danio rerio)
.
Environ. Boil. Fishes
86
,
403
-
409
.
Carpenter
,
R. E.
,
Korzan
,
W. J.
,
Bockholt
,
C.
,
Watt
,
M. J.
,
Forster
,
G. L.
,
Renner
,
K. J.
and
Summers
,
C. H.
(
2009
).
Corticotropin releasing factor influences aggression and monoamines: modulation of attacks and retreats
.
Neuroscience
158
,
412
-
425
.
Castanheira
,
M. F.
,
Conceição
,
L. E. C.
,
Millot
,
S.
,
Rey
,
S.
,
Bégout
,
M.-L.
,
Damsgård
,
B.
,
Kristiansen
,
T.
,
Höglund
,
E.
,
Øverli
,
Ø.
and
Martins
,
C. I. M.
(
2017
).
Coping styles in farmed fish: consequences for aquaculture
.
Rev. Aquac
9
,
23
-
41
.
Chervet
,
N.
,
Zöttl
,
M.
,
Schürch
,
R.
,
Taborsky
,
M.
and
Heg
,
D.
(
2011
).
Repeatability and heritability of behavioural types in a social cichlid
.
International Journal of Evolutionary Biology
2011
,
321729
.
Cubitt
,
K. F.
,
Winberg
,
S.
,
Huntingford
,
F. A.
,
Kadri
,
S.
,
Crampton
,
V. O.
and
Øverli
,
Ø.
(
2008
).
Social hierarchies, growth and brain serotonin metabolism in Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) kept under commercial rearing conditions
.
Physiol. Behav
94
,
529
-
535
.
Culbert
,
B. M.
,
Gilmour
,
K. M.
and
Balshine
,
S.
(
2019
).
Social buffering of stress in a group-living fish
.
Proc. R. Soc. B
286
,
20191626
.
Dahlbom
,
S. J.
,
Lagman
,
D.
,
Lundstedt-Enkel
,
K.
,
Sundström
,
L. F.
and
Winberg
,
S.
(
2011
).
Boldness predicts social status in zebrafish (Danio rerio)
.
PLOS ONE
6
,
e23565
.
Dara
,
M.
,
Dioguardi
,
M.
,
Vazzana
,
M.
,
Vazzana
,
I.
,
Accardi
,
D.
,
Carbonara
,
P.
,
Alfonso
,
S.
and
Cammarata
,
M.
(
2022
).
Effects of social hierarchy establishment on stress response and cell phagocytosis in gilt-head sea bream (Sparus aurata)
.
Fishes
7
,
75
.
D'Amore
,
D. M.
,
Rios-Cardenas
,
O.
and
Morris
,
M. R.
(
2015
).
Maternal investment influences development of behavioural syndrome in swordtail fish, Xiphophorus multilineatus
.
Anim. Behav.
103
,
147
-
151
.
Davenport
,
K. E.
(
1996
).
Characteristics of the current international trade in ornamental fish, with special reference to the European Union
.
Revue Scientifiqueet Technique de l'Office International des Epizooties
15
,
435
-
443
.
Dingemanse
,
N. J.
and
Réale
,
D.
(
2005
).
Natural selection and animal personality
.
Behaviour
142
,
1159
-
1184
.
Earley
,
R. L.
,
Edwards
,
J. T.
,
Aseem
,
O.
,
Felton
,
K.
,
Blumer
,
L. S.
,
Karom
,
M.
and
Grober
,
M. S.
(
2006
).
Social interactions tune aggression and stress responsiveness in a territorial cichlid fish (Archocentrus nigrofasciatus)
.
Physiol. Behav
88
,
353
-
363
.
Eaton
,
L.
,
Edmonds
,
E. J.
,
Henry
,
T. B.
,
Snellgrove
,
D. L.
and
Sloman
,
K. A.
(
2015
).
Mild maternal stress disrupts associative learning and increases aggression in offspring
.
Horm. Behav.
71
,
10
-
15
.
Eriksen
,
M. S.
,
Færevik
,
G.
,
Kittilsen
,
S.
,
Mccormick
,
M. I.
,
Damsgard
,
B.
,
Braithwaite
,
V. A.
,
Braastad
,
B. O.
and
Bakken
,
M.
(
2011
).
Stressed mothers – troubled offspring: a study of behavioural maternal effects in farmed Salmo salar
.
J. Fish Biol.
79
,
575
-
586
.
Ferrari
,
S.
,
Horri
,
K.
,
Allal
,
F.
,
Vergnet
,
A.
,
Benhaim
,
D.
,
Vandeputte
,
M.
,
Chatain
,
B.
and
Bégout
,
M.-L.
(
2016
).
Heritability of boldness and hypoxia avoidance in European seabass, Dicentrarchus labrax
.
PLoS ONE
11
,
e0168506
.
Frost
,
A. J.
,
Winrow-Giffen
,
A.
,
Ashley
,
P. J.
and
Sneddon
,
L. U.
(
2007
).
Plasticity in animal personality traits: does prior experience alter the degree of boldness?
Proceedings: Biological Sciences
274
,
333
-
339
.
Gesto
,
M.
,
Soengas
,
J. L.
,
Rodríguez-Illamola
,
A.
and
Míguez
,
J. M.
(
2014
).
Arginine vasotocin treatment induces a stress response and exerts a potent anorexigenic effect in rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss
.
J. Neuroendocrinol
26
,
89
-
99
.
Greenwood
,
A. K.
,
Wark
,
A. R.
,
Fernald
,
R. D.
and
Hofmann
,
H. A.
(
2008
).
Expression of arginine vasotocin in distinct preoptic regions is associated with dominant and subordinate behaviour in an African cichlid fish
.
Proc. Biol. Sci.
275
,
2393
-
2402
.
Gronquist
,
D.
and
Berges
,
J. A.
(
2013
).
Effects of aquarium-related stressors on the zebrafish: a comparison of behavioural, physiological, and biochemical indicators
.
J. Aquatic Health
25
,
53
-
65
.
Herczeg
,
G.
,
Ghani
,
N. I. A.
and
Merilä
,
J.
(
2016
).
On plasticity of aggression: influence of past and present predation risk, social environment and sex
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
70
,
179
-
187
.
Hoelzer
,
G.
(
1987
).
The effect of early experience on aggression in two territorial scorpaenid fishes
.
Environ. Biol. Fishes
19
,
183
-
194
.
Hofmann
,
H. A.
,
Benson
,
M. E.
and
Fernald
,
R. D.
(
1999
).
Social status regulates growth rate: consequences for life-history strategies
.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
96
,
14171
-
14176
.
Höglund
,
E
,
Balm
,
P. H. M.
and
Winberg
,
S.
(
2000
).
Skin darkening, a potential social signal in subordinate Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus): The regulatory role of brain monoamines and pro-opiomelanocortin-derived peptides
.
J. Exp. Biol.
203
,
1711
-
1721
.
Höglund
,
E.
,
Balm
,
P. H. M.
and
Winberg
,
S.
(
2002
).
Behavioural and neuroendocrine effects of environmental background colour and social interaction in Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus)
.
J. Exp. Biol.
205
,
2535
-
2543
.
Höglund
,
E.
,
Kolm
,
N.
and
Winberg
,
S.
(
2001
).
Stress-induced changes in brain serotonergic activity, plasma cortisol and aggressive behavior in Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus) is counteracted by L-DOPA
.
Physiol. Behav
74
,
381
-
389
.
Höglund
,
E.
,
Silva
,
P. I. M.
,
Vindas
,
M. A.
and
Øverli
,
O.
(
2017
).
Contrasting coping styles meet the wall: a dopamine driven dichotomy in behavior and cognition
.
Front. Neurosci.
11
,
383
.
Höglund
,
E.
,
Øverli
,
O.
and
Winberg
,
S.
(
2019
).
Tryptophan metabolic pathways and brain serotonergic activity: a comparative review
.
Front. Endocrinol.
10
,
158
.
Holekamp
,
K. E.
and
Strauss
,
E. D.
(
2016
).
Aggression and dominance: an interdisciplinary overview
.
Curr. Opin. Behav. Sciences
12
,
44
-
51
.
Hostetler
,
C. M.
and
Ryabinin
,
A. E.
(
2013
).
The CRF system and social behavior: a review
.
Front. Neurosci.
7
,
92
.
Hsu
,
Y.
and
Wolf
,
L. L.
(
1999
).
The winner and loser effect: Integrating multiple experiences
.
Anim. Behav.
57
,
903
-
910
.
Huntingford
,
F. A.
(
1976a
).
Relationship between Anti-predator behavior and aggression among conspecifics in 3-spined stickleback, Gasterosteus aculeatus
.
Anim. Behav.
24
,
245
-
260
.
Huntingford
,
F. A.
(
1976b
).
Comparison of reaction of sticklebacks in different reproductive conditions towards conspecifics and predators
.
Anim. Behav.
24
,
694
-
697
.
Huntingford
,
F. A.
(
2004
).
Implications of domestication and rearing conditions for the behaviour of cultivated fishes
.
J. Fish. Biol
65
,
122
-
142
.
Huntingford
,
F. A.
and
Kadri
,
S.
(
2014
).
Defining, assessing and promoting the welfare of farmed fish
.
Revue Scientifique et Technique de L'Office International des Epizooities
33
,
233
-
244
.
Iwata
,
E.
,
Masamoto
,
K.
,
Kuga
,
H.
and
Ogino
,
M.
(
2021
).
Timing of isolation from an enriched environment determines the level of aggressive behavior and sexual maturity in Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens)
.
BMC Zool.
6
,
1
-
11
.
Jeong
,
I.
,
Kim
,
E.
,
Seong
,
J. Y.
and
Park
,
H.-C.
(
2019
).
Overexpression of spexin 1 in the dorsal habenula reduces anxiety in Zebrafish
.
Front. Neural Circuits
13
,
53
.
Johansen
,
I. B.
,
Sorensen
,
C.
,
Sandvik
,
G. K.
,
Nilsson
,
G. E.
,
Höglund
,
E.
,
Bakken
,
M.
and
Øverli
,
Ø.
(
2012
).
Neural plasticity is affected by stress and heritable variation in stress coping style
.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol. D Genomics Proteomics
7
,
161
-
171
.
Keck
,
V.
,
Edgerton
,
D. S.
,
Hajizadeh
,
S.
,
Swift
,
L. L.
,
Dupont
,
W. D.
,
Lawrence
,
C.
and
Boyd
,
K. L.
(
2015
).
Effects of habitat complexity on pair-housed Zebrafish
.
J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci.
54
,
378
.
Keller-Costa
,
T.
,
Canário
,
A. V. M.
and
Hubbard
,
P. C.
(
2015
).
Chemical communication in cichlids: a mini-review
.
Gen. Comp. Endocrinol.
221
,
64
-
74
.
Koolhaas
,
J. M.
,
de Boer
,
S. F.
,
Buwalda
,
B.
and
van Reenen
,
K.
(
2007
).
Individual variation in coping with stress: a multidimensional approach of ultimate and proximate mechanisms
.
Brain Behav. Evol.
70
,
218
-
226
.
Larson
,
E. T.
,
O'malley
,
D. M.
and
Melloni
,
R. H.
Jr
(
2006
).
Aggression and vasotocin are associated with dominant–subordinate relationships in zebrafish
.
Behav. Brain Res.
167
,
94
-
102
.
Lepage
,
O.
,
Vilchez
,
I. M.
,
Pottinger
,
T. G.
and
Winberg
,
S.
(
2003
).
Time-course of the effect of dietary L-tryptophan on plasma cortisol levels in rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss)
.
J. Exp. Biol.
206
,
3589
-
3599
.
Lim
,
C. H.
,
Lee
,
Y. M. S.
,
Soga
,
T.
and
Parhar
,
I.
(
2019
).
Evolution of structural and functional diversity of spexin in mammalian and non-mammalian vertebrate species
.
Front. Endocrinol.
10
,
379
.
Lim
,
C. H.
,
Soga
,
T.
,
Levavi-Sivan
,
B.
and
Parhar
,
I. S.
(
2020
).
Chronic social defeat stress up-regulates spexin in the brain of nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus)
.
Sci. Rep.
10
,
7666
.
Maruska
,
K. P.
and
Fernald
,
R. D.
(
2011
).
Social regulation of gene expression in the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal axis
.
Physiology
26
,
412
-
423
.
Maruska
,
K. P.
and
Fernald
,
R. D.
(
2012
).
Contextual chemosensory urine signaling in an African cichlid fish
.
J. Exp. Biol.
215
,
68
-
74
.
Maruska
,
K. P.
(
2015
).
Social transitions cause rapid behavioral and neuroendocrine changes
.
Integr. Comp. Biol.
55
,
294
-
306
.
Mccormick
,
S. D.
,
Shrimpton
,
J. M.
,
Carey
,
J. B.
,
O'dea
,
M. F.
,
Sloan
,
K. E.
,
Moriyama
,
S.
and
Björnsson
,
B. T.
(
1998
).
Repeated acute stress reduces growth rate of Atlantic salmon parr and alters plasma levels of growth hormone, insulin-like growth factor I and cortisol
.
Aquaculture
168
,
221
-
235
.
Milewski
,
T. M.
,
Lee
,
W.
,
Champagne
,
F. A.
and
Curley
,
J. P.
(
2022
).
Behavioural and physiological plasticity in social hierarchies
.
Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci.
377
,
20200443
.
Montero
,
D.
,
Lalumera
,
G.
,
Izquierdo
,
M. S.
,
Caballero
,
M. J.
,
Saroglia
,
M.
and
Tort
,
L.
(
2009
).
Establishment of dominance relationships in gilthead sea bream Sparus aurata juveniles during feeding: effects on feeding behavior, feed utilization and fish health
.
J. Fish Biol
74
,
790
-
805
.
Moreira
,
P. S. A.
,
Pulman
,
K. G. T.
and
Pottinger
,
T. G.
(
2004
).
Extinction of a conditioned response in rainbow trout selected for high or low responsiveness to stress
.
Horm. Behav
46
,
450
-
457
.
Nakajo
,
H.
,
Chou
,
M.-Y.
,
Kinoshita
,
M.
,
Appelbaum
,
L.
,
Shimazaki
,
H.
,
Tsuboi
,
T.
and
Okamoto
,
H.
(
2020
).
Hunger potentiates the habenular winner pathway for social conflict by orexin-promoted biased alternative splicing of the AMPA receptor gene
.
Cell Reports
31
,
107790
.
O'Connor
,
K. I.
,
Metcacalfe
,
N. B.
and
Taylor
,
A. C.
(
1999
).
Does darkening signal submission in territorial contests between juvenile Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar
.
Anim. Behav.
58
,
1269
-
1276
.
O'Connell
,
L. A.
and
Hofmann
,
H. A.
(
2011a
).
Genes, hormones and circuits: an integrative approach to study the evolution of social behaviour
.
Front. Neuroendocrinol.
32
,
320
-
335
.
O'Connell
,
L. A.
and
Hofmann
,
H. A.
(
2011b
).
The vertebrate mesolimbic reward system and social behaviour network: a comparative synthesis
.
J. Comp. Neurol.
519
,
3599
-
3639
.
Ogawa
,
S.
,
Pfaff
,
D. W.
and
Parhar
,
I. S.
(
2021
).
Fish as a model in social neuroscience: conservation and diversity in the social brain network
.
Biol. Rev.
96
,
999
-
1020
.
Okamoto
,
H.
,
Cherng
,
B.-W.
,
Nakajo
,
H.
,
Chou
,
M.-Y.
and
Kinoshita
,
M.
(
2020
).
Habenula as the experience-dependent controlling switchboard of behavior and attention in social conflict and learning
.
Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.
68
,
36
-
43
.
Oliveira
,
R. F.
,
Silva
,
J. F.
and
Simões
,
J. M.
(
2011
).
Fighting Zebrafish: characterization of aggressive behavior and winner–loser effects in Zebrafish
.
Jun
2011
,
73
-
81
.
Øverli
,
Ø.
,
Harris
,
C. A.
and
Winberg
,
S.
(
1999
).
Short-term effects of fights for social dominance and the establishment of dominant-subordinate relationships on brain monoamines and cortisol in rainbow trout
.
Brain Behav. Evol.
54
,
263
-
275
.
Øverli
,
Ø.
,
Korzan
,
W. J.
,
Larson
,
E. T.
,
Winberg
,
S.
,
Lepage
,
O.
,
Pottinger
,
T. G.
,
Renner
,
K. J.
and
Summers
,
C. H.
(
2004
).
Behavioral and neuroendocrine correlates of displaced aggression in trout
.
Horm. Behav.
45
,
324
-
329
.
Øverli
,
Ø.
,
Sorensen
,
C.
,
Pulman
,
K. G. T.
,
Pottinger
,
T. G.
,
Korzan
,
W. J.
,
Summers
,
C. H.
and
Nilsson
,
G. E.
(
2007
).
Evolutionary background for stress-coping styles: Relationships between physiological, behavioral, and cognitive traits in non-mammalian vertebrates
.
Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.
31
,
396
-
412
.
Perreault
,
H. A. N.
,
Semsar
,
K.
and
Godwin
,
J.
(
2003
).
Fluoxetine treatment decreases territorial aggression in a coral reef fish
.
Physiol. Behav.
79
,
719
-
724
.
Persson
,
L.
and
Alanärä
,
A.
(
2014
).
The effect of shelter on welfare of juvenile Atlantic salmon Salmo salar reared under a feed restriction regimen
.
J. Fish. Biol.
85
,
645
-
656
.
Pottinger
,
T. G.
and
Carrick
,
T. R.
(
1999
).
Modification of the plasma cortisol response to stress in rainbow trout by selective breeding
.
Gen. Comp. Endocrinol.
116
,
122
-
132
.
Pottinger
,
T. G.
and
Carrick
,
T. R.
(
2001
).
Stress responsiveness affects dominant-subordinate relationships in rainbow trout
.
Horm. Behav
40
,
419
-
427
.
Price
,
A. C.
,
Weadick
,
C. J.
,
Shim
,
J.
and
Rodd
,
F. H.
(
2008
).
Pigments, patterns, and fish behavior
.
Zebrafish
5
,
297
-
307
.
Reddon
,
A. R.
,
Voisin
,
M. R.
,
Menon
,
N.
,
Marsh-Rollo
,
S. E.
,
Wong
,
M. Y. L.
and
Balshine
,
S.
(
2011
).
Rules of engagement for resource contests in a social fish
.
Anim. Behav.
82
,
93
-
99
.
Reddon
,
A. R.
,
O'connor
,
C. M.
,
Marsh-Rollo
,
S. E.
,
Balshine
,
S.
,
Gozdowska
,
M.
and
Kulczykowska
,
E.
(
2015
).
Brain nonapeptide levels are related to social status and affiliative behaviour in a cooperatively breeding cichlid fish
.
R. Soc. Open Sci.
2
,
1
-
12
.
Rodriguez-Santiago
,
M.
,
Nührenberg
,
P.
,
Derry
,
J.
,
Deussen
,
O.
,
Francisco
,
F. A.
,
Garrison
,
L. K.
,
Garza
,
S. F.
,
Hofmann
,
H. A.
and
Jordan
,
A.
(
2020
).
Behavioral traits that define social dominance are the same that reduce social influence in a consensus task
.
Proc. Nal. Acad. Sci. USA
117
,
18566
-
18573
.
Ruiz-Gomez
,
M. D.
,
Kittilsen
,
S.
,
Höglund
,
E.
,
Huntingford
,
F. A.
,
Sørensen
,
C.
,
Pottinger
,
T. G.
,
Bakken
,
M.
,
Winberg
,
S.
and
Øverli
,
O.
and
Overli
,
O.
(
2008
).
Behavioral plasticity in rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) with divergent coping styles: When doves become hawks
.
Horm. Behav
54
,
534
-
538
.
Ruiz-Gomez
,
M. D.
,
Huntingford
,
F. A.
,
Øverli
,
Ø.
,
Thörnqvist
,
P.-O.
and
Hoglund
,
E.
(
2011
).
Response to environmental change in rainbow trout selected for divergent stress coping styles
.
Physiol. Behav.
102
,
317
-
322
.
Sadoul
,
B.
,
Blumstein
,
D. T.
,
Alfonso
,
S.
and
Geffroy
,
B.
(
2021
).
Human protection drives the emergence of a new coping style in animals
.
PLoS Biol.
19
,
e3001186
.
Saeji
,
J. P. J.
,
Vervurg-Van-Kemenade
,
L. B. M.
,
Van Muiswinkel
,
W. B.
and
Wiegertjes
,
G. F.
(
2003
).
Daily handling stress reduces resistance of carp to Trypanoplasma borreli: in vitro modulatory effects of cortisol on leukocyte function and apoptosis
.
Develop. Comp. Immunol
27
,
233
-
245
.
Sakakura
,
Y.
and
Tsukamoto
,
K
. (
1998
).
Ontogeny of aggressive behaviour in schools of yellowtail, Seriola quinqueradiata
. In
When Do Fishes Become Juveniles? Developments In Environmental Biology Of Fishes
, vol.
19
(ed.
G. H.
Copp
,
V.
Kováč
and
K.
Hensel
), pp.
231
-
242
.
Dordrecht
:
Springer
.
Sakakura
,
Y.
and
Tsukamoto
,
K.
(
2002
).
Onset and development of aggressive behavior in the early life stage of Japanese flounder
.
Fish. Sci.
68
,
854
-
861
.
Schjolden
,
J.
,
Stoskhus
,
A.
and
Winberg
,
S.
(
2005
).
Does individual variation in stress responses and agonistic behavior reflect divergent stress coping strategies in juvenile rainbow trout?
Physiol. Biochem. Zool.
78
,
715
-
723
.
Semsar
,
K.
,
Perreault
,
H. A. N.
and
Godwin
,
J.
(
2004
).
Fluoxetine-treated male wrasses exhibit low AVT expression
.
Brain Res.
1029
,
141
-
147
.
Simões
,
J. M.
,
Barata
,
E. N.
,
Harris
,
R. M.
,
O'Connell
,
L. A.
,
Hofmann
,
H. A.
and
Oliveira
,
R. F.
(
2015
).
Social odors conveying dominance and reproductive information induce rapid physiological and neuromolecular changes in a cichlid fish
.
BMC Genomics
16
,
114
.
Sloman
,
K. A.
and
Armstrong
,
J. D.
(
2002
).
Physiological effects of dominance hierarchies: laboratory artefacts or natural phenomena?
J. Fish Biol.
61
,
1
-
23
.
Sloman
,
K. A.
,
Baldwin
,
L.
,
Mcmahon
,
S.
and
Snellgrove
,
D.
(
2011
).
The effects of mixed-species assemblage on the behaviour and welfare of fish held in home aquaria
.
Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.
135
,
160
-
168
.
Sloman
,
K. A.
,
Bouyoucos
,
I. A.
,
Brooks
,
E. J.
and
Sneddon
,
L. U.
(
2019
).
Ethical considerations in fish research
.
J. Fish Biol.
94
,
556
-
577
.
Sneddon
,
L. U.
and
Wolfenden
,
D. C. C
. (
2019
).
Ornamental Fish (Actinopterygii)
. In
UFAW Companion Animal Handbook
(ed.
J.
Yeates
), pp.
440
-
466
.
Wiley Blackwell
.
Sneddon
,
L. U.
,
Hawkesworth
,
S.
,
Braithwaite
,
V. A.
and
Yerbury
,
J.
(
2006
).
Impact of environmental disturbance on the stability and benefits of individual status within dominance hierarchies
.
Ethology
112
,
437
-
447
.
Sneddon
,
L. U.
,
Schmidt
,
R.
,
Fang
,
Y.
and
Cossins
,
A. R.
(
2011
).
Molecular correlates of social dominance: a novel role for ependymin in aggression
.
PLoS ONE
6
,
e18181
.
Soares
,
M. C.
,
Cardoso
,
S. C.
,
Carvalho
,
T. D. S.
and
Maximino
,
C.
(
2018
).
Using model fish to study the biological mechanisms of cooperative behaviour: A future for translational research concerning social anxiety disorders? Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol
.
Biol. Psychiatry
.
82
,
205
-
215
. .
Stevens
,
C. H.
,
Reed
,
B. T.
and
Hawkins
,
P.
(
2021
).
Enrichment for laboratory zebrafish—a review of the evidence and the challenges
.
Animals
11
,
698
.
Summers
,
C. H.
and
Winberg
,
S.
(
2006
).
Interactions between the neural regulation of stress and aggression
.
J. Exp. Biol.
209
,
4581
-
4589
.
Teles
,
M. C.
,
Cardoso
,
S. D.
and
Oliveira
,
R. F.
(
2016a
).
Social plasticity relies on different neuroplasticity mechanisms across the brain social decision-making network in zebrafish
.
Front. Behav. Neurosci.
10
,
16
.
Teles
,
M. C.
,
Gozdowska
,
M.
,
Kalamarz-Kubiak
,
H.
,
Kulczykowska
,
E.
and
Oliveira
,
R. F.
(
2016b
).
Agonistic interactions elicit rapid changes in brain nonapeptide levels in zebrafish
.
Horm. Behav.
84
,
57
-
63
.
Thomson
,
J. S.
,
Watts
,
P. C.
,
Pottinger
,
T. G.
and
Sneddon
,
L. U.
(
2012
).
Plasticity of boldness in rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss: do hunger and predation influence risk-taking behaviour?
Horm. Behav.
61
,
750
-
757
.
Thomson
,
J. S.
,
Watts
,
P. C.
,
Pottinger
,
T. G.
and
Sneddon
,
L. U.
(
2016
).
HPI reactivity does not reflect changes in personality among trout introduced to bold or shy social groups
.
Behaviour
153
,
1589
-
1610
.
Tibbetts
,
E. A.
,
Pardo-Sanchez
,
J.
and
Weise
,
C.
(
2022
).
The establishment and maintenance of dominance hierarchies
.
Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B
377
,
20200450
.
Turnbull
,
J. F.
,
North
,
B. P.
,
Ellis
,
T.
,
Adams
,
C. E.
,
Bron
,
J.
,
Macintyre
,
C. M.
and
Huntingford
,
F. A
. (
2008
).
Stocking density and the welfare of farmed salmonids
. In
Fish Welfare
(ed.
E. J.
Branson
), pp
111
-
120
.
Oxford
:
Blackwell
.
Wallace
,
K. J.
,
Choudhary
,
K. D.
,
Kutty
,
L. A.
,
Le
,
D. H.
,
Lee
,
M. T.
,
Wu
,
K.
and
Hofmann
,
H. A.
(
2022
).
Social ascent changes cognition, behaviour and physiology in a highly social cichlid fish
.
Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B
377
,
20200448
.
Weber
,
D. N.
and
Ghorai
,
J. K.
(
2013
).
Experimental design affects social behavior outcomes in adult zebrafish developmentally exposed to lead
.
Zebrafish
10
,
294
-
302
.
Winberg
,
S.
and
Lepage
,
O.
(
1998
).
Elevation of brain 5-HT activity, POMC expression, and plasma cortisol in socially subordinate rainbow trout
.
American J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol.
274
,
R645
-
R654
.
Winberg
,
S.
and
Nilsson
,
G. E.
(
1993
).
Time-course of changes in brain serotonergic activity and brain tryptophan levels in dominant and subordinate juvenile arctic charr
.
J. Exp. Biol.
179
,
181
-
195
.
Winberg
,
S.
,
Nilsson
,
G. E.
and
Olsen
,
K. H.
(
1991
).
Social rank and brain levels of monoamines and monoamine metabolites in arctic-charr, Salvelinus alpinus (L)
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol.
168
,
241
-
246
.
Winberg
,
S.
,
Nilsson
,
G. E.
and
Olsen
,
K. H.
(
1992
).
Changes in brain serotonergic activity during hierarchical behavior in arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus L) are socially induced
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol.
170
,
93
-
99
.
Winberg
,
S.
and
Thörnqvist
,
P.-O.
(
2016
).
Role of brain serotonin in modulating fish behavior
.
Curr. Zool.
62
,
317
-
323
.
Winberg
,
S.
,
Øverli
,
O.
and
Lepage
,
O.
(
2001
).
Suppression of aggression in rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) by dietary L-tryptophan
.
J.Exp. Biol.
204
,
3867
-
3876
.
Winberg
,
S.
,
Schjolden
,
J.
,
Øverli
,
O.
and
Pottinger
,
T.
(
2007
).
Stress and stress coping in fish, behavioural correlates and neuroendocrine mechanisms
.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Mol. Integr. Physiol.
146
,
S77
.
Zhang
,
Z.
,
Fu
,
Y.
,
Zhang
,
Z.
,
Zhang
,
X.
and
Chen
,
S.
(
2021
).
A comparative study on two territorial fishes: the influence of physical enrichment on aggressive behavior
.
Animals
11
,
1868
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing or financial interests.