ABSTRACT
Foot placement can be selected to anticipate upcoming perturbations, but it is unclear how this anticipatory strategy is influenced by available response time or precise knowledge of the perturbation's characteristics. This study investigates anticipatory and reactive locomotor strategies for repeated underfoot perturbations with varying levels of temporal certainty, physical certainty, and available response time. Thirteen healthy adults walked with random underfoot perturbations from a mechanized shoe. Temporal certainty was challenged by presenting the perturbations with or without warning. Available response time was challenged by adjusting the timing of the warning before the perturbation. Physical certainty was challenged by making perturbation direction (inversion or eversion) unpredictable for certain conditions. Linear-mixed effects models assessed the effect of each condition on the percentage change of margin of stability and step width. For perturbations with one stride or less of response time, we observed few changes to step width or margin of stability. As response time increased to two strides, participants adopted wider steps in anticipation of the perturbation (P=0.001). Physical certainty had little effect on gait for the step of the perturbation, but participants recovered normal gait sooner when the physical nature of the perturbation was predictable (P<0.001). Despite having information about the timing and direction of upcoming perturbations, individuals do not develop perturbation-specific feedforward strategies. Instead, they use feedback control to recover normal gait after a perturbation. However, physical certainty appears to make the feedback controller more efficient and allows individuals to recover normal gait sooner.
INTRODUCTION
Bipedal gait requires active neuromotor control on a step-to-step basis to avoid falls (Bauby and Kuo, 2000). With each step, the center of mass (CoM) accelerates away from the current stance foot, and in order not to fall, one must redirect the CoM with a new step. It is generally accepted that foot placement is controlled relative to the CoM kinematic state (e.g. its position and velocity) and that this is the dominant mechanism for achieving stable gait (i.e. gait that successfully avoids falls) (Bruijn and van Dieën, 2018). For example, constraining the CoM during gait creates a reduction in step width (SW), and constraining mediolateral (ML) foot placement creates a similar reduction in ML CoM motion (Arvin et al., 2016a,b; Mahaki et al., 2019). Furthermore, perturbations to the CoM kinematic state during stance create a predictable shift in the subsequent foot placement (Hof and Duysens, 2013; Wang and Srinivasan, 2014). While the link between the kinematic state of the CoM and foot placement is well known, the relationship between foot placement and disruptions to the center of pressure (CoP) remains less clear.
Complex environments with underfoot perturbations that disrupt the CoP are common to daily living. In such environments, humans use a combination of anticipatory and reactive strategies to maintain balance. For instance, foot placement and joint torque strategies can be used in both feedforward (anticipatory) and feedback (reactive) manners when walking over complex terrain (Bruijn and van Dieën, 2018; Reimann et al., 2018). Precisely anticipating a gait disturbance requires: (1) certainty about when the perturbation will occur, (2) an internal representation of the physical disturbance (e.g. location and magnitude relative to one's own body state), and (3) enough time to develop and execute a motor command (Fig. 1). When individuals lack certainty about the physical nature or specific timing of an upcoming disturbance, a cautious gait strategy may be adopted, characterized by a shorter step length and wider SW (Hak et al., 2012; 2013; McAndrew Young et al., 2012). For example, healthy young adults have been shown to exhibit cautious gait when walking while blindfolded (Hallemans et al., 2009; Saucedo and Yang, 2017) and when they are generally aware of upcoming slip or translation perturbations but don't know the specific timing (Lawrence et al., 2015; Nestico et al., 2021). Yet, it remains unclear how the temporal and physical nature of upcoming perturbations contribute to cautious gait; perturbations with certain temporal and physical features, and sufficient available response time, may allow individuals to implement a precise anticipatory strategy specific to task demands.
We previously reported results in which healthy young adults completed walking tasks with underfoot eversion perturbations similar to stepping on a small rock that were either unexpected (no warning) or expected (warning tone approximately 600 ms before the perturbation) (Kreter et al., 2021). When perturbations were unexpected, participants exhibited no kinematic differences relative to their normal walking pattern. Yet, when given a warning tone one stride prior to a perturbation, participants exhibited a different strategy in the swing phase prior to foot placement. Specifically, the acceleration profile of the swing foot suggested that they adopted a more medial foot placement relative to their normal gait. In theory, a medial shift to foot placement would neutralize the lateral shift to the CoP from the perturbation and allow participants to retain a normal kinematic relationship between the CoP and CoM. We speculated that participants may have developed an internal model of the physical characteristics of the perturbation through sensory feedback and implemented an anticipatory strategy specific to the task's physical demands. However, the reliance on acceleration data made it difficult to conclusively characterize the anticipatory strategies that participants adopted.
The purpose of this study was to further probe how healthy adults anticipate well-defined CoP perturbations with differing levels of physical certainty, temporal certainty and available response time. Temporal certainty was challenged by delivering an underfoot perturbation with or without advanced warning, available response time was challenged by providing warnings of upcoming disturbances with 0.5, 1 and 2 strides’ notice before the perturbation, and physical certainty was challenged by randomly delivering perturbations that either inverted or everted the foot. We hypothesized that individuals would adopt a perturbation-specific anticipatory stepping strategy, seeking to minimize the disruption to the normal kinematic relationship between the CoM and CoP, if they knew when (temporal certainty) to expect the perturbation and how (physical certainty) it would disrupt their CoP. We also hypothesized that anticipatory stepping strategies would be limited as the available response time decreased. Finally, we expected that reducing an individual's certainty related to the physical characteristics of a perturbation would lead to a more cautious gait strategy featuring a larger margin of stability (MoS) and wider SW before the perturbation.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants
A power analysis (G*Power, Universitat Kiel, Germany) with an effect size of (Cohen's F=0.345), power threshold of 0.8, and a significance level of α=0.05 revealed a sample size of 13 participants would be appropriate for this study. An effect size (Cohen's F=0.69) was calculated using data from our previous study that had trials with unexpected (zero available response time) and expected (one stride available response time) perturbations in healthy control participants. As a result of the specific levels of response time tested here, this project was powered using an effect size equal to 50% of the effect size above (Cohen's F=0.345).
Thirteen healthy young adults [26.8±5.6 years old (mean±s.d.); 6 female, 7 male] provided written informed consent in this University of Utah institutional review board-approved study. Exclusion criteria included: (1) a history of neurological or behavioral pathologies that could explain balance deficits; (2) a history of musculoskeletal injuries within the past year that could impact balance; (3) any reconstructive surgery of the lower extremities; (4) and any current medications that could cause neuromotor deficits.
Protocol
Participants wore a set of custom shoes containing small, mechanized plastic blocks just proximal to the 1st and 5th metatarsophalangeal joints of the left foot (Fig. 2). The blocks were controlled by micro-servo motors housed in the sole of the shoe. The block and motor were recessed into the sole such that they would not interfere with normal gait (Kim et al., 2013). When triggered, the recessed block deployed during the swing phase of gait and resulted in a ∼6 deg inversion of eversion of the ankle. A pair of force-sensitive resistors on the heel of the shoe communicated with a microcontroller attached to the side of the shoe and registered heel strikes. For trials with perturbations, the control box would trigger the perturbation randomly between every 5th and 9th stride. During most trials, participants received a warning that the perturbation was upcoming. The warning came in the form of a loud beep, played through a pair of wireless headphones that the participants wore as they walked (Kreter et al., 2021). Participants were informed that the timing of the perturbation warning would remain consistent within trials but may change between trials.
While wearing the shoes, participants completed eight walking trials, six of which contained perturbations. Participants walked along an instrumented treadmill at a comfortable self-selected speed. Participants spent 10 min acclimating to treadmill walking before any trials were recorded. To determine preferred walking speed, we used the method reported by Jordan and colleagues (Jordan et al., 2007).
Participants completed six walking trials with perturbations, each 5 min in length, and a pair of 2 min trials without perturbations before and after the perturbation trials. The first perturbation trial was always an unexpected condition that included perturbations and no warning. The remaining five perturbation trials were split into two sequences that manipulated either available response time or physical certainty. The available response time sequence contained three randomized expected perturbation conditions with warning tones occurring one half of a stride (i.e. one step), one stride (i.e. two steps) and two strides (i.e. four steps) before the perturbation, respectively. The second sequence contained two additional conditions. The first involved expected inversion perturbations with a one stride warning. The second contained random delivery of inversion or eversion perturbations with a one stride warning. Before the first perturbation trial (unexpected condition), participants were informed that the following six trials would involve small underfoot perturbations that felt like stepping on a small rock. After the unexpected perturbation trial, participants were told that for the remaining perturbation trials they would hear warning tone before the perturbation occurred. Each perturbation trial included ∼30 perturbations (mean±s.d.=31.9±6.7 perturbations) (Fig. 3).
Instrumentation
A 3D motion capture system (Bonita V10, VICON, Oxford, UK) surrounding the testing environment captured positional marker data at a rate of 200 Hz. Kinetic data were recorded at a rate of 1000 Hz from two force plates embedded beneath the left and right belts of an instrumented treadmill (BERTEC, Columbus, OH). Participants were outfitted with reflective markers in a custom lower-body marker configuration. The custom marker configuration included markers bilaterally at the anterior superior iliac spine (ASIS), posterior superior iliac spine (PSIS), the lateral epicondyle of the femur and the lateral malleolus. Four markers were placed on each shoe including at the 1st, 2nd and 5th metatarsophalangeal joints, and the calcaneus. Forces, moments and CoP position were recorded from the force platforms embedded under each belt of the treadmill.
Data processing and statistical analysis
Motion capture data was processed in Vicon Nexus (v. 2.12). Gait events, outcome measures and statistical tests were computed with a custom code in MATLAB (v. R2020b, The MathWorks Inc., Natick MA, USA). Kinetic and kinematic data were filtered using a 4th order phaseless low-pass Butterworth filter. Heel strike and toe-off events were determined with an algorithm using the maximal displacement of the heel and toe markers from the sacrum (Zeni et al., 2008). Perturbation steps were identified by the change in foot roll angle during stance. For statistical analysis, two anticipatory steps, the perturbation step and three recovery steps were isolated and compared against the inter-trial unperturbed steps. To account for participants acclimating to the association between the warning tone and perturbation in each trial, the first three perturbations in each trial were excluded from data analysis. The number of steps to return to normal gait following the perturbation was also determined using the 95% confidence interval of the percent difference between perturbation steps and normal steps.
Lateral MoS (Hof et al., 2005) and SW were calculated for each step of each trial as measures of gait stability. MoS is a measure of dynamic stability that assesses the distance between the extrapolated center of mass (xCoM) and the base of support (BoS) (Hof, 2008; Watson et al., 2021). The ML position of the CoP was used to approximate the lateral BoS. The geometric center between the four reflective markers of the pelvis was used to estimate the position of the CoM. The velocity of the CoM (vCoM) was calculated using a central difference algorithm. For this study, the lateral direction was defined using the global reference frame aligned with the treadmill. However, to clarify interpretation of MoS, the positive direction was opposite for left and right steps. MoS was identified at contralateral toe-off for each step and SW calculated as the ML distance between heel markers at heel strike on successive steps. The percentage change in MoS and SW were calculated relative to the mean of normal steps within a given trial. Steps were classified as normal if they were not within the four steps preceding or following a perturbation. Mass-normalized vertical impulse, stancetime and swingtime were also calculated across each step (see Supplementary Materials and Methods and Figs S1–S3).
To test the effects of temporal certainty, physical certainty, and available response time on balance measures within subjects, linear mixed-effects models were fit for each outcome with fixed effects of condition and random intercepts for subjects. A test of fixed effects assessed significant differences between conditions. Pairwise contrasts were performed for conditions that reported a significant test of fixed effects. Supplementary correlation analyses were performed to assess the association between MoS during the perturbation step and SW at the first recovery step. All statistical tests were assessed at α=0.05.
RESULTS
Effect of available response time and temporal certainty on MoS and SW
Changes to available response time and temporal certainty had no statistically significant effects on MoS for any steps surrounding the perturbation with the exception of the second step after the perturbation (Fig. 4). For the second step after the perturbation, there was a significant effect of condition on MoS (P=0.047). Pairwise contrasts confirmed the significant effect of response time on MoS during the second recovery step, with two strides of warning condition exhibiting a larger increase to MoS than the condition with one stride (P=0.006). There were no significant effects of temporal certainty on MoS for any step (Table 1).
Available response time had a significant effect on SW at two steps before the perturbation (P=0.046), the perturbation step (P=0.005), the second recovery step (P=0.009) and the third recovery step (P=0.015). Pairwise contrasts confirmed the significant effects at each step. Specifically, two steps before the perturbation, participants had an increased step width in the two-stride warning condition relative to the one-stride warning condition (P=0.009). Participants exhibited a larger SW during the perturbation steps with two strides of available response time than during the perturbation steps of any other level of available response time (one-stride, P=0.002; half-stride, P=0.008). At the second recovery step, only the one-stride warning condition returned to a normal SW and was significantly less than the two-stride warning condition (P=0.004). By the third recovery step, all conditions returned to normal; however, the two-stride and half-stride warning conditions were still significantly elevated relative to the one-stride warning condition (two-stride, P=0.006; half-stride, P=0.016) (Fig. 4). Contrasts revealed a significant effect of temporal certainty on SW in the step before the perturbation (P=0.024) and the step of the perturbation (P=0.009).
Effect of physical certainty on MoS and SW
Varying the physical certainty had no significant effect on MoS were observed during the preparatory steps or perturbation step (Fig. 5). There was a significant effect of condition on MoS during the second (P=0.014) and third (P=0.004) recovery steps following the perturbation. During the second recovery step, MoS was greater in the mixed perturbation condition compared with the eversion perturbation condition (P=0.004). For the third recovery step, MoS was significantly greater during the mixed perturbation condition compared with both eversion (P=0.002) and inversion (P=0.015) conditions (Table 2).
There was a significant effect of physical certainty on SW during the each of the three recovery steps [step 1 (P=0.044), step 2 (P=0.009) and step 3 (P<0.001)]. For the initial recovery step, pairwise contrasts revealed that SW was significantly wider for the inversion condition than the eversion condition (P=0.013), but not the mixed condition (P=0.213). During the second recovery step, participants exhibited a significantly wider SW during the mixed condition than both the eversion (P=0.002) and inversion (P=0.039) conditions. During the third recovery step, SW was still significantly wider during the mixed perturbation condition relative to the eversion condition (P<0.001), but not the inversion condition (P=0.058) (Fig. 5).
Relationship between MoS during perturbation and SW during recovery
MoS during the perturbation step was a significant predictor of SW during the recovery step for all conditions (Fig. 6). The MoS during eversion with one stride of warning explained the least amount of variance in SW during the first recovery step (r2=0.22, P<0.001) and the MoS during inversion with one stride of warning exhibited the strongest relationship (r2=0.43, P<0.001). For trials testing the effect of available response time, MoS during the perturbation step explained 32% of the variance in SW during the first recovery step (r2=0.32, P<0.001). For trials testing the effect of physical certainty, MoS during the perturbation step explained 32% of the variance in SW during the first recovery step (r2=0.32, P<0.001).
DISCUSSION
This study aimed to further investigate the effects of: (1) temporal certainty, (2) available response time and (3) physical certainty on kinematics during gait with expected underfoot perturbations. We predicted that, with greater temporal certainty, participants would select a specific foot placement to counteract the shift to CoP from the underfoot perturbation. We expected that this strategy would become less feasible, and thus less frequent, as available response time decreased. For this perturbation, we did not find a significant effect of temporal certainty or available response time except when two strides of warning were provided. For steps with greater physical certainty, we expected participants to implement a task-specific foot placement strategy to mitigate changes to underfoot CoP, but adopt a cautious strategy when the perturbation was less physically certain. Specifically, we expected a wider SW for inversion perturbations and a narrower SW for eversion perturbations, but no change to MoS. We did not observe perturbation-specific foot placement strategies with greater physical certainty; however, participants required more steps to recover their normal gait when perturbations had less physical certainty.
Temporal certainty and available response time have little impact on anticipatory or recovery strategies
Counter to our hypothesis, we did not observe a reduced SW before temporally certain eversion perturbations, regardless of available response time. The most noteworthy change to stepping behavior occurred during the two-stride warning condition, where participants adopted a wider SW in anticipation of the perturbation. One explanation for the increase with two strides of warning is that the warning may have been delivered too early. Potential stepping locations are typically sampled during the stance phase one stride prior to heel contact, and if visual information for stepping locations are available outside this time frame, but not during, stepping error increases (Matthis and Fajen, 2014; Matthis et al., 2017). Planning foot placement one stride in advance is biomechanically advantageous because it allows humans to fine-tune the balance between energetically efficient gait and stability. More explicitly, it allows for the use of foot placement in the preceding step to tailor the location of the BoS while the trailing limb can adjust push-off force and change CoM trajectory (Kuo and Donelan, 2010). Planning steps too far in advance does not necessarily have a biomechanical disadvantage, but it does require an individual to store a larger number of prepared foot placement locations in their working memory. In object manipulation tasks, humans minimize the use of visual working memory until it is necessary to complete the task at hand (Ballard et al., 1995). In a stepping task such as ours, storing the information for a specific foot placement four steps ahead (i.e. the two-stride warning condition) while also planning the three preceding steps may be impractical, leading participants to adopt a less effective anticipatory strategy instead.
The absence of a clear anticipatory strategy may also be due to the participants' inability to use vision to anticipate the disturbance. Foot placement is primarily guided by visual information during locomotion (Patla, 2004), and individuals may track obstacles until they step on them in the real world. Here, participants were forced to create an internal model of the perturbation's physical qualities by integrating proprioceptive information over repeated exposures. Participants had no reason to direct foot placement through vision as the disturbance was unavoidable and couldn't be seen. Given the futility of anticipating foot placement through vision, participants may have instead focused on developing an effective recovery strategy reliant on proprioceptive feedback.
Physical certainty of upcoming perturbations allows for quicker recovery of stability
Counter to our hypotheses, there were no clear anticipatory adjustments to MoS or SW for the steps leading up to the perturbation. However, participants in our study achieved a normal SW sooner when the perturbation had predictable physical features than when it randomly switched between inversion and eversion. Notably, when inversion and eversion perturbations from the mixed condition were assessed separately, the changes to MoS during the perturbation step and SW during the first recovery step mirrored the changes observed in the physically certain trials (Fig. 5). The quicker recovery during trials with predictable perturbations suggests that individuals are able to form a physical representation of the perturbations features and prime a response that allows them to better anticipate how a perturbation will disrupt their gait and recover more effectively. Groups with deficits to sensory integration may struggle to recover normal gait around similar perturbations in everyday life. When exposed to perturbations from a similar shoe mechanism, persons with peripheral neuropathy struggled to modify step length in response to unexpected inversion perturbations (Allet et al., 2014) and older adults adopt a hip torque strategy (Kim et al., 2013), typically reserved for larger disturbances. Future work should focus on groups with neuromotor deficits and incorporate neurological recordings such as electroencephalography (EEG) to better delineate the formation and execution of motor plans in anticipation of known upcoming disturbances (Nordin et al., 2019).
Feedforward/feedback controllers
When planning foot placement during locomotion, humans incorporate both feedforward and feedback control strategies to optimize efficiency and stability (Bruijn and van Dieën, 2018). Selection of feedforward or feedback strategies is dependent on a multitude of variables, including complexity of the walking environment and certainty related to upcoming disturbances. Feedforward locomotor strategies allow humans to anticipate gait disturbances by predicting a future body state, whereas feedback strategies are used to assess instantaneous body state to recover a normal state after a disturbance. We hypothesized that greater temporal and/or physical certainty related to upcoming perturbations would allow participants to adopt a specific feedforward strategy that incorporated proprioceptive information from repeated exposures to our perturbation to predict the effect of upcoming perturbations. Yet, our results suggest that participants primarily relied on a feedback control strategy to manage frontal plane stability. Specifically, the disruption to MoS during the perturbation step influenced SW during the first recovery step (Fig. 6). Even for the condition with two strides of warning, where participants did exhibit feedforward control prior to the perturbation, MoS during the perturbation step was still a good predictor of SW in the first recovery step. These results suggest that the perturbation to the CoP may be influencing CoM dynamics throughout stance and influencing the subsequent foot placement, which is congruent with studies showing that instantaneous CoM dynamics guide foot placement in the subsequent step (Arvin et al., 2016b; Mahaki et al., 2019).
While a longer available response time failed to demonstrably aid perturbation recovery, greater physical certainty allowed participants to implement a more effective feedback strategy and return to normal gait after just one or two recovery steps. The use of a feedback control strategy may be due to the infinite number of body states an individual can adopt to accomplish a step with a perturbation. While step-to-step kinematic differences are subtle, any change creates a novel kinematic state to predict a disturbance within; our previous study with a similar perturbation demonstrated different magnitudes of ankle roll for different stepping characteristics (Kreter et al., 2021). Even though the disturbance in this study has fixed physical qualities, predicting its exact impact on body state with a feedforward model is less certain than waiting to develop a feedback model, especially given the amount of time individuals have between each step (∼500 ms). Other recent studies with predictable and unpredictable shifts to the CoM during gait also suggest that individuals use feedback from instantaneous shifts to the kinematic state to recover stability and that certainty related to the timing or direction of a disturbance may just enhance the feedback control strategy (Major et al., 2020). Additionally, the perturbations used in our study are very small and pose little threat to stability for healthy adults; future work should investigate perturbations of greater magnitude or perturbations with a faster cadence as these factors may force individuals to weigh feedforward and feedback control strategies differently.
Limitations
This study should be interpreted within the context of its limitations. First, only the left foot received perturbations. A unilateral perturbation was selected to simplify the interpretation of the warning. Participants may have adopted gait strategies to support the left limb and mitigate the impact of perturbations. Such strategies may have included anticipatory strategies that were not measured in this study such as increasing joint level stiffness, changing reflex gains, and increasing cognitive focus. Additionally, the subtle differences between treadmill and overground walking may have influenced participant gait. For instance, treadmill walking keeps gait speed constant and removes the ability to arrest gait in the face of destabilization or significantly vary step length without coincident variation of cadence. When walking overground in everyday life, such options are available and provide a wider range of strategies to select from and greater maneuverability. Participants may have also altered their stepping behavior because of the gap between the treadmill belts and the limited width of the treadmill for forward progression. Finally, the small size of the perturbation and repeated exposure may have resulted in participants not considering it a major threat after the initial exposures.
Conclusion
This study investigated the effects of underfoot perturbations during gait with varying levels of physical certainty, temporal certainty and available response time. Contrary to our hypotheses, greater certainty (temporal or physical) did not contribute to the adoption of more refined anticipatory kinematic gait strategies. Rather, participants waited to assess instantaneous body state during the perturbation and recovered stable gait by adjusting stepping behavior in the steps following, and greater physical certainty allowed individuals to recovery normal gait sooner. A priori knowledge related to physical certainty may promote greater success in paradigms aimed at improvement of locomotor stability.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Tyler Ho and Claire Rogers for their assistance with data collection and processing.
Footnotes
Author contributions
Conceptualization: N.K., K.E.G., P.C.F.; Methodology: N.K.; Software: C.L.; Formal analysis: N.K.; Investigation: N.K.; Resources: C.L.; Data curation: N.K.; Writing - original draft: N.K.; Writing - review & editing: N.K., C.L., K.E.G., P.C.F.; Visualization: N.K.; Supervision: P.C.F.; Funding acquisition: P.C.F.
Funding
This work was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shiver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development of the National Institutes of Health (award no. K12HD073945 to P.C.F.). Opinions, interpretations, and conclusions are those of the author and are not necessarily endorsed by the funders. Deposited in PMC for release after 12 months.
References
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing or financial interests.