Behavioural innovation and problem solving are widely considered to be important mechanisms by which animals respond to novel environmental challenges, including those induced by human activities. Despite their functional and ecological relevance, much of our current understanding of these processes comes from studies in vertebrates. Understanding of these processes in invertebrates has lagged behind partly because they are not perceived to have the cognitive machinery required. This perception is, however, challenged by recent evidence demonstrating sophisticated cognitive capabilities in insects despite their small brains. Here, we studied innovation, defined as the capacity to solve a new task, of a solitary bee (Osmia cornuta) in the laboratory by exposing naive individuals to an obstacle removal task. We also studied the underlying cognitive and non-cognitive mechanisms through a battery of experimental tests designed to measure associative learning, exploration, shyness and activity levels. We found that solitary bees can innovate, with 11 of 29 individuals (38%) being able to solve a new task consisting of lifting a lid to reach a reward. However, the propensity to innovate was uncorrelated with the measured learning capacity, but increased with exploration, boldness and activity. These results provide solid evidence that non-social insects can solve new tasks, and highlight the importance of interpreting innovation in the light of non-cognitive processes.

Animals exhibit an extraordinarily wide repertoire of behaviours. Bees, for example, have developed a broad range of sophisticated behaviours that facilitate foraging, nesting, navigation and communication (Roulston and Goodell, 2011; Loukola et al., 2017). Although the ecological and evolutionary importance of behaviour is widely recognized, our current understanding of how new behaviours emerge is insufficiently understood. Some simple behaviours have a clear genetic basis, and hence may have been acquired through mutation and natural selection. Studies in Drosophila show, for example, that a mutation in a single neuropeptide caused several abnormalities in their behavioural circadian rhythms (i.e. biological clocks; Renn et al., 1999). However, the accumulation of mutations seems insufficient to explain the emergence of new behaviours. Rather, the development of novel behaviours from more simple cognitive processes requires the processing of new knowledge by means of experience to guide decision making (Dukas, 2008). The emergence of new learnt behaviours is a process known as behavioural innovation (Ramsey et al., 2007; Lefebvre et al., 2004; Reader, 2003; Sol, 2003).

The concept of innovation has attracted considerable interest because of its broad implications for ecology and evolution (Ramsey et al., 2007; Lefebvre et al., 2004; Reader, 2003; Sol, 2003). Innovation suggests the possibility of constructing plastic behavioural responses to novel ecological challenges, thereby potentially enhancing the fitness of the individual animals when exposed to unusual or novel situations. For instance, evidence is accumulating that innovation ability enhances the success of animals when introduced to novel environments (Sol et al., 2005). By changing the relationship of individuals with the environment, innovative behaviours also have great potential to influence the evolutionary responses of the population to selective pressures (Lefebvre et al., 2004; Reader et al., 2016). Hence, in the context of global change, innovative behaviours are considered central to understanding how animals will respond to rapid changes induced by human activities.

While innovation is considered to be one of the main processes behind the emergence of novel behaviours in vertebrates (Reader, 2003; Ramsey et al., 2007), the relevance of innovation is currently insufficiently understood in insects. Insect behaviour has been studied for many years (e.g. Maeterlinck, 1901), and the traditional notion holds that it tends to be relatively inflexible and stereotypical. This perception partially arises because of their small brains and smaller number of neurons than in more studied taxa like mammals or birds (Dukas, 2008). Such a belief is, however, changing as evidence accumulates of unsuspected sophisticated capabilities beyond the most simple associative learning that transcend basic forms of cognition (Avargues-Weber et al., 2012), including rule learning (Gil et al., 2007), numerosity (Howard et al., 2018; Howard et al., 2019; Dacke and Srinivasan, 2008), cultural transmission (Alem et al., 2016) and exploratory learning (Menzel and Giurfa, 2001; Degen et al., 2016). Even adaptations to global change have been observed recently, such as solitary bees cutting plastic bags instead of leaves to build their nests (Allasino et al., 2019). The fact that insects exhibit sophisticated responses to new situations suggests that this new behaviours may also be commonly acquired through the process of innovation.

Here, we addressed the critical questions of whether insects are capable of innovation and how they achieve it. For example, innovation may be a result of trial and error, rather than a more complex cognitive process. We used a solitary common bee – Osmia cornuta (Megachilidae) – as a model system to address these questions. While our current understanding of cognition in solitary bees is limited in comparison to that of eusocial species (e.g. Chittka and Thompson, 2009), solitary bees are easy to rear and manipulate in captivity (Jin et al., 2015). An advantage of solitary bees is that they can be tested individually for innovative propensity without having to consider the pitfalls of separating individuals from the social group. Importantly, solitary bees comprise most of the bee fauna and are suffering worldwide population declines associated with rapid human-induced environmental change (Goulson et al., 2015), posing at risk the essential pollination services that they provide for cultivated crops and wild plants (Ollerton et al., 2011). Thus, there is an urgent need to assess whether and how they are capable of innovation to cope with new environmental challenges.

The capacity to innovate is difficult to measure directly (Lefebvre et al., 2004), but one widely adopted approach is the use of problem-solving experiments motivated by a food reward (Bouchard et al., 2007; Griffin et al., 2014). In our experiments, we exposed naive O. cornuta bees to a novel task consisting of lifting a lid to reach a food reward, an assay that mimics the encounter of a new complex flower. Whether or not individuals solve the task and the latency to do so can be used as measures of innovation performance (Sol et al., 2011). Because some bees were capable of innovation, we investigated the underlying mechanisms. We first explored whether the propensity to innovate reflects a domain-general ability to learn. Hence, we related our measures of innovation performance to measures of performance in a simple associative learning test. Next, we tested the effect of a number of emotional and state-dependent intrinsic features that are suspected to either facilitate or inhibit innovation (Reader, 2003; Houston and McNamara, 1999; Sol et al., 2012), including exploration, shyness and activity levels. We finally considered whether problem-solving ability might be explained by sex, an additional intrinsic parameter (Houston and McNamara, 1999). In O. cornuta, females are more involved in parental activities (e.g. they are in charge of all nest provisioning activities) and are typically larger than males (Bosch, 1994). These fundamental differences in the biology and ecology between sexes are expected to affect how they deal with novel challenges, potentially affecting their problem-solving ability.

Study subjects

Osmia cornuta (Latreille 1805) cocoons were bought from the company WAB-Mauerbienenzucht (Konstanz, Germany) and kept cold at 4°C. Before and during the experiments, cocoons were put in 15 ml Falcon tubes in a pitch black environment and kept in an incubator at 26°C for 24–48 h until the emergence of offspring. In total, 101 females and 42 males were born, and used in the experiments. In order to force bees to walk instead of fly, we anaesthetized them with a cold shock treatment and cut their right wings (Jin et al., 2014; Crook, 2013). Walking bees have been used in behavioural essays before, showing consistent responses (Jin et al., 2014).

Experimental device

We conducted the experiments in a controlled environment laboratory at the Institut für Biologie–Neurobiologie (Freire Universität Berlin) from February to April 2017. Behavioural assays were conducted in an experimental device composed of two parts, the ‘arena’ (Fig. 1A) and the ‘dome’ (Fig. 1B). The arena was a 30×30×10 cm empty methacrylate rectangular prism with no roof, containing a piece of grey cardboard as the floor and placed over a wooden structure. The dome was a dark brown upside-down plastic flowerpot, illuminated homogeneously with attached LED lamps. The dome covered the arena to create a controlled environment for the experiments. We attached different geometrical figure patterns in the inside walls to facilitate the orientation of the bees during the tests (Jin et al., 2014). The dome had a hole in the roof to attach a video camera to record the tests. Citral odour was perfused evenly and restored regularly, as it is known to stimulate bumblebees, and probably other bees, during foraging (Lunau, 1991; Shearer and Boch, 1966).

Fig. 1.

Experimental setup. (A) The experimental arena, consisting of a neutral grey floor, surrounded by plastic walls with plastic cornices attached to prevent the bees from escaping. (B) The dome used to cover with arena, with a landscape pattern displayed inside and a webcam placed at the top to record the experiments. (C–F) The experiment was composed of four different displays. In assay 1 (C), the bee started inside a refuge. The aim of the assay was to see whether the bee stayed in the refuge (as a shyness proxy), and/or explored the colour cues around it. In assays 2 and 3 (D), the bee was exposed to two drinking stalks, one rewarded and the other empty. The colour was randomly selected but maintained across the assays. In assay 4 (E), the learning test, the display was the same as in assay 2 and 3, but this time we removed the reward and both stalks were empty. In assay 5 (F), the innovation test, the display was the same as in assay 2 and 3, but this time we covered the reward with a lid, forcing the bee to innovate to lift the lid to access the reward.

Fig. 1.

Experimental setup. (A) The experimental arena, consisting of a neutral grey floor, surrounded by plastic walls with plastic cornices attached to prevent the bees from escaping. (B) The dome used to cover with arena, with a landscape pattern displayed inside and a webcam placed at the top to record the experiments. (C–F) The experiment was composed of four different displays. In assay 1 (C), the bee started inside a refuge. The aim of the assay was to see whether the bee stayed in the refuge (as a shyness proxy), and/or explored the colour cues around it. In assays 2 and 3 (D), the bee was exposed to two drinking stalks, one rewarded and the other empty. The colour was randomly selected but maintained across the assays. In assay 4 (E), the learning test, the display was the same as in assay 2 and 3, but this time we removed the reward and both stalks were empty. In assay 5 (F), the innovation test, the display was the same as in assay 2 and 3, but this time we covered the reward with a lid, forcing the bee to innovate to lift the lid to access the reward.

Experimental protocol

Over 3 days, each individual took part in a sequence of 5 behavioural assays (Fig. 1C–F) of 15 min each designed to measure five different behaviours: exploration, shyness, activity, learning and innovation (see Table 1). We waited 4 h between trials if the next trial was done the same day and around 16 h if the next trial needed to be done the next day (Fig. 1C–F). First, we defined innovation as the ability to solve a problem in a new situation, i.e. lifting a cardboard cue in a new environment. Because the mechanisms behind innovation are complex and we do not know what may be driving it, we controlled for related behaviours within and in independent tests. Activity, measured as the proportion of time in movement, not resting, was measured for every trial. Individuals did not show any correlation in their activity levels over the trials (Fig. S1) and, therefore, we did not estimate a single average activity value for each individual. Activity levels did not decrease over the trials (linear model Activity∼Trial, estimate±s.e.=0.003±0.008, P=0.718). Note that not every bee survived to perform all the assays; only 45% of the individuals that started the experiment reached the final assay. Although individuals were not fed during the experimental process other than during the trials, the lack of correlation between the number of feeding events and activity rates during the leaning test (Pearson correlation=−0.09) or the innovation test (Pearson correlation=−0.01) suggests that this high mortality is not attributable to starvation.

Table 1.

Allvariables measured during the tests

Allvariables measured during the tests
Allvariables measured during the tests

The first assay aimed at measuring exploration and shyness. Note that these tests represent snapshots of bee behaviour, and as such should be interpreted as transient behaviours for which the stability over time is unknown. The arena included four coloured cardboard cues (2 blue and 2 yellow; Fig. 1C). The bee was placed in a little cardboard refuge and was kept inside it for 5 min to allow habituation. Next, the refuge was opened and the individual was allowed to explore the arena. To quantify exploration, we recorded whether the bee touched all the cardboard cues during the assay and the time it took to do so. Shyness was measured as the initial time spent inside the refuge (Table 1). Re-entering the refuge was originally thought to be an indicator of shyness; however, analysis of the videos showed that bees did not re-enter the refuge to stay inside and hide, but rather did so as part of their arena exploration.

The second and third assays were the associative learning assays, where we trained bees to associate a colour with a reward (Fig. 1D). The individuals started all tests inside a black opaque box cover that was lifted at the start of the experiment. We displayed two cardboards cues with drinking stalks on them, one rewarded with 50% sucrose solution and the other empty. Blue and yellow cardboard are well discriminated by bees (Vorobyev and Menzel, 1999; Hempel de Ibarra et al., 2014). Hence, the reward for each individual was randomly assigned to one of these two colours for both trials and we let individuals explore the stalks and eat ad libitum over a period of 15 min. The position (left or right) of the reward was randomly assigned for each individual in each trial.

In the fourth assay, the learning test, we tested whether individuals had learned to associate colours with rewards as trained. The test consisted of both cues displayed as in the second and third assays, but this time with both stalks empty (Fig. 1E). We measured whether the individuals approached the formerly rewarded coloured cue and quantified the time it took to do so. Otherwise, if the bee first checked the wrong feeder, we considered this as a failure in the learning test. To ensure that bees had learned to associate colour and reward, we switched the colour of the rewarded stalk between the two learning assays in 36 randomly selected individuals (hereafter, ‘control’ group). While more sophisticated general learning abilities could not be measured, associative learning is a widespread elemental skill in insects which can be easily measured and serves as a first-order test for the importance of simple learning processes.

In the final assay, we measured the propensity for innovation by using the same coloured cue and reward combination as in assays 2 and 3, but this time the stalk containing the reward was covered with a cardboard lid (Fig. 1F). Bees had thus to innovate, i.e. lift the cardboard, to reach the reward. Innovation propensity was measured in terms of innovation success and latency to innovate (Table 1). Control bees used in the learning assays were not tested for innovation.

Data analysis

We modelled problem-solving performance in the innovation assay as a function of associative learning, shyness, exploration and activity (see Table 1 for definitions). We first modelled the success or failure in solving the task using a Bayesian generalized linear model with a Bernoulli family and a logit link (Package brms; Bürkner, 2017). Second, to model the latency to solve the task, we instead used survival analyses based on Cox proportional hazards regressions for continuous predictors (Cox and Oakes, 2018; Table 2). Survival analysis allowed us to account for censored data, as some individuals did not pass the test within the stipulated time frame. Hence, for individuals not solving a particular task (e.g. exploration or learning), we assigned to them a maximum latency of 15 min and consider them censored individuals.

Table 2.

Multivariate modelcoefficients for innovation success and learning as a function of latency learning, shyness, exploration and activity

Multivariate modelcoefficients for innovation success and learning as a function of latency learning, shyness, exploration and activity
Multivariate modelcoefficients for innovation success and learning as a function of latency learning, shyness, exploration and activity

In order to avoid model over-parametrization, we used only the quantitative proxies of shyness, exploration and learning (i.e. latency; Table 1). However, note that latency to learn and learning success are related one to each other, as bees not passing the test where assigned a maximum latency of 15 min and the most successful bees solved the test in <2 min. This creates a bimodal distribution which reflects learning success. Using learning success instead does not change the our conclusions. In addition, as activity levels were variable across trials (Fig. S1), we only included activity levels during the test evaluated. Sex was not added as a co-variable because of the limited sample size and skewed proportion of females (6 males, 23 females). Finally, learning success and latency were modelled in a similar way to innovation; that is, as a function of shyness, exploration and activity during the learning test, but this time including sex (9 males, 34 females).

In summary, for innovation, we built multivariate models with latency to exit the refuge (i.e. shyness), latency to explore the full arena (i.e. exploration), latency to perform the associative learning test (i.e. learning) and activity as predictors. For learning, we built multivariate models with latency to exit the refuge (i.e. shyness), latency to explore the full arena (i.e. exploration), activity and sex.

Our experiments showed that O. cornuta bees were able to innovate. Eleven out of the 29 bees we tested for innovation solved the novel task, lifting the lid to reach the reward within the 15 min of the assay. Detailed observations show that 10 out the 29 individuals innovating did so by using the mandibles or the head, and after some seconds exploring the lid, while only one individual opened it accidentally while walking straight forwards and found the reward below. Osmia cornuta bees were also able to learn, with 63% of individuals succeeding in the learning test (n=48, χ2=3, d.f.=1, P=0.08), while control bees had a success rate closer to that expected by random (n=36, 52% success, χ2=0.11, d.f.=1, P=0.74). Males tended to learn better than females, showing slightly higher success rates (Table 2C) and learning faster (Table 2D). However, latency to innovate showed no relationship with associative learning (Table 2B, Fig. 2B).

Fig. 2.

Innovation related to each measured behaviour. (A) Shyness, (B) learning, (C) exploration and (D) activity. The graphs plot the estimates extracted from the multivariate model described in Table 2A, measuring success (1) or failure (0) in the innovation test. Shading represents confidence intervals. Points represent individual performance.

Fig. 2.

Innovation related to each measured behaviour. (A) Shyness, (B) learning, (C) exploration and (D) activity. The graphs plot the estimates extracted from the multivariate model described in Table 2A, measuring success (1) or failure (0) in the innovation test. Shading represents confidence intervals. Points represent individual performance.

Instead, innovation success and latency were better explained by individual differences in shyness, exploration and activity (Fig. 2, Table 2). First, shyer individuals were the worst innovators. The probability of innovating dropped from 0.80 for bees that spent 2 s inside the refuge to 0.01 for bees that did not leave the refuge in the first assay (Table 2A, Fig. 2A). Shyer individuals were also slower at solving the innovation test (Table 2B). In fact, out of all bees that did not leave the refuge in the first test (our proxy of shyness) and reached the innovation test, none of them passed the innovation test in subsequent assays.

Second, slower explorers were also better at the innovation test. Bees that spent more time solving the exploration test had a greater chance of succeeding in the innovation test (Table 2A, Fig. 2C). These individuals also solved the innovation test faster (Table 2B). Finally, active bees during the innovation test had a better chance of solving the innovation test (Table 2A, Fig. 2D), as evidenced by a positive correlation between the velocity at solving the test and the proportion of time active during the test (Table 2B). Unlike innovation, learning was not affected by shyness, exploration or activity (Table 2B,C; Fig. 3).

Fig. 3.

Learning related to each measured behaviour. (A) Shyness, (B) exploration, (C) activity and (D) sex. The graphs are extracted from the multivariate model described in Table 2C, measuring success (1) or failure (0) in the learning test. The width of the bars in D is proportional to the number of individuals tested. Shading in A–C represents confidence intervals. Points represent individual performance.

Fig. 3.

Learning related to each measured behaviour. (A) Shyness, (B) exploration, (C) activity and (D) sex. The graphs are extracted from the multivariate model described in Table 2C, measuring success (1) or failure (0) in the learning test. The width of the bars in D is proportional to the number of individuals tested. Shading in A–C represents confidence intervals. Points represent individual performance.

Innovation-like behaviours have previously been observed in wild solitary bees. These include the use of new materials for nesting (Allasino et al., 2019) and anecdotal examples of bees nesting in new places, such as cardboard, wooden blocks (Bosch and Kemp, 2001) or Styrofoam blocks (MacIvor and Moore, 2013). However, innovation in the context of problem solving had never been demonstrated before in controlled laboratory experiments with solitary bees. Here, we show that 38% of the tested individuals were able to lift the lid, and this was mainly achieved by using their mandibles or head, and not by accidentally walking around the spur.

Although innovation is generally believed to be a dimension of domain-general cognition (Lefebvre et al., 2004), we did not find evidence that individuals that were better at associative learning solved the innovation task faster. The failure to relate innovation and associative learning does not simply reflect that we studied associative learning over shorter training periods, as success in the learning test was comparable with that found in previous similar experiments using more training days (e.g. Jin et al., 2014: Jin et al., 2015). However, associative learning is a simple elemental skill available to most animal species, even with the simplest nervous systems (Zhang et al., 2005), and perhaps more sophisticated general learning abilities (Bouchard et al., 2007) show a different connection with innovation capacity.

Regardless, a more likely explanation is that other factors are more relevant for innovation and may have masked the effect of associative learning. Indeed, we found consistent differences between fast and slow innovators in their tendency to approach and explore the experimental apparatus. Specifically, individuals that were able to lift the lid to access the food reward tended to be bolder and to explore more slowly than those that failed to solve the task. As suggested for other taxa, there may be a trade-off between exploration speed and accuracy, which could translate into how information is processed. For example, in great tits (Parus major), fast explorers return more quickly to previously experienced foraging patches whereas slow explorers prefer to seek new information or update old information close to the feeders (Matthysen et al., 2010). Boldness and exploration have previously been identified as important determinants of innovation propensity in vertebrates and highlight that innovation propensity may largely reflect particular motivational states or emotional responses of individuals to novel situations rather than cognitive differences (Sol et al., 2013). To what degree our measured single values of boldness and exploration are stable in time or more transient behaviours remains to be tested. In line with this conclusion, successful innovators also exhibited higher activity levels. Activity may reflect motivation to feed, which in other animals has been found to be a major determinant of innovation propensity (e.g. Sol et al., 2013). However, it may also increase the chance of solving the task accidentally by trial and error. However, closed environmental spaces can also be stressful and what we defined as ‘fast exploring’ could be a by-product of stereotyped stress behaviours. In any case, we found large variation in how bees approach and/or solve the task, meaning that there are probably individual differences in task solving.

The lack of support for the importance of associative learning does not mean that innovation does not require learning. Learning is not only necessary to fix the new behaviour in the individual repertoire (Ramsey et al., 2007; Lefebvre et al., 2004; Reader, 2003; Sol, 2003) but also important to solve the task itself. Indeed, we found that bees that succeeded in the innovation test went directly towards the lid covering the reward, probably reflecting that they had learnt the rewarding colour during training assays. In our assays, several individuals tested were able to associate colour and reward – after only two training trials – regardless of their differences in shyness, exploration and activity. Thus, the lack of effect of learning ability on innovation might reflect that most individuals were similarly proficient in associative learning. Testing other more sophisticated learning domains would be important to advance our knowledge in this direction.

Learning is widely held to have important advantages in the wild. In bees, learning is critically important for vital tasks such as foraging, identification of high-quality foraging sites, finding the right mixture of nectar and pollen, and navigating back to the nest for brood provisioning (Roulston and Goodell, 2011; Minckley et al., 2013). Surprisingly, we found intriguing sex-related differences in learning. Males showed a tendency to perform better in the associative learning test than females. This is unexpected because females have to deal with more tasks during their lifetime, including foraging and nest provisioning, and may perhaps indicate that the cognitive demands for males to locate females are higher than suspected. However, it is known that males and females can vary in problem solving in other taxa (Hopper et al., 2014; Lucon-Xiccato et al., 2020).

Our results suggest that solitary bees can accommodate their behaviour to novel contexts through innovative behaviours. In the context of global change, the ability to rapidly adapt behaviour to novel contexts seems highly relevant. In novel environments, bees must, for instance, solve how to forage on new plant species, which sometimes presents complex flowers with which bees have not co-evolved (Bartomeus et al., 2010). Therefore, we should abandon the notion that insect behaviour is inflexible and stereotypical, and better appreciate that insects can readily adjust their behaviour to changing conditions through innovation and learning.

We would like to thank Nanxiang Jin for his help with setting up the experimental device and the FU Berlin Institute of Biology for sharing their equipment.

Author contributions

Conceptualization: M.Á.C., I.B.; Methodology: M.Á.C., R.M., I.B.; Formal analysis: M.Á.C., I.B.; Investigation: M.Á.C.; Resources: R.M., D.S., I.B.; Data curation: M.Á.C.; Writing - original draft: M.Á.C.; Writing - review & editing: M.Á.C., R.M., D.S., I.B.; Visualization: M.Á.C.; Supervision: R.M., I.B.; Project administration: D.S.; Funding acquisition: D.S.

Funding

This research was supported by a grant from Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad (CGL2013-47448-P).

Alem
,
S.
,
Perry
,
C. J.
,
Zhu
,
X.
,
Loukola
,
O. J.
,
Ingraham
,
T.
,
Søvik
,
E.
and
Chittka
,
L.
(
2016
).
Associative Mechanisms Allow for Social Learning and Cultural Transmission of String Pulling in an Insect. PLoS Biology 14, e1002564.
Allasino
,
M. L.
,
Marrero
,
H. J.
,
Dorado
,
J.
and
Torretta
,
J. P.
(
2019
).
Scientific note: first global report of a bee nest built only with plastic
.
Apidologie
50
,
230
-
233
.
Avargues-Weber
,
A.
,
Dyer
,
A. G.
,
Combe
,
M.
and
Giurfa
,
M.
(
2012
).
Simultaneous mastering of two abstract concepts by the miniature brain of bees
.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA
109
,
7481
-
7486
.
Bartomeus
,
I.
,
Vilà
,
M.
and
Steffan-Dewenter
,
I.
(
2010
).
Combined effects of Impatiens glandulifera invasion and landscape structure on native plant pollination
.
J. Ecol.
98
,
440
-
450
.
Bosch
,
J.
(
1994
).
Osmia cornuta Latr. (Hym., Megachilidae) as a potential pollinator in almond orchards
.
J. Appl. Entomol.
117
,
151
-
157
.
Bosch
,
J.
and
Kemp
,
W.
(
2001
).
How to Manage the Blue Orchard Bee As an Orchard Pollinator
.
Sustainable Agriculture Network
.
Bouchard
,
J.
,
Goodyer
,
W.
and
Lefebvre
,
L.
(
2007
).
Social learning and innovation are positively correlated in pigeons (Columba livia)
.
Anim. Cogn.
10
,
259
-
266
.
Bürkner
,
P.-C.
(
2017
).
brms: an R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan
.
J. Stat. Softw.
80
.
Chittka
,
L.
and
Thomson
,
J. D.
(eds.). (
2009
).
Cognitive Ecology of Pollination, Cognitive Ecology of Pollination
.
Cambridge
:
Cambridge University Press
.
Cox
,
D. R.
and
Oakes
,
D.
(
2018
).
Analysis of Survival Data
.
Routledge
.
Crook
,
R. J.
(
2013
).
The welfare of invertebrate animals in research: can science's next generation improve their lot?
Postdoc J.
Dacke
,
M.
and
Srinivasan
,
M. V.
(
2008
).
Evidence for counting in insects
.
Anim. Cogn.
11
,
683
-
689
.
Degen
,
J.
,
Kirbach
,
A.
,
Reiter
,
L.
,
Lehmann
,
K.
,
Norton
,
P.
,
Storms
,
M.
,
Koblofsky
,
M.
,
Winter
,
S.
,
Georgieva
,
P. B.
,
Nguyen
,
H.
et al. 
(
2016
).
Honeybees learn landscape features during exploratory orientation flights
.
Curr. Biol.
26
,
2800
-
2804
.
Dukas
,
R.
(
2008
).
Evolutionary biology of insect learning
.
Annu. Rev. Entomol.
53
,
145
-
160
.
Gil
,
M.
,
De Marco
,
R. J.
,
Menzel
,
R.
(
2007
).
Learning reward expectations in honeybees
.
Learn. Mem.
14
,
491
-
496
.
Goulson
,
D.
,
Nicholls
,
E.
,
Botías
,
C.
and
Rotheray
,
E. L.
(
2015
).
Bee declines driven by combined Stress from parasites, pesticides, and lack of flowers
.
Science
347
,
1255957
.
Griffin
,
A. S.
,
Diquelou
,
M.
and
Perea
,
M.
(
2014
).
Innovative problem solving in birds: a key role of motor diversity
.
Anim. Behav.
92
,
221
-
227
.
Hempel de Ibarra
,
N.
,
Vorobyev
,
M.
and
Menzel
,
R.
(
2014
).
Mechanisms, functions and ecology of colour vision in the honeybee
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol.
200
,
411
-
433
.
Hopper
,
L. M.
,
Price
,
S. A.
,
Freeman
,
H. D.
,
Lambeth
,
S. P.
,
Schapiro
,
S. J.
and
Kendal
,
R. L.
(
2014
).
Influence of personality, age, sex, and estrous state on chimpanzee problem-solving success. Animal Cognition 17, 835-847.
Houston
,
A. I.
and
McNamara
,
J. M.
(
1999
).
Models of Adaptive Behaviour: An Approach Based on State
.
Cambridge University Press
.
Howard
,
S. R.
,
Avarguès-Weber
,
A.
,
Garcia
,
J. E.
,
Greentree
,
A. D.
and
Dyer
,
A. G.
(
2018
).
Numerical ordering of zero in honey bees
.
Science
360
,
1124
-
1126
.
Howard
,
S. R.
,
Avarguès-Weber
,
A.
,
Garcia
,
J. E.
,
Greentree
,
A. D.
and
Dyer
,
A. G.
(
2019
).
Numerical cognition in honeybees enables addition and subtraction
.
Sci. Adv.
5
,
eaav0961
.
Jin
,
N.
,
Landgraf
,
T.
,
Klein
,
S.
and
Menzel
,
R.
(
2014
).
Walking bumblebees memorize panorama and local cues in a laboratory test of navigation
.
Anim. Behav.
97
,
13
-
23
.
Jin
,
N.
,
Klein
,
S.
,
Leimig
,
F.
,
Bischoff
,
G.
and
Menzel
,
R.
(
2015
).
The neonicotinoid clothianidin interferes with navigation of the solitary bee Osmia cornuta in a laboratory test
.
J. Exp. Biol.
218
,
2821
-
2825
.
Lefebvre
,
L.
,
Reader
,
S. M.
and
Sol
,
D.
(
2004
).
Brains, innovations and evolution in birds and primates
.
Brain. Behav. Evol.
63
,
233
-
246
.
Loukola
,
O. J.
,
Perry
,
C. J.
,
Coscos
,
L.
and
Chittka
,
L.
(
2017
).
Bumblebees show cognitive flexibility by improving on an observed complex behavior
.
Science
355
,
833
-
836
.
Lucon-Xiccato
,
T.
,
Gatto
,
E.
and
Bisazza
,
A.
(
2019
).
Male and female guppies differ in problem-solving abilities. Current Zool. 66, 83-90.
Lunau
,
K.
(
1991
).
Innate flower recognition in bumblebees (Bombus terrestris, B. lucorum; Apidae): optical signals from stamens as landing reaction releasers
.
Ethology
88
,
203
-
214
.
MacIvor
,
J. S.
and
Moore
,
A. E.
(
2013
).
Bees collect polyurethane and polyethylene plastics as novel nest materials
.
Ecosphere
4
,
art155
.
Maeterlinck
,
M.
(
1901
).
The Life of the Bee [La Vie des Abeilles]
. Translated from French into English by Alfred Sutro.
Dodd, Mead and Company
.
Archipoche
.
Matthysen
,
E.
,
Van Overveld
,
T.
,
Van de Casteele
,
T.
and
Adriaensen
,
F.
(
2010
).
Family movements before independence influence natal dispersal in a territorial songbird
.
Oecologia
162
,
591
-
597
.
Menzel
,
R.
and
Giurfa
,
M.
(
2001
).
Cognitive architecture of a mini-brain: the honeybee
.
Trends Cogn. Sci.
5
,
62
-
71
.
Minckley
,
R. L.
,
Roulston
,
T. H.
and
Williams
,
N. M.
(
2013
).
Resource assurance predicts specialist and generalist bee activity in drought
.
Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.
280
,
20122703
.
Ollerton
,
J.
,
Winfree
,
R.
and
Tarrant
,
S.
(
2011
).
How many flowering plants are pollinated by animals?
Oikos
120
,
321
-
326
.
Ramsey
,
G.
,
Bastian
,
M. L.
and
van Schaik
,
C.
(
2007
).
Animal innovation defined and operationalized
.
Behav. Brain Sci.
30
,
393
-
407
.
Reader
,
S.
(
2003
).
Innovation and social learning: individual variation and brain evolution
.
Anim. Biol.
53
,
147
-
158
.
Reader
,
S. M.
,
Morand-Ferron
,
J.
and
Flynn
,
E.
(
2016
).
Animal and human innovation: novel problems and novel solutions
.
Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.
371
,
20150182
.
Renn
,
S. C. P.
,
Park
,
J. H.
,
Rosbash
,
M.
,
Hall
,
J. C.
and
Taghert
,
P. H.
(
1999
).
A pdf neuropeptide gene mutation and ablation of PDF neurons each cause severe abnormalities of behavioral circadian rhythms in Drosophila
.
Cell
99
,
791
-
802
.
Roulston
,
T. H.
and
Goodell
,
K.
(
2011
).
The role of resources and risks in regulating wild bee populations
.
Annu. Rev. Entomol.
56
,
293
-
312
.
Shearer
,
D. A.
and
Boch
,
R.
(
1966
).
Citral in the Nassanoff pheromone of the honey bee
.
J. Insect Physiol.
12
,
1513
-
1521
.
Sol
,
D.
(
2003
).
Behavioural flexibility: a neglected issue in the ecological and evolutionary literature
.
Anim. Innov.
,
63
-
82
.
Sol
,
D.
,
Duncan
,
R. P.
,
Blackburn
,
T. M.
,
Cassey
,
P.
and
Lefebvre
,
L.
(
2005
).
Big brains, enhanced cognition, and response of birds to novel environments
.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
102
,
5460
-
5465
.
Sol
,
D.
,
Griffin
,
A. S.
,
Bartomeus
,
I.
and
Boyce
,
H.
(
2011
).
Exploring or avoiding novel food resources? The novelty conflict in an invasive bird
.
PLoS ONE
6
,
e19535
.
Sol
,
D.
,
Maspons
,
J.
,
Vall-llosera
,
M.
,
Bartomeus
,
I.
,
García-Peña
,
G. E.
,
Piñol
,
J.
and
Freckleton
,
R. P.
(
2012
).
Unraveling the life history of successful invaders. Science 337, 580-583.
Sol
,
D.
,
Lapiedra
,
O.
and
González-Lagos
,
C.
(
2013
).
Behavioural adjustments for a life in the city
.
Anim. Behav.
85
,
1101
-
1112
.
Vorobyev
,
M.
and
Menzel
,
R.
(
1999
).
Flower advertisement for insects: bees, a case study
. In
Adaptive Mechanisms in the Ecology of Vision
(S. N. Archer, M. B. A. Djamgoz, E. R. Loew, J. C. Partridge and S. Vallerga), pp.
537
-
553
.
Dordrecht
:
Springer
.
Zhang
,
Y.
,
Lu
,
H.
and
Bargmann
,
C. I.
(
2005
).
Pathogenic bacteria induce aversive olfactory learning in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature 438, 179-184.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

Supplementary information