ABSTRACT
The circadian clock is an internal timekeeping system shared by most organisms, and knowledge about its functional importance and evolution in natural environments is still needed. Here, we investigated the circadian clock of wild-caught threespine sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) at the behavioural and molecular levels. Although their behaviour, ecology and evolution are well studied, information on their circadian rhythms are scarce. We quantified the daily locomotor activity rhythm under a light:dark cycle (LD) and under constant darkness (DD). Under LD, all fish exhibited significant daily rhythmicity, while under DD, only 18% of individuals remained rhythmic. This interindividual variation suggests that the circadian clock controls activity only in certain individuals. Moreover, under LD, some fish were almost exclusively nocturnal, while others were active around the clock. Furthermore, the most nocturnal fish were also the least active. These results suggest that light masks activity (i.e. suppresses activity without entraining the internal clock) more strongly in some individuals than others. Finally, we quantified the expression of five clock genes in the brain of sticklebacks under DD using qPCR. We did not detect circadian rhythmicity, which could indicate either that the clock molecular oscillator is highly light-dependent, or that there was an oscillation but that we were unable to detect it. Overall, our study suggests that a strong circadian control on behavioural rhythms may not necessarily be advantageous in a natural population of sticklebacks and that the daily phase of activity varies greatly between individuals because of a differential masking effect of light.
INTRODUCTION
Many behaviours and physiological processes in living organisms exhibit daily rhythmicity, for example, locomotor and feeding activity, hormone secretion and metabolism (Refinetti, 2016). Some of these rhythms persist in the absence of external cues, because they are driven by an endogenous mechanism called the circadian clock (Kumar, 2017). Found in almost all life forms, this internal clock usually has an intrinsic period of approximately 24 h and is entrained by temporal signals such as the light:dark cycle, so that the phase of circadian rhythms is synchronized with relevant environmental variables (e.g. being awake when feeding or mating opportunities are present). The circadian clock thus allows the anticipation of daily environmental changes and the coordination of biological functions, and can have fitness consequences (Vaze and Sharma, 2013; Dominoni et al., 2017). The heart of the circadian clock is a cell-autonomous molecular oscillator made up of a transcription–translation feedback loop that involves positive and negative elements (Bell-Pedersen et al., 2005). In mammals, BMAL and CLOCK are positive elements that induce the transcription of period (per) and cryptochrome (cry). PER and CRY are negative elements that inhibit their own transcription by downregulating the activity of BMAL and CLOCK (Rosensweig and Green, 2020). Generally speaking, the expression level of bmal and clock is in antiphase with that of per and cry (Takahashi, 2017). These four clock genes are highly conserved in animals, but, because of genome duplication events, several of them retain two paralogous copies in the different vertebrate lineages (Bell-Pedersen et al., 2005).
In the last decade, our knowledge of the organization and functioning of circadian rhythms in animals has expanded with the study of various wild species, building on the work mostly acquired in laboratory settings with model organisms (Mus musculus, Danio rerio and Drosophila melanogaster) (Kronfeld-Schor et al., 2013; Schwartz et al., 2017). This growing body of research shows that the implication of the circadian clock in driving biological rhythms can vary greatly depending on a species’ biology (reviewed in Bloch et al., 2013; Hazlerigg and Tyler, 2019) and that the opportunities, threats and challenges that organisms face in natural environments can influence their circadian rhythms (Hut et al., 2012; Helm et al., 2017). For example, some species adjust the phase of their circadian activity rhythm in response to light intensity (Chiesa et al., 2010), food availability (Lopez-Olmeda et al., 2010; Ware et al., 2012), predation risk (Pellman et al., 2015) and social interactions (Fuchikawa et al., 2016). In order to improve our understanding of the functional importance of the circadian clock in nature (i.e. the benefits it provides to an individual in a given environment) and which selection pressures can shape the evolution of circadian rhythms, we must continue to investigate a diversity of species that have evolved in various ecological contexts and that are amenable to experimental and physiological studies (Kronfeld-Schor et al., 2013; Schwartz et al., 2017).
Owing to their species diversity and the fact that they are adapted to a wide variety of habitats, fish offer an exceptional opportunity to broaden our knowledge of the impact of the circadian clock and of the environment in driving biological rhythms. Knowledge about the molecular oscillator and the clock organization in fish comes mainly from zebrafish studies (reviewed in Vatine et al., 2011; Frøland Steindal and Whitmore, 2019). At the behavioural level, daily and circadian rhythms have been studied in several fish species and have often been described as more plastic than that of mammals (reviewed in Reebs, 2002). While it is generally easy to label mammal species as diurnal, nocturnal or crepuscular (but there are exceptions: Hut et al., 2012), it is often less straightforward in fish. For example, under a light:dark cycle, some Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus) were diurnal, others were nocturnal, and some were active around the clock (Vera et al., 2009). Similar interindividual variation has been reported in goldfish (Carassius auratus; Iigo and Tabata, 1996) and in Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar; Richardson and McCleave, 1974). Moreover, in mammals, robust circadian rhythmicity under constant darkness or light is considered the norm (although there are exceptions: Bloch et al., 2013; Hazlerigg and Tyler, 2019). Although strong circadian rhythms have also been described in fish (Kavaliers, 1980; Ooka-Souda and Kabasawa, 1995; Iigo and Tabata, 1997), the existence of arrhythmic individuals has been reported in several populations under constant conditions (Iigo and Tabata, 1996; Hurd et al., 1998; Herrero et al., 2003; Vera et al., 2006, 2009). That being said, most chronobiological studies in fish have been performed with populations that have lived in artificial conditions for several generations (either for aquaculture or for laboratory experiments). As the ecological context in which the species evolved is rarely considered, it is not clear whether the plasticity of daily and circadian behaviours has an adaptive value in the environment. Furthermore, the fish molecular oscillator has almost never been studied in wild populations (but see Beale et al., 2013), limiting our understanding of the evolution of clock mechanisms in response to environmental pressures.
The threespine stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) is well studied in ecology and evolution (McKinnon et al., 2019). This small fish is also well suited to answer questions about the ecological and evolutionary implications of the circadian clock through the study of its natural phenotypic variation, which can be combined with experimental work. Stickleback ecotypes are found in diverse habitats (marine waters, salt marshes, streams, rivers, lakes, etc.) and display morphological, physiological and behavioural adaptations to these environments (Bell and Foster, 1994; Ostlund-Nilsson et al., 2007; Kitano et al., 2010; Di-Poi et al., 2014; Di Poi et al., 2016; Ishikawa et al., 2019). Many of the environmental pressures that differ between ecotypes, such as the presence of predators and parasites, prey availability, light intensity and social interactions (Ostlund-Nilsson et al., 2007), have the potential to influence circadian rhythms (Helm et al., 2017). This could be achieved either through selective pressure resulting in genetic divergence, or through phenotypic plasticity, i.e. the effects of the environment on the development of a phenotype, here, the circadian rhythm itself. As sticklebacks are also known for their interindividual variation in behaviour, called personality (activity, boldness, sociality, etc.; Huntingford, 1976; Bell, 2005; Wark et al., 2011), it is also possible that they exhibit interindividual variation in circadian rhythms. So far, it has been suggested that circadian molecular mechanisms may vary between ecotypes similarly to traits at other levels of biological organization, although the functional impact of this difference is not known. For example, using common garden-raised sticklebacks from two lake-stream pairs, a previous study reported that a gene that is part of the molecular oscillator (cry1ab) was upregulated in the liver of stream sticklebacks compared with lake sticklebacks (Hanson et al., 2017). Studying circadian rhythms in sticklebacks will help us to better understand the functional importance and the evolution of the circadian clock in natural environments.
In comparison to what is known about the ecology and evolution of sticklebacks, very little knowledge is available on their circadian rhythms and clock. In fact, the existence of a circadian clock has never been demonstrated in this species. At the behavioural level, the circadian rhythms of sticklebacks have been studied once under constant light conditions. This study showed that the frequency with which males visited their nests (in the hope of finding eggs deposited by a female) did not display circadian rhythmicity in constant light (Sevenster et al., 1995). Regarding the daily activity rhythm (i.e. under a light:dark cycle), some evidence suggests that sticklebacks are diurnal. For instance, stickleback visual opsins (Rennison et al., 2012) correspond to those of diurnal fish (Carleton et al., 2020). Moreover, previous studies reported that sticklebacks were mostly captured during the day in the wild (Worgan and FitzGerald, 1981; Sjoberg, 1985; Reebs et al., 1995). In contrast, night activity (Reebs et al., 1984; Quinn et al., 2012) and night feeding (Mussen and Peeke, 2001) have been observed in some sticklebacks. At the physiological level, we know that melatonin levels (a hormone that plays a key role in the regulation of circadian rhythms) are higher during the night than during the day in sticklebacks (Mayer et al., 1997; Kulczykowska et al., 2017; Pomianowski et al., 2020) as in most vertebrates (Challet, 2007; Falcón et al., 2009), but we do not know whether this rhythm is driven by the clock or solely by light (Falcón et al., 2009). At the molecular level, time-of-day variation in the expression of per1b and clock1b has been observed in the liver of sticklebacks, but as this was measured under a light:dark cycle, we do not know whether this rhythm is self-sustained (Prokkola et al., 2015).
In this study, using wild-caught threespine sticklebacks, we investigated the circadian clock of this species at the behavioural and molecular levels. Our first objective was to determine whether the daily rhythm of locomotor activity is under circadian clock control, and we hypothesized that it is indeed the case. Our prediction was that sticklebacks would show a significant rhythm of locomotor activity under constant darkness (DD). Our second objective was to determine the phase of activity of sticklebacks under LD. We hypothesized that sticklebacks are diurnal. Our prediction was that the daily activity would be mainly performed during the light phase. Our third objective was to quantify the molecular oscillation of five clock genes (bmal1a, clock1b, clock2, per1b and cry1b) in the brain, an organ that is potentially implicated in the control of circadian rhythms. We hypothesized that clock gene expression shows circadian rhythmicity under DD. Our prediction was that the expression level of bmal1a, clock1b and clock2 would be in antiphase with that of per1b and cry1b.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Fish sampling and housing
All work was carried out in compliance with Animal Care and Use Guidelines, under a permit of the Comité de Protection des Animaux de l'Université Laval (CPAUL, permit 2018066-2). We collected threespine sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus Linnaeus 1758) from the wild population of lac Témiscouata (47°48′37.1″N 68°51′56.6″W, Québec, Canada) in June 2019. We did not have specific information on the daily activity patterns of this species in lac Témiscouata. We thus sampled fish with a beach seine so that we could collect all individuals in the water column no matter if they were resting at the bottom of the lake or swimming at the surface. We sampled fish in the morning (around 08:00), in the afternoon (around 15:00) and in the evening (around 19:00) to account for the possibility that some individuals migrate daily between different parts of the lake. After they were captured, sticklebacks were kept in coolers containing water from the lake. Oxygenation was ensured by aquarium air pumps. Sticklebacks were brought back to the Laboratoire de Recherches en Sciences Environnementales et Médicales (LARSEM) at Université Laval (Québec, Canada). In the animal facility, fish were held in two 1000 l water tanks (n=140 per tank) and were fed brine shrimp and nutritious flakes twice a day (morning and late afternoon). They were exposed to non-breeding environmental conditions, a water temperature of 14±0.4°C and a 12 h:12 h light:dark cycle with lights on at 06:00 and lights off at 18:00. Fish were allowed to acclimate to the laboratory conditions for 11 weeks before the start of the experiments.
Activity monitoring system
To monitor locomotor activity, 18 fish were transferred to an adjacent room and individually placed in 2 l experimental tanks. A white plexiglass separated each tank to prevent fish from seeing each other. Lightning was provided by three full-spectrum LED light bars (Plant 3.0, Fluval) mounted above the tanks. Illuminance was measured by a lux meter (LX1330B, Dr.meter) and was around 500 lux at the water surface. We chose this illuminance value based on previous studies in other fish species (Iigo and Tabata, 1996; Whitmore et al., 2000; Bayarri et al., 2004; Lopez-Olmeda et al., 2010). A dark plastic curtain was hanging in front of the tanks to ensure a constant illumination (or darkness) when we needed to enter the room for maintenance.
Each experimental tank was equipped with an infrared photoelectric sensor (E3Z-D67, Omron) placed in the lower third of the front wall (Fig. S1). We had previously established that this position was optimal to record stickleback movements (Fig. S2). Every time a fish interrupted the infrared light beam that was emitted by the sensor, an output signal was sent to a controller (ILC 131 ETH, Phoenix Contact). Each interruption was counted as one movement (programming of the controller was carried out using PC Worx software from Phoenix Contact). Data were retrieved by connecting a computer to the controller.
Experimental design
Because we could monitor 18 fish at a time, we divided individuals into three groups (Fig. 1). Individuals were allowed to acclimate to the experimental tanks for at least 3 days before the start of the experiment. For all three groups, food was provided by hand once a day at a random time within the 24-h period [previously determined using the RAND() function in Excel software]. We used a dim red light when food was provided during the dark phase.
Locomotor activity
Group 1 was exposed to a 12 h:12 h light:dark (LD) cycle for 8 days (lights on at 06:00 and lights off at 18:00) followed by 10 days of constant darkness (DD). Group 1 was used to quantify locomotor activity under LD and DD. Groups 2 and 3 were also exposed to LD for 8 days and used to quantify locomotor activity under LD. We chose to record for 8 days in LD and 10 days in DD because previous studies in fish used a wide array of number of days of locomotor activity recording, ranging from 2–3 days (Beale et al., 2013; Tudorache et al., 2018) to 1–2 weeks (Iigo and Tabata, 1996, 1997; Hurd et al., 1998; Herrero et al., 2003), and even up to 1 month (Kavaliers, 1981; Cavallari et al., 2011). As authors do not usually explain their choice, we decided on average values. Moreover, we decided to record activity for two more days in DD than in LD to ensure that we would not miss a rhythm in DD.
Clock gene expression
Groups 2 and 3 were also used to quantify clock gene expression rhythms. To do so, on the ninth day of the experiment with groups 2 and 3, lights were not turned on at 06:00 so all fish were exposed to DD for at least 24 h. As the molecular oscillator is entrained by light, this 24-h period of DD is necessary to quantify the true internal clock rhythm, i.e. the rhythm without light entrainment. On the tenth day, we sampled the brain and the caudal fin of four randomly selected individuals every 6 h throughout a 24-h cycle (06:00, 12:00, 18:00, 00:00; see Fig. 1). Sampling of individuals at multiple times over a 24-h period is typical of clock gene expression studies in fish (e.g. Feliciano et al., 2011; Prokkola et al., 2015; Tudorache et al., 2018). Sticklebacks were euthanized with an overdose of MS-222 (250 mg l−1). Tissue collection was performed in darkness with the help of a dim red light and took less than 3 min per fish. After dissection, brains and caudal fins were immediately stabilized in RNAlater (Ambion) and stored at −20°C. We used caudal fins to determine sex with the IDH genetic sex marker (Peichel et al., 2004).
Choice of genes
We chose to quantify the expression of bmal1a, clock1b, clock2, per1b and cry1b, although sticklebacks have several other clock genes (Table 1). We chose these five genes for three reasons. First, we wanted to quantify positive (bmal, clock) and negative (per, cry) elements to have an overall view of the transcription-translation feedback loop. Second, we chose genes that have an ortholog in the zebrafish to compare our results with what is known from this model organism (Table 1). Third, we avoided quantifying per2a and cry1aa because these two genes are mainly light-induced (in opposition to being clock controlled) in the zebrafish (Pando et al., 2001; Tamai et al., 2007; Vatine et al., 2009), so their expression rhythm rapidly loses its amplitude under DD (e.g. Beale et al., 2013) and thus would not be informative in our study in DD.
Gene expression in the brain
We studied clock gene expression using a quantitative real-time PCR (qPCR) approach. We extracted total RNA in the brain of sticklebacks and performed a DNase digestion using the miRNeasy Mini Kit (Qiagen) combined with the RNase-Free DNase Set (Qiagen). We stored RNA at −70°C. We quantified RNA using the Quant-iT RiboGreen RNA Assay Kit (Invitrogen) and assessed RNA quality and integrity with the RNA 6000 Nano Kit (Agilent). All samples showed RNA integrity numbers (RIN) greater than 9.0. For all samples we reverse-transcribed 10 µl of RNA at 100 ng µl−1 with 4 µl of the 5X qScript cDNA SuperMix (Quantabio) and 6 µl of RNase-free water in a final volume of 20 µl. Following the manufacturer's protocol, thermocycling parameters were 25°C for 5 min, 42°C for 30 min and 85°C for 5 min.
We obtained cDNA sequences of bmal1a, clock1b, clock2, per1b and cry1b from the Ensembl Genome Browser (version 98) and designed primers using Primer3 (Table 2). We performed an in silico specificity screen with the Amplify4 software to ensure that primers for a given gene were not amplifying any paralogs. We also verified specificity of primers and absence of primer dimers with melting curves (60–95°C). To further guarantee that the primers were amplifying the targeted genes, we analyzed amplicons by Sanger sequencing. We assessed PCR amplification efficiency of each primer pair with a qPCR experiment using a four- or five-point standard curve made of a fivefold dilution series of pooled cDNA samples. Efficiency is reported in Table 2.
We performed qPCR experiments in the 7500 Fast Real-Time PCR System (Applied Biosystems) using 5 µl of cDNA at 1 ng µl−1, 10 µl of the 2X PerfeCTa SYBR Green FastMix (Quantabio), 1 µl of primer pairs at 10 µmol l−1 (final concentration of 250 nmol l−1 for each primer) and 4 µl of nuclease-free water for a total volume of 20 µl. All samples were run in triplicate on a single 96-well plate for a given gene. We included no-template and no-reverse transcription controls. The thermocycling protocol was 95°C for 3 min (initial denaturation), followed by 40 cycles of 15 s at 95°C (denaturation) and 45 s at 60°C (annealing).
We used the NormFinder software (Andersen et al., 2004) to identify the optimal reference gene (or combination of reference genes) for our experiment between ubc, hprt1, rpl13a, gapdh and β-actin (Table 3). We performed the analysis on 12 cDNA samples that were previously obtained in the same conditions as experimental samples during a pilot study. The NormFinder algorithm identified ubc as the most stable gene between time points. We thus calculated the relative expression of target genes using the ΔΔCq method adjusted for efficiency of each primer pairs (Pfaffl, 2001) with ubc as the reference gene.
Data analysis
Locomotor activity rhythm
Of the 54 individuals that we used in our experiments, six were discarded from analyses because they died during experiments (n=3) or were parasitized (n=3). For the 48 remaining individuals, we gathered locomotor activity data (number of movements) in 10-min bins for analysis purposes. Actograms, activity profiles and χ2 periodograms were produced using the ActogramJ plugin in ImageJ (Schmid et al., 2011) for each fish under LD (n=48) and under DD (n=17). The χ2 periodogram analysis calculates Qp values for multiple periods within a fixed range. The period with the highest Qp value corresponds to the estimated period of the rhythm. Because Qp has a probability distribution of χ2 (with a P−1 degree of freedom, where P is the period), we can determine whether the Qp value for the estimated period is significant with α=0.05 (Sokolove and Bushell, 1978). In other words, the periodogram analysis lets us know whether the rhythm is significant and, if so, what is the period of this rhythm. We first performed the periodogram analysis using periods ranging between 0 and 32 h, but we did not find any significant ultradian endogenous rhythms (i.e. rhythms with periods shorter than circadian rhythms). Thus, we show periodograms with periods ranging between 16 and 32 h to facilitate visualization. We performed all other statistical analyses using R software version 4.0.1 (https://www.r-project.org/). When needed, we evaluated the normal distribution of data using Q–Q plots and the Shapiro–Wilk test, and we verified homogeneity of variances using Levene's test.
Masking effect of light
We evaluated the difference in the average activity level (movements per 10 min) during the light phase in LD and the subjective light phase in DD using a paired t-test. This comparison allows us to assess the importance of the masking effect of light, which can suppress or enhance activity without entraining the internal clock (Mrosovsky, 1999). We also verified whether the difference in the average activity level during the light phase in LD and the subjective light phase in DD differs between sexes using a t-test.
Phase of activity
Although our hypothesis was that sticklebacks are diurnal, we observed a tendency towards nocturnality under LD. Thus, to quantify the phase of activity in each fish, we calculated the percentage of the total daily activity performed during the dark phase (also referred to as the night activity). By assessing the night activity, we observed large interindividual variation for the phase of activity, but we also noticed large interindividual variation in total daily activity. We thus assessed sex differences in night activity and in total daily activity using t-tests and we evaluated the correlation between these two variables using Pearson's correlation test. Data are represented as means±s.e.m.
Clock gene expression rhythms in the brain
Among the six individuals that were discarded from the analysis, five were from groups 2 and 3, so there were 31 individuals left for the brain gene expression analysis. We thus sampled eight individuals at 06:00, 18:00 and 00:00 (n=8) and seven individuals at 12:00 (n=7). Moreover, one individual was removed from the 18:00 time point for clock2 because it was identified as an extreme outlier using the identify_outliers() function from the rstatix package in R (https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=rstatix). We evaluated differences in relative gene expression between time points using one-way ANOVA. Relative gene expression was also subjected to cosinor analysis using the cosinor2 package (https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=cosinor2). The cosinor analysis fits a cosine function with a known period (24 h) to the expression values so that we can estimate the amplitude, the acrophase (peak time) and the mesor (mean of all expression values) of the rhythm (Refinetti et al., 2007). This procedure also calculates the probability that the amplitude is significantly different from zero using the F-distribution. When the P-value is <0.05, we can consider that gene expression shows significant circadian rhythmicity.
RESULTS
Locomotor activity rhythm
Under a 12 h:12 h light:dark cycle (LD), a significant daily rhythmicity of 24.0 h (χ2 periodogram analysis, P<0.05) was observed for all fish (see Fig. 2 for data from representative individuals). Under constant darkness (DD), most individuals were arrhythmic (Fig. 3A,C,E) and only three out of 17 sticklebacks (18%) showed significant circadian rhythmicity (χ2 periodogram analysis, P<0.05; Fig. 3B,D,F) with periods of 24.8, 25.0 and 26.3 h.
Masking effect of light
The average activity level (movements per 10 min) was significantly lower during the light phase in LD than during the subjective light phase in DD (paired t-test, P<0.001, n=17; Fig. S3). The difference in the average activity level during the light phase in LD and the subjective light phase in DD was not significantly different between males (n=7) and females (n=10) (t-test, P=0.3).
Phase of activity
Under LD, a few sticklebacks showed a well-defined phase of activity and were almost strictly nocturnal (Fig. 2A,D). However, most individuals displayed an unclear phase of activity and were just slightly more active during the night than during the day (Fig. 2B,C,E,F). On average, sticklebacks displayed 61.8±1.3% (n=48) of their daily activity during the dark phase. There was interindividual variation in the phase of activity, as measured by the percentage of the total daily activity displayed during the dark phase (also referred as the night activity; Fig. 4A), with individuals spending 46.5% to 87.5% of their active time at night. Of note, the three fish that were rhythmic in DD (described above) were not among the most nocturnal fish, as they displayed on average 53.0%, 52.9% and 57.0% of their daily activity during the night under LD. There was no significant difference between males (n=22) and females (n=26) in night activity (t-test, P=0.3). Under LD, sticklebacks also showed large interindividual variation in the total daily activity, ranging from around 550 to 2750 movements day−1 (Fig. 4B). Males (1655±99 movements day−1, n=22) were significantly more active than females (1357±95 movements day−1, n=26) (t-test, P=0.04). There was also a significant negative correlation between night activity and total daily activity (Pearson's correlation test, r=−0.3, P=0.04, n=48) so that the most nocturnal fish were also the least active (Fig. 4C).
Clock gene expression rhythms in the brain
We did not find any significant time-of-day variation in the relative expression of bmal1a, clock1b, clock2, per1b and cry1b in the brain of sticklebacks (one-way ANOVA, P>0.05, Fig. 5). In addition, the cosinor analysis did not detect any significant circadian rhythmicity in the relative expression of the five genes (P>0.05).
DISCUSSION
The circadian clock is an internal timekeeping system shared by almost all living organisms and has been mostly studied in model organisms. Although knowledge about the functional importance and the evolution of circadian rhythms in natural environments is mounting, combining studies at the behavioural and molecular levels in individuals from natural populations but in controlled experimental settings is still in its early phase. In this study, using wild-caught sticklebacks, we investigated the circadian clock of this species at the behavioural and molecular levels. Our first objective was to determine whether the daily rhythm of locomotor activity is under circadian clock control using a manipulation of the photoperiod. Under LD, all fish exhibited significant daily rhythmicity, whereas under DD, only a few individuals remained rhythmic. This result indicates that the circadian clock controls the locomotor activity rhythm in only a few sticklebacks, revealing a noteworthy interindividual variation. Our second objective was to determine the phase of activity of sticklebacks under LD. Contrary to our hypothesis, sticklebacks were mostly nocturnal. However, we observed again large interindividual variation: some fish were almost exclusively nocturnal while others were just slightly more active during the night than during the day. This variation was negatively correlated with the total daily activity, meaning that the most nocturnal fish were also the least active. This result suggests that light suppresses activity more strongly in some individuals, making them the most nocturnal fish. Our third objective was to describe the molecular oscillation of five clock genes (bmal1a, clock1b, clock2, per1b and cry1b) in the brain of sticklebacks under DD. Contrary to our hypothesis, we reported a lack of circadian rhythmicity for the five genes in the brain, which could indicate either that clock gene expression is not endogenously controlled, or that there was a significant oscillation but that we were unable to detect it, as a result of the large biological variation observed among individuals or because of technical issues.
Locomotor activity rhythm under constant darkness
We found striking interindividual variation in circadian rhythms of activity in threespine sticklebacks. Our finding that not all individuals display a significant circadian rhythm of locomotor activity has been reported previously in other fish species under constant conditions (30% to 57% of individuals were rhythmic depending on the species studied; Iigo and Tabata, 1996; Herrero et al., 2003; Vera et al., 2006, 2009). In our experiment, 18% of sticklebacks were rhythmic in constant darkness. Thus, a lack of circadian control on the locomotor activity rhythm seems common in fish, although strong circadian rhythmicity for all individuals has also been reported in some species (Kavaliers, 1980; Ooka-Souda and Kabasawa, 1995; Iigo and Tabata, 1997). An advantage of not being under the strict control of the circadian clock could be that it allows the fish to rapidly adjust their phase of activity when critical changes occur in the environment, such as a shift in food availability, predation risk, mating opportunities, presence of parasites, etc. This is demonstrated by jet lag in animals that are strongly influenced by their internal clock, such as humans: it takes several days to adjust the phase of activity to a new environment and this re-entrainment is associated with many negative effects on health and cognitive performance (Waterhouse et al., 2007). Thus, in fish populations facing a particularly fluctuating environment, the individuals may benefit from being flexible and able to adjust their phase of activity, rather than their activity being rigidly controlled by their internal timekeeping system. For instance, the stickleback population in lac Témiscouata has to cope with several aquatic and avian predators (Reimchen, 1994; Tessier et al., 2008). All these fish and birds likely forage at various moments during the day and might themselves change their phase of activity according to various environmental factors or throughout the year. Sticklebacks thus probably must deal with many conflictual – and sometimes unpredictable – daily patterns in predation risk. Indeed, the lac Témiscouata population shows strong anti-predator morphology and behaviour, even when laboratory reared (Lacasse and Aubin-Horth, 2012). Having a flexible daily schedule could further help sticklebacks to deal with several types of predators. In contrast, the fact that some individuals kept an activity rhythm in constant darkness highlights that the extensive interindividual variation seen in many traits in sticklebacks, such as personality (Huntingford, 1976; Bell, 2005; Aubin-Horth et al., 2012), is also present in their circadian rhythms. Whether the variation quantified in these wild individuals arises from genetic variation or developmental plasticity in their early environment will need to be tested using common-environment experiments (Greenwood et al., 2011; Di-Poi et al., 2014). This interindividual variation suggests the hypothesis that there is more than one successful way to regulate daily activities in that environment.
For the majority of the individuals that were not rhythmic in constant darkness, a lack of circadian regulation does not mean that they do not have a functional clock. It is possible that the clock molecular oscillator is partially uncoupled from the effectors, e.g. the locomotor system. For instance, uncoupling between clock gene expression rhythm and behavioural rhythm has been reported in the Mexican cavefish (Astyanax mexicanus; Beale et al., 2013). Similarly, the neuronal activity in the suprachiasmatic nucleus (the clock master oscillator in mammals) of guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus) shows robust circadian rhythmicity, but the animals express very unclear and weak activity rhythms (Kurumiya and Kawamura, 1988). It is thus possible that daily activities are not regulated by the clock molecular oscillator in sticklebacks as well. If this is the case, other behaviours, or physiological processes – such as the daily variation in melatonin level – would be expected to be controlled by the circadian clock. It is also possible that other environmental factors entrain the circadian clock of sticklebacks. For instance, food availability was shown to entrain circadian locomotor activity rhythms in goldfish (Carassius auratus; Sánchez-Vázquez et al., 1997), tench (Tinca tinca; Herrero et al., 2005) and zebrafish (Danio rerio; Lopez-Olmeda et al., 2010). In our study, sticklebacks could only be entrained by the light:dark cycle because they were fed at random times and all other environmental cues were held constant. In future studies, asking whether other environmental factors can entrain circadian rhythms in sticklebacks would help us to understand what temporal cues are important for these fish in their natural environment. Alternatively, the photoperiodic conditions we used might have been inadequate and it could have led us to mistakenly think that the light:dark cycle could entrain the circadian clock in only a few sticklebacks. For instance, under LD, transitions between the light and the dark phases were very sudden, which is obviously not the case in nature as the sun sets and rises progressively. The sharp increase in activity observed every day just after the lights were turned off might indicate that this event was stressful for the fish. In future experiments, using a light gradient at sunrise and at sunset could help to better reproduce natural conditions (e.g. Lazado et al., 2014).
Masking effect of light and phase of activity under the light:dark cycle
Having established that the locomotor activity rhythm of sticklebacks is not controlled by the internal clock in most individuals, our results suggest that the masking effect of light contributes to the significant daily rhythm that we observed for all fish under LD. The masking effect of light refers to the direct influence of the photic signal on an organism's behaviour, that is to say without the entrainment of its internal clock (Mrosovsky, 1999). As sticklebacks were generally more nocturnal under LD, the masking effect of light should suppress activity in this species (Mrosovsky, 1999). This is exactly what we observed: sticklebacks were less active during the light phase in LD than during the subjective light phase in DD (same hours of the day but different lighting conditions). This result indicates that light suppresses activity in sticklebacks, the definition of a masking effect.
We had hypothesized that sticklebacks are diurnal based on the fact that their visual opsins (Rennison et al., 2012) correspond to those of diurnal fish (Carleton et al., 2020) and that they are mostly captured during the day in the wild (Worgan and FitzGerald, 1981; Sjoberg, 1985; Reebs et al., 1995). However, we found that sticklebacks were, on average, mostly nocturnal under LD. The fact that some fish were almost strictly nocturnal suggests that sticklebacks can find food at night, using either visual or chemical cues (which has already been suggested by Mussen and Peeke, 2001). Whether sticklebacks do perform night activity in the wild is not known and might depend on several factors. In the laboratory, some sticklebacks might have chosen to be active during the night because they did not need to extensively rely on their visual system to find food (as their tank was quite small) and because they perceived the dark phase as safer. It has been previously shown in some species that there can be differences between the phase of daily activity in the laboratory and in the natural environment (reviewed in Calisi and Bentley, 2009). For instance, while mice (Mus musculus) are known for their nocturnal behaviour in the laboratory, they show variable phases of activity and are sometimes even exclusively diurnal when they are held in a semi-natural environment (Daan et al., 2011). Therefore, it is possible that sticklebacks are nocturnal in the laboratory and diurnal in their natural environment, and this could be verified using acoustic telemetry (March et al., 2010; Hussey et al., 2015; Alós et al., 2017). It should also be noted that we did not provide shelters in the experimental tanks, but previous studies reported that the presence of shelters could influence the daily activity rhythm of fish, mostly by reducing activity during the light phase and increasing activity during the dark phase (Harden Jones, 1956; Orpwood et al., 2010; Larranaga and Steingrímsson, 2015). As sticklebacks were already mostly nocturnal without shelter, it would be interesting to test whether adding shelters would make them even more nocturnal.
Although consistent patterns of activity among individuals from the same population exist in fish (Helfman, 1986), large interindividual variation in the phase of daily activity has been reported in several fish species (reviewed in Reebs, 2002). In accordance with these observations in other species, we showed that some sticklebacks were clearly nocturnal under LD, while others displayed an unclear phase of activity and were just slightly more active during the night than during the day. We also observed that sticklebacks that restricted their daily activity to the dark phase were also the least active. The masking effect of light could thus be involved: some fish were more nocturnal because light suppressed their activity more strongly than that of the other fish. Observation of less active individuals in wild populations has already been reported in other fish species (Slavík and Horký, 2012; Závorka et al., 2016; Alós et al., 2017). Moreover, in accordance with our results, it has been shown that the less active fish react more to variations in light intensity than the more active individuals in wild brown trout juveniles (Salmo trutta) (Závorka et al., 2016). The ultimate cause of this interindividual variation is not known, but it could be that some fish have less energy to invest in activity and need to optimize the timing of their daily activity. They would thus benefit from being strongly affected by the light signal because it would allow them to only be active at the most optimal time of the day, which seems to be during the night for sticklebacks in our experiment. Finally, we must also consider the fact that our activity measure may be affected by the photoelectric sensors used, which only covered a portion of the tanks, even though we tested the optimal vertical sensor position. Thigmotaxis (or ‘wall-hugging’) refers to a centrophobic, stress-related behaviour found in fish as well as in mammals (Maximino et al., 2010). Thigmotaxis could not be assessed in our experiment because we did not use horizontal sensors. If some individuals were more anxious than others in our study, they might have swum very close to the wall of their tank and been less detected by the sensor, for example during the day. Therefore, the fish that we detected as less active and more nocturnal might have been as active as the other fish around the clock, but spending more time swimming close to the wall during the day. In future experiments, this bias could be avoided by using more than one photoelectric sensor on each tank.
Under LD, we also observed a significant sex difference in the total daily activity: males were more active than females. We reported that light did not suppress activity differently between sexes, so the masking effect of light is not the cause. One potential explanation is that males invest more energy in their daily activities because they have a higher energetic demand (Chmura et al., 2020) and forage more than females to find food in their tank. Another potential explanation is that if males were less anxious than females in our study, our activity monitoring system might have detected them more (as explained above). Lower anxiety levels in males than in females have been reported, for instance, in humans (Donner and Lowry, 2013) and in fish (Fontana et al., 2020). In summary, our results suggest that circadian and daily locomotor activity rhythms display large interindividual variation in sticklebacks, which seems to be a common feature in fish (Reebs, 2002). As mammals tend to exhibit more robust circadian behavioural rhythms (although there are exceptions: Bloch et al., 2013; Hazlerigg and Tyler, 2019), our study highlights the importance of investigating a wide diversity of species to better understand the evolution of circadian clocks.
Clock gene expression rhythms in the brain
We did not detect any significant circadian rhythmicity in the relative expression of core clock genes in the brain of sticklebacks under constant darkness (DD), which suggests that either the molecular oscillator is highly light-dependent or that there was a significant oscillation but we were unable to detect it. The first interpretation implies that clock gene expression rhythms are not endogenously controlled, which contrasts with what has been observed in the brain or neural tissues of many other fish species (Whitmore et al., 1998; Cermakian et al., 2000; Patiño et al., 2011; Vera et al., 2013; Moore and Whitmore, 2014; Costa et al., 2016; Ceinos et al., 2019). A more parsimonious explanation is that a biological or technical effect prevented us from detecting any significant rhythmicity. First, it is possible that sticklebacks displayed interindividual variation in their acrophases (i.e. different peak times) of clock gene expression, so that the variation at each time point was too great to allow detection of a significant rhythm. Interestingly, interindividual variation in peak times of clock gene expression is often reported in natural populations, for example in humans (Teboul et al., 2005; Nováková et al., 2013; Ferrante et al., 2015; Takahashi et al., 2018). In fish, clock gene expression has not been quantified often in wild-caught populations, with the notable exception of the Mexican cavefish (Astyanax mexicanus) (Beale et al., 2013). Without surprise, it was shown that a wild population of Mexican cavefish displayed greater interindividual variation in clock gene expression than a laboratory population, a result that could be explained by greater genetic variation in the wild population (Beale et al., 2013). To demonstrate that wild sticklebacks display different peak times of clock gene expression, the same fish would have to be sampled multiple times over a 24-h period. As the sampling would need not to be lethal, using fin samples could be considered (Cavallari et al., 2011; Beale et al., 2013; Mogi et al., 2017).
Technical issues could also explain the fact that we did not detect significant circadian rhythmicity. We quantified clock gene expression in the whole brain, but if different regions of the stickleback brain host independent molecular oscillators that display different circadian rhythms or if some brain tissues are arrhythmic, using the whole brain might have drowned the rhythmic signal. For instance, previous studies in mammals reported that the same clock gene can have various peak times of expression in different brain regions (Abe et al., 2002; Mure et al., 2018). In fish, few studies have quantified clock gene expression in different brain regions, the size of this organ often being limiting. Among those that did, some reported distinct expression peaks between brain regions (Cermakian et al., 2000; Huang et al., 2010), but several others rather observed similar expression peaks throughout the brain (Whitmore et al., 1998; Weger et al., 2013; Moore and Whitmore, 2014; Costa et al., 2016). Besides, whole brains have often been used successfully to quantify clock gene expression rhythms in fish, both under LD (Lopez-Olmeda et al., 2010; Sánchez et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2015; Tudorache et al., 2018) and under DD (Whitmore et al., 1998; Cermakian et al., 2000; Vera et al., 2013; Moore and Whitmore, 2014). It thus seems that we could have detected significant rhythmicity using the whole brain of sticklebacks. That being said, in future studies, it would be possible to sample specific regions of the stickleback brain such as the diencephalon (which contains the hypothalamus) and the midbrain (which contains the optic tectum) (Sanogo et al., 2012; Greenwood and Peichel, 2015; Bell et al., 2016). These regions have been used a few times to quantify clock gene expression rhythms in other fish species (Feliciano et al., 2011; Martín-Robles et al., 2012; Moore and Whitmore, 2014; Costa et al., 2016). Another possibility would be to sample other organs such as the heart and the liver, which are commonly used to study the clock molecular oscillator in fish (Sánchez et al., 2010; Cavallari et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2015).
In this study, we showed that there is interindividual variation in the circadian rhythm of locomotor activity in wild sticklebacks, with most individuals exhibiting activity not controlled by their clock. In addition, we found that sticklebacks were mostly nocturnal under LD, but we observed large interindividual variation that could be due to a differential response to the masking effect of light among individuals. In future studies, asking whether a lack of circadian control is common in wild populations of sticklebacks or whether it is driven by specific environmental challenges (such as high predation risk) will allow us to better understand what selection pressures can shape the evolution of circadian rhythms. Moreover, assessing whether other biological rhythms are more strongly controlled by the clock and whether the stickleback circadian system can be entrained by other environmental factors (such as food availability) will inform us about the functional importance of the circadian clock in this species. In parallel, studying the molecular oscillator will tell us what clock mechanisms underlie potential differences in circadian rhythms between populations and individuals. Importantly, in the study of gene expression, interindividual variation will need to be addressed and the choice of target organs used to quantify clock gene expression will affect the capacity to detect significant rhythmicity. Overall, our study suggests that a strong circadian control on behavioural rhythms is not necessarily advantageous in a natural population of sticklebacks and that the masking effect of light is potentially responsible for the large interindividual variation in the daily phase of activity.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Dany Turcotte from Westburne Canada for his valuable advice regarding the photoelectric sensors and for graciously volunteering his time to program the controller. We thank Verônica A. Alves and Morgane Philippe for the field work, Sann Delaive for help with tissue sampling, and the personnel of the LARSEM at Université Laval for their help with fish rearing. We thank Chloé S. Berger, Florent Sylvestre and Verônica A. Alves for their comments on previous versions of the manuscript. We also thank Nicolas Cermakian for his valuable advice on this research.
Footnotes
Author contributions
Conceptualization: M.-P.B., N.A.-H.; Methodology: M.-P.B., N.A.-H.; Formal analysis: M.-P.B.; Investigation: M.-P.B.; Writing - original draft: M.-P.B., N.A.-H.; Supervision: N.A.-H.; Funding acquisition: M.-P.B., N.A.-H.
Funding
This project was funded by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) Discovery Grant to N.A.-H., and by an NSERC Graduate Scholarship and a Fonds de recherche du Québec Nature et technologies (FRQNT) Scholarship to M.-P.B. Deposited in PMC for immediate release.
Data availability
Datasets are available from the Dryad digital repository (Brochu and Aubin-Horth, 2021): https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.4xgxd259s.
References
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing or financial interests.