ABSTRACT
Mechanosensory-cued hatching (MCH) is widespread, diverse and important for survival in many animals. From flatworms and insects to frogs and turtles, embryos use mechanosensory cues and signals to inform hatching timing, yet mechanisms mediating mechanosensing in ovo are largely unknown. The arboreal embryos of red-eyed treefrogs, Agalychnis callidryas, hatch prematurely to escape predation, cued by physical disturbance in snake attacks. When otoconial organs in the developing vestibular system become functional, this response strengthens, but its earlier occurrence indicates another sensor must contribute. Post-hatching, tadpoles use lateral line neuromasts to detect water motion. We ablated neuromast function with gentamicin to assess their role in A. callidryas’ hatching response to disturbance. Prior to vestibular function, this nearly eliminated the hatching response to a complex simulated attack cue, egg jiggling, revealing that neuromasts mediate early MCH. Vestibular function onset increased hatching, independent of neuromast function, indicating young embryos use multiple mechanosensory systems. MCH increased developmentally. All older embryos hatched in response to egg jiggling, but neuromast function reduced response latency. In contrast, neuromast ablation had no effect on the timing or level of hatching in motion-only vibration playbacks. It appears only a subset of egg-disturbance cues stimulate neuromasts; thus, embryos in attacked clutches may receive unimodal or multimodal stimuli. Agalychnis callidryas embryos have more neuromasts than described for any other species at hatching, suggesting precocious sensory development may facilitate MCH. Our findings provide insight into the behavioral roles of two mechanosensory systems in ovo and open possibilities for exploring sensory perception across taxa in early life stages.
INTRODUCTION
Animals ubiquitously use vibration to inform their behavior (Cocroft et al., 2014; Hill et al., 2019; Hill, 2009). Most research in this area focuses on vibrational communication signals in the context of mate choice, competition and parental care (Cocroft et al., 2014). These signals are intentionally produced and generally operate between conspecifics. In addition, passively produced, unintentional vibrational cues can inform behavior. A smaller subset of research examines how animals use such incidental vibrations and other physical disturbance cues, generated either abiotically or biotically, to express context-appropriate behaviors. Conspecifics can generate disturbance cues (Doody et al., 2012), but other sources abound, such as weather (Marquez et al., 2016) and, more commonly, predators and prey (Bacher et al., 1997; Brownell and Leo van Hemmen, 2001; Castellanos and Barbosa, 2006; Oberst et al., 2017; Pfannenstiel et al., 1995; Roberts, 2018). Physical disturbances generated incidentally as animals move might be especially useful as cues because they can be difficult to conceal in a predator–prey context. Even embryos can sample incidental disturbance cues from their environment (Warkentin, 2005, 2011a; Warkentin et al., 2021). Similar to later life stages, embryos use physical disturbance cues from abiotic sources such as rainfall (Roberts, 2001) and wave action (Griem and Martin, 2000; Martin et al., 2011), or biotic sources such as conspecifics (Endo et al., 2019; Noguera and Velando, 2019), hosts (Wang et al., 2012; Whittington and Kearn, 1988, 2011) and predators (Doody and Paull, 2013; Warkentin, 2005). From flatworms and insects to frogs and turtles, embryos of all sorts use disturbance cues to inform their hatching timing, yet the mechanisms mediating vibration and other mechanosensing in ovo are largely unknown.
The red-eyed treefrog, Agalychnis callidryas, is an excellent species in which to study how embryos detect and respond to physical disturbance or vibrations. As adults, these arboreal amphibians lay eggs on plants overhanging rainforest ponds. Usually within 7 days, embryos hatch and fall into the water below, where they will develop as tadpoles, but they can hatch prematurely to escape from threats to eggs, including pathogens, flooding and terrestrial egg predators (Warkentin and Caldwell, 2009). This escape-hatching response changes dramatically over development. The earliest observed hatching occurs at 3 days post-oviposition in response to strong hypoxia cues, but the onset of predator-induced, mechanosensory-cued hatching (MCH) does not occur until the following day (Warkentin et al., 2017). During the period of development between the onset of hatching responses to hypoxia and those to mechanosensory cues, embryos are clearly able to hatch to escape threats. Nonetheless, they do not use this ability to flee from predators. Hatching ability is, therefore, not the sole constraint limiting the onset of escape-hatching responses to predator attacks.
The embryos of A. callidryas clearly use cues in multiple sensory modalities, including vibration (Warkentin, 2005; Warkentin et al., 2019), hypoxia (Rogge and Warkentin, 2008) and light level (Güell and Warkentin, 2018), to inform hatching. In sensing physical disturbances, these embryos might use one or multiple mechanosensory systems, either to perceive different cue components available in attacks or as potentially redundant or synergistic sensors of the same cue component. As a first step toward determining the role of mechanosensory system development in the ontogeny of predator-induced hatching ability, we examined a general vertebrate motion sensor, the vestibular system of the inner ear (Jung et al., 2019). When otoconial organs in the developing vestibular system become functional, MCH increases substantially, but its earlier occurrence at a low level indicates that vestibular mechanoreceptors cannot be the only sensors that enable hatching when eggs are physically disturbed (Jung et al., 2019).
The mechanosensory lateral line system is found in all fishes and aquatic life stages of amphibians and serves to detect movement, vibrations and pressure gradients in the surrounding water. It is composed of mechanoreceptive neuromast sensory organs with hair cells that are sensitive to local water displacement (Lannoo, 1999) and are similar in morphology and function to hair cells in the auditory and vestibular system of vertebrates (Mogdans, 2019; Roberts et al., 1988). The lateral line system and vestibular system are responsive to many of the same stimulus fields (Braun and Coombs, 2000) and the hair cells of the lateral line and inner ear even have comparable thresholds of pressure detection (Van Netten, 2006). When something moves in the water, it creates water motion that deflects the ciliary bundles of the hair cells of the neuromasts, which opens mechanically gated ion channels (Harris et al., 1970; Sand et al., 1975). Upon deflection by water flow, hair cells within neuromasts depolarize and release neurotransmitter onto afferent neuron terminals, which then convey this information to the hindbrain (Liao, 2010; Raible and Kruse, 2000). This stimulus information helps fish and aquatic amphibians orient in currents (Elepfandt, 1982; Montgomery et al., 1997), maintain position within a school (Partridge and Pitcher, 1980), find prey (Bleckmann, 1980; Hoekstra and Janssen, 1985; Montgomery and Macdonald, 1987; Pohlmann et al., 2004) and detect predators (Montgomery, 1989; Schwalbe et al., 2012). Fish and aquatic amphibians are able to detect low-level water motion with both the lateral line system and the inner ear, but the relative roles of these sensory systems are unclear (Karlsen and Sand, 1987).
Here, we investigated the potential contribution of the embryonic lateral line system to sensing physical disturbance cues that inform escape-hatching decisions of A. callidryas embryos during snake attacks. When snakes bite, bump and pull at eggs, embryos may receive complex mechanosensory stimuli, including motion, pressure and tactile elements (Jung et al., 2019). The flow of perivitelline fluid, which constantly circulates around embryos, may also be altered as snakes stretch or squash egg capsules, thereby altering input to the lateral line system in the egg. However, not all embryos in an attacked clutch receive these complex cues. Embryos more distant from the snake may receive only whole-egg motion cues, transmitted through the clutch, but it is unclear whether or how such whole-egg motion could stimulate the lateral line system (Jung et al., 2019). To assess the role of the lateral line and vestibular systems in sensing complex mechanosensory stimuli, we used a simulated attack cue (jiggling eggs in a standardized fashion with a blunt probe; Warkentin et al., 2017). We tested embryos with functional and inactivated lateral line systems, before and after the onset of vestibular function. To assess whether egg motion alone stimulates the lateral line system, we used a custom-made vibration playback system to shake eggs in trays without providing concurrent tactile or egg deformation cues (Warkentin et al., 2019, 2021) and tested embryos with functional and inactivated lateral line systems. Thus, we assessed the role of two mechanosensory systems, in two disturbance cue contexts, in a critical anti-predator behavior demonstrated by developing embryos.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Egg clutch collection and care
We collected young (0–1 day old) A. callidryas (Cope 1862) egg clutches and the leaves on which they were laid from the Experimental Pond in Gamboa, Panama (9.120894 N, 79.704015 W) and brought them to a nearby laboratory (at ambient temperature and humidity) at the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute. We mounted clutches on plastic cards for support, positioned them over aged tap water in plastic cups, then placed them inside plastic bins with screen lids connected to an automatic misting system (Mist King, Jungle Hobbies, www.mistking.com) set to mist clutches with rainwater at regular intervals to maintain hydration. Most clutches are laid between 22:00 h and 02:00 h, so we assigned embryos to daily age-classes and report developmental timing starting from midnight of their oviposition night (Warkentin, 2002, 2005). All tested individuals were staged based on external morphological markers (Warkentin, 2017) and were morphologically normal, in developmental synchrony with siblings in their clutch, and in intact, turgid eggs at the start of experiments. Hatchlings from gentamicin-treated eggs were reared in the laboratory under ambient conditions until hair cells regenerated (Hernández et al., 2007; Ma et al., 2008; Thomas et al., 2015; Williams and Holder, 2000), as confirmed with 4-di-2-ASP staining (details below), within 3 days of hatching. All hatchlings were returned to the pond from which they were collected. This research was conducted under permits from the Panamanian Environmental Ministry (SC/A-10-18 and SE/A-42-19) and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute (2017-0601-2020-2).
Vestibulo-ocular reflex measurement
To assess vestibular function, we measured the roll-induced vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) of newly hatched tadpoles or manually decapsulated embryos using previously described methods (Horn and Sebastian, 1996; Horn et al., 1986; Jung et al., 2019). We placed hatchlings in a close-fitting tube of water and used a custom-made tadpole rotator to roll them about their body axis 180 deg in each direction, photographing them in frontal view every 15 deg (Jung et al., 2019). This method allowed us to measure vestibular function rapidly and without anesthesia. From each photograph, we measured right and left eye angle and body axis angle using ImageJ (Schneider et al., 2012). From each angular measurement series, we constructed an individual VOR curve using a sine-fitting function in Python (v.2.7.9, Build 1, Python Software Foundation). The peak-to-peak amplitude of the curve fit is the measured VOR magnitude.
Lateral line system visualization
To visualize hair cells in neuromasts, newly hatched or decapsulated tadpoles were immersed in a 500 µmol l−1 solution of the fluorophore 4-(4-diethylaminostyryl)-1-methylpyridinium iodide (4-di-2-ASP, Sigma D-3418, Sigma-Aldrich) for 5 min at room temperature in the dark. Tadpoles were then rinsed in aged tap water to remove excess fluorophore and placed in a shallow Petri dish or snout-up in a modified pipette mount. This method allowed us to label neuromast hair cells and photograph them in live embryos without anesthesia or treatment with a paralytic. We used an Olympus stereoscope equipped with epifluorescence (GFP filter set) to photograph each individual. We considered neuromasts showing fluorescence to be functional and a complete lack of fluorescence to indicate neuromast inactivation. Fluorescence gradually faded within hours and disappeared within a day of treatment. Treated individuals developed normally, as verified by comparison with untreated controls.
Lateral line system knockout
To test the role of the lateral line system in predator-induced, mechanosensory-cued escape-hatching behavior, we used the ototoxic aminoglycoside antibiotic gentamicin to temporarily ablate neuromast function (Kroese and van den Bercken, 1982; Montgomery et al., 1997; Van Trump et al., 2010). At embryonic age 3 days, we removed individual eggs from clutches and placed groups of eight siblings into hexagonal polystyrene weigh boats (4.5 cm width×1 cm depth). We added 1 ml of rainwater to each boat, partially submerging eggs, and waited 1 h for water absorption to increase and standardize turgidity before treatment. To begin treatment, we removed the remaining water and added a fresh 1 ml of rainwater into each boat. Control eggs remained in rainwater, while treatment eggs received a timed dosage series of gentamicin sulfate solution-10 (MP Biomedicals™, Fisher Scientific) mixed into the rainwater to gradually increase the concentration (starting dose 2 µl, +2 µl at 3 h, +2 µl at 6 h, +0.5 µl at 9 h, +0.5 µl at 10.5 h). The final concentration was the minimum dosage that effectively blocked lateral line system function, determined based on pilot experiments. The gradual increase in concentration was necessary to maintain egg turgor and membrane integrity, as needed for hatching assays, also based on pilot experiments. At 13 h, we extracted 0.5 ml of solution from each boat to increase air exposure of the eggs and ensure sufficient oxygen availability for these terrestrial eggs as they continued developing in the boats until hatching-response tests were carried out. Only eggs that maintained normal turgidity until testing were used to assess embryo responses to mechanosensory cues. We confirmed lateral line system blocking with 4-di-2-ASP staining in each experiment (Fig. 1A,B). To assess whether gentamicin damaged hair cells in embryonic ears, as suggested previously (Bagger-Sjoback, 1997; Simmons et al., 2004; Van Trump et al., 2010; Yan et al., 1991), we compared VOR of stage-matched (within one developmental stage of each other) control and gentamicin-treated siblings, across all experiments described below (Fig. 1C).
Hatching-response test: manual egg jiggling
To assess the MCH response of gentamicin-treated (Fig. 1A) and control (Fig. 1B) embryos across ontogeny, we performed a standard egg-jiggling assay (simulated attack), using a blunt probe to manually jiggle eggs in an intermittent pattern for 5 min (Jung et al., 2019; Warkentin et al., 2017). This simulated attack includes complex motion and tactile elements, and transient deformation of egg membranes. We tested 1202 individual embryos from 60 clutches (at least 5 individuals per treatment per clutch) from 2 July to 5 August 2019. We moved sibling pairs of eggs (treatment and control individuals) into their own weigh boats with a drop of water and manually jiggled them with a blunt metal probe, alternating 15 s of stimulation and 15 s of rest (i.e. stimulating eggs in a pair alternately) for 5 min or until the embryo hatched.
We exposed embryos to jiggling cues during three developmental periods (Fig. 2A): (1) across the onset of vestibular function (N=544 individuals later subdivided based on VOR measurements; age: range 4.03–4.36 days, mean±s.e.m. 4.19±0.003 days; stage 27–28, 27.19±0.017), (2) after vestibular function was well established (N=360 individuals; 4.54–5.03 days, 4.74±0.007 days; stage: 27–30, 28.99±0.010), and (3) closer to spontaneous hatching (N=298 individuals, age: 5.29–5.56 days, 5.39±0.003 days, stage: 29–32, 30.44±0.046). For the youngest age group, we measured the VOR of all individuals that hatched (N=67 individuals) and checked for lateral line system blocking of all hatched, gentamicin-treated individuals (N=3). We used VOR measurements to determine vestibular function onset times, dividing siblings into subsets tested before and after VOR onset. For the two older groups, we measured VOR in at least one gentamicin-treated and one control individual per clutch (N=111 individuals). We used 4-di-2-ASP staining to check for lateral line system blocking in at least one individual per gentamicin-treatment boat with hatching and visualized neuromasts in one untreated control per testing session (i.e. the period of hours over which a group of trays were tested; N=31 individuals).
For the youngest developmental period, we determined the start of testing for each sibship based on external developmental markers (Warkentin, 2017). We began testing early in stage 27, near the mean first appearance of MCH across clutches (Warkentin et al., 2017) and shortly before the onset of vestibular function (Jung et al., 2019), then stimulated pairs of siblings sequentially until VOR was visible in tested hatchlings. After analysis of VOR tests, we estimated the onset of vestibular function for each clutch as the first time point at which any individual from the clutch showed a VOR>10 deg. Applying this criterion generated two groups: ‘no VOR’ (N=286 individuals; age: 4.03–4.36 days, 4.17±0.005 days; all stage 27) and ‘with VOR’ (N=258 individuals; age: 4.07–4.36 days, 4.20±0.005 days; stage 27–28, 27.23±0.027). These two groups differ significantly in age (Wilcoxon rank sum: z=3.8, P=0.00007). In prior work using manual egg jiggling at stages 26–29 and measuring the VOR of every individual, the hatching response of individuals ‘without vestibular function’ was similarly low (23%) using criteria of <1 or <10 deg, and it increased substantially as VOR increased to ca. 30–40 deg (Jung et al., 2019). Here, using the VOR of measured individuals to estimate the presence/absence of vestibular function in their unmeasured siblings allowed us to test for MCH in many more individuals than we could measure for VOR, but entails some classification errors. Applying our criterion to a prior VOR dataset (Jung et al., 2019), we estimated 14% false positives (individuals lacking VOR but classified as ‘with VOR’) and 22% false negatives (individuals with VOR but classified as ‘no VOR’) from among the untested siblings; such classification errors would reduce the chance of detecting an effect of vestibular function on hatching in the current data.
Hatching-response test: vibration playbacks
To provide a motion-only stimulus, without concurrent tactile cues, we performed vibration playbacks to groups of embryos held in custom-made egg-trays (Warkentin et al., 2019, 2021). Trays held 8–15 eggs (11.79±0.25 eggs per tray) in individual funnel-shaped spaces, allowing hatched tadpoles to slide through the tray to the water below. We tested 1132 individuals in 96 trays from 63 clutches from 25 July to 7 August 2019 (age: 5.40–5.75 days, 5.56±0.009 days, stage: 30–33.67, 31.27±0.080). We aimed to test embryos of the same age as the 5 day individuals tested with jiggling cues, but because of logistic constraints, playback embryos were, on average, 4 h older than jiggled embryos. Embryos were treated with gentamicin or as rainwater controls at age 3 days, as described above, and were moved to individual spaces in egg-trays early at embryonic age 4 days, when eggs could be easily handled without inducing hatching. Full trays were maintained on racks over aged tap water in humidors until testing, with regular misting until age 5 days (Warkentin et al., 2019, 2021).
We designed a synthetic low-frequency vibration stimulus to elicit high hatching rates, based on prior playbacks to 5 day old embryos (Caldwell et al., 2009; Jung et al., 2019; Warkentin et al., 2006b, 2017, 2019, 2021). We generated white noise in MATLAB and filtered it using a custom-written script (available upon request) to compensate for non-linearities in the shaker transfer function and generate a frequency distribution similar to snake-attack vibrations (Caldwell et al., 2009), with high energy below 50 Hz and intensity dropping off above that (Fig. 3A). The temporal pulse pattern included a 3-pulse ‘primer’ and a series of seven 10-pulse groups separated by 30 s intervals of silence (Fig. 3B). Within each pulse group, the base temporal pattern had a cycle length of 2 s, consisting of 0.5 s pulses of vibration with roughly rectangular amplitude envelopes separated by 1.5 s intervals of silence (Fig. 3C). To record playback vibrations, we attached a small (0.14 g) AP19 accelerometer (AP Technology International B.V., Oosterhout, The Netherlands) to an egg-tray using dental wax (Warkentin et al., 2021). Accelerometer output was routed through an AP5000/10 charge-to-voltage converter to a B&K 1704 signal conditioner (Brüel & Kjær, Nærum, Denmark), digitized with a Focusrite Scarlett 2i2 external sound card (focusriteplc.com), and recorded using Raven Pro 1.3 bioacoustics software (Cornell University Laboratory of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, USA) on a Macbook Air computer. The RMS amplitude of the playback vibrations, excluding intervals of silence, was 6.5 ms−2.
Playback methods followed published work detailing vibration presentation via egg-trays (Warkentin et al., 2019, 2021). For testing, we clamped egg-trays holding embryos to a custom-made interface on a rigid post attached to an electrodynamic minishaker (Model 4810, Brüel & Kjær). The shaker, post and tray were horizontally leveled, with foam supports under the post and tray edge and water under the embryos. Thus, embryos were moved horizontally and hatchlings fell into the water below. Shaker output was controlled by Audacity 2.1.0 on a 2014 Macbook Air, via a custom-made amplifier (E. Hazen, Boston University Electronic Design Facility). We recorded any hatching induced by the set-up procedure (N=3 individuals), then allowed 5 min for acclimation before starting the playback (N=38 individuals hatched during acclimation). Individuals that hatched before the stimulus started were not considered part of the test. We noted whether and when (to the nearest second) each embryo hatched during stimulus playback and 3 min of post-playback observation (Fig. 3D–F). We then immediately (within 20 min) measured VOR of a subset of individuals, including a pair of gentamicin-treated and control individuals from 32 clutches (plus 14 clutches with unpaired data). We confirmed hatching competence of embryos remaining unhatched by manually stimulating them with a blunt metal probe (N=251 individuals). We also used 4-di-2-ASP staining to confirm lateral line blocking in at least one individual per treated tray with hatching and to visualize neuromasts in one control per testing session (N=43 treated, 5 untreated individuals). We staged 3 haphazardly selected hatchlings from each tray (Warkentin, 2017).
Lateral line system ontogeny
To determine at what point during lateral line development the hatching response to jiggling cues begins, we compared the ontogeny of the lateral line system and of MCH, using a developmental series of 79 embryos from 25 clutches from 6–11 August 2018. For each individual, we first tested for MCH, using the same manual egg-jiggling stimulus as in the first hatching response test (Warkentin et al., 2017). Test clutches were transported just prior to hatching competence and tested in an air-conditioned laboratory of the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute in Gamboa. We recorded latency to hatch and embryos that remained unhatched after 5 min were manually decapsulated using sharp forceps. We staged embryos under a stereoscope (Warkentin, 2017). Neuromasts were then stained using 4-di-2-ASP as described above and photographed in frontal and dorsal views. The frontal view showed neuromasts in the ventral, oral, infraorbital, nasal and supraorbital lines (Fig. 4A), while the dorsal view showed neuromasts in the nasal, supraorbital, middle and dorsal lines (Fig. 4B). All neuromasts within each line were labeled and counted using ImageJ software (Schneider et al., 2012). We took several photos in each view, in different focal planes, framing and magnification, then counted neuromasts in each line from the photo(s) that showed them best. Counts from supraorbital and middle neuromast lines were extrapolated from one side, assuming bilateral symmetry within individuals. When lines extended across multiple photos, we used landmarks to avoid double-counting or missing neuromasts. We averaged counts per line from two independent counters and summed them across lines to estimate the total number of neuromasts per individual (Fig. 4C; Table S1). We also measured tadpole total length from dorsal images (Fig. 4D).
Statistics
All statistical tests were carried out in the R statistical environment (v.3.6.2, R Development Core Team 2019, http://www.r-project.org) in RStudio (v.1.2.5033, RStudio Team 2019, https://rstudio.com/). We used generalized linear mixed models within the ‘lme4’ package (Bates et al., 2015) with clutch as a random effect and likelihood ratio tests to compare nested models for fixed effects and interaction effects on the number of neuromasts (error distribution: negative binomial), hatching responses (binomial) and first hatching latency (gamma). We used the fitconds function within the ‘fitplc’ package to plot curve fits for all hatching latencies (weibull) within trays (Duursma and Choat, 2017).
RESULTS
Lateral line system knockout with gentamicin
Our gentamicin treatment completely inactivated all neuromasts with no evidence of differences in the VOR of stage-matched gentamicin-treated and non-treated siblings (χ21=2.3, P=0.1302; Fig. 1).
Hatching-response test: egg jiggling
Across the onset of VOR (mean age 4.17 days without vestibular function versus 4.2 days with vestibular function), both lateral line and vestibular function strongly and independently increased the likelihood of hatching in response to egg jiggling (gentamicin: χ21=88.2, P<2.2e−16; VOR: χ21=30.4, P=3.5e−8; interaction: χ21=0.7, P=0.4; Fig. 2A,B). In embryos lacking vestibular function (mean age 4.17 days), gentamicin reduced the hatching response significantly from 10% to 1% (χ21=14.4, P=0.0001; Fig. 2B). Just after the onset of VOR, gentamicin still reduced hatching significantly (41% versus 2%; χ21=73.8, P<2.2e−16; Fig. 2B). While both age and neuromast function increased hatching, the gentamicin effect on MCH decreased with age (age: χ23=200.1, P<2.2e−16; gentamicin: χ21=105.7, P<2.2e−16; interaction: χ23=19.9, P=0.0002; Fig. 2B). At 4.7 days, gentamicin still significantly reduced hatching (70% versus 41%; χ21=35.4, P=2.7e−09) but at 5.4 days, close to spontaneous hatching, all jiggled eggs hatched, with or without functional neuromasts (Fig. 2B).
Latency to hatching decreased with age and lateral line function (age: χ23=79.2, P<2.2e−16; gentamicin: χ21=36.9, P=1.266e−09; interaction: χ23=3.8, P=0.29; Fig. 2C). The low hatching response of gentamicin-treated embryos before and just after the onset of VOR limited our sample size for measurement of latency, reducing statistical power for comparisons (Fig. 2C). However, gentamicin treatment increased latency to hatching at both 4.7 days (χ21=17.1, P=3.5e−05) and 5.4 days (χ21=21.2, P=4.1e−06) with no indication that the effect decreased with age (interaction: χ21=2.84, P=0.09; Fig. 2C).
Hatching-response test: vibration playbacks
To assess whether egg motion alone can stimulate the lateral line system, we used vibration playbacks to shake gentamicin-treated and untreated embryos held in custom-made egg trays. In these playbacks, lateral line function had no effect on the proportion of embryos hatched (χ21=1.8, P=0.1787; Fig. 3D) or the timing of hatching (χ21=1.1, P=0.2986; Fig. 3E,F).
Lateral line system ontogeny
The number of neuromasts in all seven lines increased with developmental stage (Fig. 4C; Table S1). Total number of neuromasts increased with size (Fig. 4D), age (Fig. 4E) and, most strongly, developmental stage (Fig. 4F). From stage 27 to 29, the number of neuromasts more than doubled (Fig. 4F). Both developmental stage (χ21=50.9, P=9.5e−13) and the total number of neuromasts (χ21=15.7, P=7.5e−5) were significant predictors of hatching in egg-jiggling experiments (Fig. 4G). When embryos had fewer than a threshold number of neuromasts (247), none hatched, whereas embryos with more neuromasts often hatched.
DISCUSSION
We demonstrate that the developing lateral line and vestibular systems both contribute to escape-hatching of red-eyed treefrog embryos, and that the roles of these sensors change during development and vary with disturbance cue type.
Lateral line system knockout with gentamicin
Using gentamicin, we achieved complete temporary lateral line ablation in A. callidryas embryos with no evidence of vestibular system damage (Fig. 1). Others found evidence that gentamicin can cause vestibular system damage when administered intramuscularly (Bagger-Sjoback, 1997; Yan et al., 1991) or by direct immersion post-hatching (Simmons et al., 2004; Song et al., 1995; Van Trump et al., 2010). Our study is the first to investigate the effects of gentamicin administered incrementally to embryos developing in ovo, which was necessary to avoid water loss from eggs and, later, test for hatching responses. The gradual passage of gentamicin across the vitelline membrane into the perivitelline space while eggs were in the treatment bath, and the potential loss of gentamicin from older eggs maintained in egg-trays before vibration playbacks, means we do not know the precise concentrations embryos were exposed to over time. However, our longest exposure durations and peak exposure concentrations exceeded those in previous studies (Simmons et al., 2004; Song et al., 1995; Van Trump et al., 2010), supporting that for some animals under some exposure conditions gentamicin can selectively damage hair cells in the lateral line without impairing the function of hair cells in the vestibular system. Lateral line neuromasts are directly exposed to the fluid bathing an embryo, but hair cells of the inner ear are not, and the barriers protecting these internal cells likely change with development.
Notably, we found no evidence for ear damage in early stages, at the onset of vestibular function, and also in more developed 5 day old embryos. Immature and recently regenerated hair cells are resistant to aminoglycoside antibiotics (Dai et al., 2006; Hashino and Salvi, 1996; Murakami et al., 2003; Van Trump et al., 2010). Thus, in the youngest tested embryos, vestibular hair cells may have had only brief gentamicin exposure after maturing to a point of vulnerability. In the lateral line system, neuromasts began to regain fluorescence, when stained with 4-di-2-ASP, within hours of hatching and cessation of gentamicin exposure. We suspect this was due to maturation of gentamicin-resistant developing hair cells. Embryos tested at 5 days would have had functional hair cells in their ears for over 24 h (29.8 h from mean onset of VOR to mean testing age) under gentamicin treatment, yet we also found no evidence for vestibular damage in these older embryos. This suggests that, compared with more mature tadpoles (Bagger-Sjoback, 1997; Simmons et al., 2004; Van Trump et al., 2010; Yan et al., 1991), embryonic anatomy may better protect otic hair cells against externally administered gentamicin.
Hatching-response test: manual egg jiggling
In embryos lacking vestibular function, a significant effect of gentamicin treatment on hatching response reveals that the onset of lateral line function precedes the onset of vestibular function and plays a key role in very early MCH (Fig. 2B). Immediately following the onset of VOR, the lateral line system continues to contribute strongly to risk assessment and hatching (Fig. 2B). The overall higher hatching rate in VOR-positive animals, across both treated and control individuals (Fig. 2B), is consistent with a key role of otoconial organs in embryonic vibration sensing (Jung et al., 2019). However, at the onset of MCH, neither lateral line nor vestibular function alone enabled a strong response; the multimodal combination of input from both senses greatly increased the likelihood of hatching in a simulated attack (Fig. 2B).
Soon after A. callidryas gain hatching competence, the lateral line system plays a critical role in sensing and responding to predator cues. We tested the effect of gentamicin on jiggling-induced hatching in two later periods to determine whether the dependence of MCH on lateral line function changes developmentally. In A. callidryas, embryos hatch spontaneously from 5 to 7 days, while younger embryos almost never hatch if undisturbed (Güell and Warkentin, 2018; Hite et al., 2018; Warkentin, 2000; Warkentin et al., 2001). From age 4–5 days, embryos become more likely to escape during snake and wasp attacks (Gomez-Mestre and Warkentin, 2007; Warkentin, 1995, 2000) and to hatch in vibration playbacks (Jung et al., 2019; Warkentin et al., 2019, 2021). Over this same developmental period, our results show that MCH responses become less dependent on multimodal input from the lateral line plus vestibular system. This might reflect increasing strength of vestibular input as embryo ears develop (Jung et al., 2018).
When animals use multimodal sensory integration to inform behavior, a sensor may contribute to a response without being required for its occurrence (Angelaki and Cullen, 2008). We examined the latency from stimulus onset to the hatching response (Fig. 2C) as a potentially more sensitive indicator of lateral line system contributions to embryo decisions, as latency affects escape success during predator attacks (Almanzar and Warkentin, 2018; Chaiyasarikul and Warkentin, 2017). We found that even embryos near the stage of spontaneous hatching, which have a very strong vestibular-mediated hatching response, still use lateral line input to accelerate their response to simulated attack cues (Fig. 2C).
Sensory integration of lateral line and inner ear inputs
Across development, embryos use both lateral line and inner ear mechanoreceptors to mediate their hatching response. Moreover, input from these two mechanosensory systems is non-redundant, motivating consideration of possible neural integration mechanisms. At least initially, the lateral line circuit and the vestibular circuit operate on parallel but separate tracks (Fay and Edds-Walton, 2008; McCormick, 1999). There are reports of both sensory systems sending fibers from their peripheral receptors to common first-order nuclei, but the convergence is minor compared with their distinct termination areas in the midbrain (McCormick et al., 2016). One shared synaptic target in the hindbrain is the Mauthner (M-) cell, a paired sensorimotor structure that receives input from the inner ear (Szabo et al., 2007) and the mechanosensory lateral line (Mirjany et al., 2011), and sends output to relay and motoneurons that activate axial musculature (Eaton et al., 2001). These cells mediate fast C-start escape responses in fishes and amphibians (Eaton et al., 2001; Korn and Faber, 1975, 2005; Korn et al., 1974), and have been suggested to play a role in hatching to escape from egg predators in zebrafish embryos (Eaton and Nissanov, 1985). However, hatching in fishes and amphibians requires enzymatic digestion of the egg envelope as a first step, with muscular activity secondarily playing a role in final membrane rupture and exit from the egg (Cohen et al., 2019; Korwin-Kossakowski, 2012). In the hatching process of A. callidryas, enzyme release and membrane rupture typically occur before the strong S-shaped contractions of axial musculature (swimming motions) that propel the embryo from its capsule (Cohen et al., 2019). Moreover, M-cells mediate very fast responses, on the order of milliseconds (Eaton and Nissanov, 1985), while MCH in A. callidryas occurs many seconds to several minutes after the onset of stimulation (Warkentin et al., 2007, 2017), suggesting that embryos integrate information over time before deciding to hatch. Indeed, the propensity of embryos to hatch increases even during periods of vibrational silence following certain vibration patterns (J. Jung, M. Guo, J. G. McDaniel and K. M. Warkentin, unpublished). Thus, it seems likely that hatching decisions are made, and input from sensory modalities may be integrated, at higher levels in the brain (McCormick et al., 2016). It would be particularly interesting to identify the mechanism controlling the rapid temporally and spatially regulated release of hatching enzyme (Cohen et al., 2019; Salazar-Nicholls et al., 2020) and link it via neural integration mechanisms to the sensory inputs that trigger the hatching process.
Hatching-response test: vibration playback
The fact that lateral line function had no effect on the proportion of embryos hatching (Fig. 3D) or their latency to hatch (Fig. 3E) in response to motion-only vibration playback suggests that only a subset of more complex physical disturbances stimulate the lateral line system, while potentially any egg motion may stimulate the vestibular system. Thus, some embryos in attacked egg clutches receive unimodal vestibular stimulation while others receive multimodal vestibular and lateral line system input. Some may also receive input from cutaneous touch receptors (Blaxter, 1987; O'Brien et al., 2012). Given the role of lateral line system input in reducing hatching latency, as well as increasing hatching likelihood in younger embryos, this variation in the type(s) of sensory input an embryo receives under different predation contexts likely contributes to the variation in the likelihood and timing of hatching during attacks (Almanzar and Warkentin, 2018; Warkentin, 2000; Warkentin et al., 2006a). The variation in received mechanosensory stimuli within a clutch may also be a mechanism contributing to threat-sensitive embryo behavior (Ferrari and Chivers, 2009; Hughey et al., 2015; Mathis et al., 2008; Van Buskirk, 2016); as predators must touch eggs to eat them, the risk of mortality during attacks is likely higher for eggs receiving multimodal cues than for those receiving motion cues alone.
Lateral line system ontogeny
In a recent study, we found hatching in response to egg jiggling to begin, on average, at stage 27.3 and considered hatching to be ‘consistent’ at stage 28.8 (Fig. 4C), the second time in the developmental series that both of two tested siblings hatched (Jung et al., 2019; Warkentin et al., 2017). This known onset of MCH aligns ontogenetically with a rapid increase of neuromast number in developing embryos (Fig. 4C). The pattern of a neuromast-number threshold for hatching (Fig. 4G) suggests that lateral line system development may limit the onset of disturbance cue sensing and associated hatching behavior.
Lateral line ontogeny in a comparative context
The number of neuromasts at hatching and their proliferation after hatching varies greatly across taxa. Some fishes hatch with only two functional neuromasts, and early larvae simply float in the water column [e.g. flounder (Kawamura and Ishida, 1985), tuna (Kawamura et al., 2003), grouper (Mukai et al., 2006), catfish (Mukai et al., 2010)]. Subsequent rapid lateral line system development is correlated with behavioral changes (avoiding obstacles, feeding, migrating, settling and surviving seasonal floods) and parallels rapid development of other sense organs such as eyes, ears, taste buds and olfactory epithelium (Kawamura and Ishida, 1985; Kawamura et al., 2003; Mukai et al., 2006, 2010). Neuromast proliferation can also occur slowly. For instance, cod larvae hatch with five lateral body neuromasts, and only add one more by feeding onset, 2–3 weeks later (Blaxter, 1984). The developmental stage at which neuromast function begins differs among species, and appears to be related to hatchling habitat and habits (Otsuka and Nagai, 1997). For instance, ayu hatch with 20 pairs of well-developed neuromasts and migrate downstream immediately upon hatching (Kawamura et al., 1983). In contrast, pale chub hatch without a single neuromast and remain in the spawning bed for 4 days, during which neuromasts develop rapidly; at emergence into the river, larvae are responsive to water flow and have nearly caught up to ayu in neuromast number (Kawamura et al., 1983). Across species, developmental increases in the number of neuromasts are closely linked to the ontogeny of mechanosensory-guided behavior (Blaxter and Fuiman, 1989; Kawamura and Ishida, 1985; Llanos-Rivera et al., 2014). However, neuromast size, shape and hair cell polarity can also affect mechanosensory sensory function (Becker et al., 2016; Webb and Shirey, 2003).
Anatomically and physiologically, amphibian neuromasts resemble superficial neuromasts of teleost fishes (Metcalfe, 1985; Simmons et al., 2004). However, we know relatively little about their early development, except in a few species. At hatching, the salamander Ambystoma mexicanum has all 60 neuromasts, while in the frog Lithobates (Rana) pipiens, most neuromasts have not yet formed; however, by the onset of feeding, the lateral line appears to be fully formed and functional in both species (Smith et al., 1988). Among amphibians, early lateral line system development has been characterized in detail in Xenopus laevis (Roberts et al., 2009; Shelton, 1970; Simmons et al., 2004; Winklbauer, 1989), but studies rarely distinguish hatching timing. Reported hatching stage varies such that X. laevis have between 0 (Carroll and Hedrick, 1974) and 14 neuromasts at hatching, but embryos with just 6–8 neuromasts swim into water currents and responsiveness increases over the next 10 h with an increase in neuromast number (Roberts et al., 2009). In our study system, the hatching response of A. callidryas embryos to disturbance begins only when they have hundreds of neuromasts (Fig. 4G). Despite the large increase in neuromast number, there appears to be little change in neuromast size (M. J. Salazar Nicholls and J. Jung, unpublished observation from confocal images) or shape (Cohen et al., 2019) from 3 to 5 days post-oviposition. It is not yet clear how physical disturbance of eggs stimulates the neuromasts of embryos inside them; however, induced perturbations of perivitelline fluid motion within the egg capsule may provide a weaker or less clear stimulus to the lateral line system than currents in open water do after hatching. Additionally, the cost of unnecessarily swimming into a current may be lower than that of hatching prematurely.
The number of neuromasts that A. callidryas have at hatching is high, even compared with other anurans in late larval development (Lannoo, 1987). Agalychnis callidryas that hatched in response to egg jiggling, at 7.7–11.0 mm total length, had neuromast counts of 323.6±11.9 (range 247–434). For comparison, in a study of 36 other anuran species examined in late larval development, at their expected peak neuromast number (Lannoo, 1987; Winklbauer, 1989), the maximum reported was 332 in Rana aurora, at 25–27.5 mm snout–vent length (Lannoo, 1987). As lateral line development depends on the size of the animal, and tadpoles tend to add neuromasts during post-hatching development (Fabrezi et al., 2012; Winklbauer, 1989), these other species presumably have fewer neuromasts at hatching. This suggests that A. callidryas have precocious lateral line development, as well as high neuromast numbers. However, we know nothing about lateral line ontogeny in other anurans with documented or suspected mechanosensory-cued escape-hatching behavior (Brown and Iskandar, 2000; Brown et al., 2010; Buckley et al., 2005; Chivers et al., 2001; Gomez-Mestre et al., 2008; Poo and Bickford, 2014; Smith and Fortune, 2009; Touchon et al., 2011). Egg predation might be a selective force favoring earlier or greater development of mechanosensory systems. A comparative analysis of lateral line and vestibular system development in relation to the distribution of MCH in anurans (Warkentin, 2011a,b) would be informative. Assessing links between sensory morphology and embryo behavior could reveal patterns of functional variation and adaptive evolution and provide further insight into how embryos use sensory information.
Embryos across diverse taxa respond to physical disturbance cues from many ecologically relevant sources (Warkentin, 2011a,b; Warkentin et al., 2021). In contexts such as antipredator defense (Doody and Paull, 2013; Warkentin, 2005) and sibling hatching synchronization (Endo et al., 2019; Noguera and Velando, 2019), embryos' ability to sense these cues is often essential for their survival, yet we know very little about the sensory systems that mediate MCH or even, in many cases, which elements of physical disturbance cues are most relevant. Herein, we show that to detect egg-disturbance cues, A. callidryas embryos use their vestibular system, a sensory mechanism likely to be relevant across vertebrates, and their lateral line system, a mechanism which may contribute to MCH in other frogs and fishes. Our study serves as proof-of-concept for the feasibility of combining neuromast ablation in ovo with hatching-response tests, and it highlights the value of latency as a sensitive response variable in the context of multimodal mechanosensing. The methods we present open opportunities for comparative research into embryo sensory ecology with other species that hatch in response to mechanosensory cues. Our findings provide insight into the functional roles of two different mechanosensory systems prior to hatching and reveal new possibilities for exploring embryonic sensory perception across taxa.
Acknowledgements
We thank J. Gregory McDaniel for collaboration in biotremology, including development of the tadpole rotator and egg-tray playback system, Jacqueline Webb and Andrea Simmons for advice on neuromast blocking, Estefany Caroline Guevara Molina, Luis Alberto Rueda Solano and Henry Macías for help with egg jiggling and care, Casey Lam for assistance measuring eye angles and body angles from photographs, members of the Gamboa Frog Group at STRI and BU Egg Science Research Group for discussions on this research, and Jacqueline Webb, Pete Buston and two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on the manuscript.
Footnotes
Author contributions
Conceptualization: J.J., K.M.W.; Methodology: J.J., S.J.S.-R., K.M.W.; Software: J.J.; Validation: J.J., S.J.S.-R., K.M.W.; Formal analysis: J.J.; Investigation: J.J., S.J.S.-R.; Resources: K.M.W.; Data curation: J.J.; Writing - original draft: J.J.; Writing - review & editing: J.J., S.J.S.-R., K.M.W.; Visualization: J.J.; Supervision: K.M.W.; Project administration: K.M.W.; Funding acquisition: K.M.W.
Funding
This research was funded by the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute (STRI), Boston University and the National Science Foundation (IOS-1354072 to K.M.W. and James Gregory McDaniel), and conducted under permits SC/A-10-18 and SE/A-42-19 from the Panamanian Ministerio de Ambiente and STRI IACUC protocol 2017-0601-2020-2.
Data availability
Data are available from the Dryad Digital Repository (Jung et al., 2020): https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.prr4xgxkc
References
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing or financial interests.