Echolocating animals exercise an extensive control over the spectral and temporal properties of their biosonar signals to facilitate perception of their actively generated auditory scene when homing in on prey. The intensity and directionality of the biosonar beam defines the field of view of echolocating animals by affecting the acoustic detection range and angular coverage. However, the spatial relationship between an echolocating predator and its prey changes rapidly, resulting in different biosonar requirements throughout prey pursuit and capture. Here, we measured single-click beam patterns using a parametric fit procedure to test whether free-ranging Atlantic spotted dolphins (Stenella frontalis) modify their biosonar beam width. We recorded echolocation clicks using a linear array of receivers and estimated the beam width of individual clicks using a parametric spectral fit, cross-validated with well-established composite beam pattern estimates. The dolphins apparently increased the biosonar beam width, to a large degree without changing the signal frequency, when they approached the recording array. This is comparable to bats that also expand their field of view during prey capture, but achieve this by decreasing biosonar frequency. This behaviour may serve to decrease the risk that rapid escape movements of prey take them outside the biosonar beam of the predator. It is likely that shared sensory requirements have resulted in bats and toothed whales expanding their acoustic field of view at close range to increase the likelihood of successfully acquiring prey using echolocation, representing a case of convergent evolution of echolocation behaviour between these two taxa.

Echolocation has evolved in species as diverse as cave birds, microchiropteran bats and toothed whales (Griffin, 1958; Schevill and McBride, 1956). In contrast to other sensory modalities such as vision or olfaction, echolocation depends on the production of a signal that travels through the environment and is reflected by objects, resulting in returning echoes that are subsequently detected and processed by the echolocating animal (Griffin, 1958). The acoustic field of view of echolocating predators is defined as the area ahead of the predator that is ensonified sufficiently to produce audible echoes (Jakobsen and Surlykke, 2010) and is given by the angular coverage, termed the beam width, and the intensity or range of the sonar. The beam width and intensity of emitted signals depend on their spectral and temporal properties and on the acoustic behaviour of the echolocating animal (Moss and Surlykke, 2001). There is increasing evidence that bats and toothed whales exhibit significant control over their biosonar (Jakobsen and Surlykke, 2010; Johnson et al., 2008; Moore et al., 2008; Wisniewska et al., 2012) and it is likely that they actively control the perception of their surroundings through changes in biosonar signals and biosonar field of view (Moss et al., 2011).

Biosonar signals are characterised by signal parameters that include source level, duration, centroid frequency, bandwidth and three-dimensional beam pattern. The source level and beam pattern are of prime importance as they define the functional range and spatial coverage of the biosonar system (Madsen et al., 2007; Urick, 1983). The source level (SL, in dB re. 1 µPa at 1 m for underwater applications) is the sound pressure level measured on the acoustic axis of the biosonar beam at a reference distance of 1 m from the source (Urick, 1983). The directivity index (DI, in dB) is the difference between the source level of the source in question and the source level of a hypothetical omnidirectional transducer radiating the same acoustic power (Urick, 1983). As the biosonar intensity drops off with increasing off-axis angle, the half-power beam width is defined as the angle at which the source level intensity has decreased to half (−3 dB) of the on-axis intensity. Whereas the DI is important when discussing sound production efficiency, the beam width is a more relevant parameter for understanding how the biosonar system performs in clutter. Focusing the sound energy into a narrow beam restricts the detection of objects to a narrow cone along the axis of the sound beam by increasing their returning echoes and by simultaneously reducing the echoes generated by objects further away from the axis of the biosonar beam. Directional emission of echolocation signals therefore narrows the acoustic field of view of the echolocating animal, facilitating target detection and discrimination within a restricted area and improving long-range biosonar performance through a higher on-axis source level (Madsen and Surlykke, 2013).

The beam width of a biosonar system depends on the dimensions of the sound-producing structure and the frequency of the emitted sound so that an increased signal frequency or an enlarged transmitter aperture will result in a narrower biosonar beam (Urick, 1983). The product of the wave number k and the transducer radius a, given as ka=(2π/λ)×a, is a useful parameter defining the relationship between the effective transducer aperture and the radiated wavelength λ (Au, 1993), with higher directionality achieved through a higher ka number. This means that animals can increase their biosonar beam width by either (1) decreasing the frequency of their outgoing sonar signals, or (2) reducing the effective size of the transmitting aperture (Au, 1993).

List of symbols and abbreviations
     
  • a

    transducer radius

  •  
  • DI

    directivity index

  •  
  • EPR

    equivalent piston radius

  •  
  • Fc

    centroid frequency

  •  
  • HPBW

    half-power beam width

  •  
  • k

    wave number

  •  
  • Qrms

    root mean square quality factor

  •  
  • SL

    source level

  •  
  • SSE

    sum of squared error

The amount of control that echolocating animals have over their biosonar beam is remarkable. Microchiropteran bats producing frequency-modulated echolocation signals reduce call amplitude, frequency content and bandwidth during the foraging buzz (Kalko, 1995). Given the relationship between frequency and directionality, this means that microchiropteran bats modify their biosonar directionality and field of view dynamically during prey pursuit and capture by changing biosonar frequency rather than aperture size (Jakobsen et al., 2012; Jakobsen and Surlykke, 2010). Echolocating delphinids studied so far also demonstrate some control over their biosonar beam. Trained delphinids are capable of changing the source level (Moore and Patterson, 1983), frequency content (Moore and Pawloski, 1990) and directionality (Au et al., 1995) of their biosonar signals, and they control their field of view further by steering the beam direction and by controlling the width of the biosonar beam (Finneran et al., 2014; Moore et al., 2008). Most of these adjustable properties may be linked to changes in biosonar frequency, and it is possible that, like in bats, control over the biosonar field of view is primarily a by-product of frequency control. However, a recent study has suggested that trained harbour porpoises may increase their biosonar beam width at close range without concurrent changes in signal frequency (Wisniewska et al., 2012). Whether delphinids modify their beam shape strictly through changes in frequency, as in bats, or use changes in the size or shape of their sound-producing structures to further modify their acoustic field of view remains uncertain, and changes in biosonar beam width have yet to be documented from free-ranging animals.

Here, we tested whether free-ranging Atlantic spotted dolphins (Stenella frontalis Cuvier 1829) can modify the width of their biosonar beam using a new method capable of estimating the beam width of individual clicks from vertical hydrophone array recordings. We demonstrate that echolocating Atlantic spotted dolphins seem to increase their field of view when they approach the recording array, and that a significant part of the beam width increase must relate to changes in the functional radiation aperture of the melon. Expanding the biosonar field of view at close range may help prevent rapid prey escape responses from taking the prey out of the acoustic field of view of the approaching predator. Our results suggest that both spectral changes to biosonar clicks and morphological changes to the sound generator may contribute to these biosonar dynamics.

We investigated the biosonar field of view using two methods. First, we developed a method for estimating the composite biosonar beam pattern for a series of on-axis echolocation clicks recorded on a one-dimensional array. Second, we then estimated the beam width of individual clicks using a parametric fit based on a circular piston model and the amplitude spectra of on-axis clicks recorded across off-axis hydrophones, and we cross-validated these estimates with the composite beam width estimate. Finally, we used the parametric spectral fit for estimating the field of view of individual echolocation clicks to show that beam width changes as a function of distance from the receiver array, and that these changes are caused in part by changes in frequency, and in part by morphological changes of the sound emitter.

Composite beam pattern estimation and method validation

Test trials with two calibrated transducers emitting directional signals were conducted. During both test trials, the transducer was turned gradually along an axis parallel to the axis of the hydrophone array to simulate the click scans of S. frontalis and other species of toothed whales that have been recorded with a linear, vertical array (e.g. Madsen et al., 2004).

To estimate the accuracy of the composite beam pattern, two variants of the same procedure were evaluated. Both variants provided reasonable estimates of the beam width (Fig. 1). The traditional error model resulted in negatively biased errors of −19% to −8% beam width estimates, whereas the logarithmic error model resulted in smaller errors of −4% to −1% beam width estimates (Table 1).

Fig. 1.

Beam pattern can be accurately estimated using a linear array. Mean beam pattern estimate of a Reson TC2116 transducer emitting a 50 kHz signal (A) and a Reson TC2130 transducer emitting a 150 kHz signal (B) as measured with a 4-hydrophone array. The on-axis sound intensity and angle of incidence from the acoustically localised source to each receiver were estimated through a second-degree polynomial fit (see Materials and methods). The sound intensity relative to the on-axis intensity is plotted against the angle of incidence for each of four receivers recording the same click (squares, colour-coded according to click number). A circular piston model with an aperture minimising the root mean square (rms) error of received sound intensity on a logarithmic decibel scale (log method) was fitted to the data (dark grey line) and 95% confidence intervals for the fit were calculated using a bootstrap method with 2000 replicates (dark grey dashed lines). The known calibration curve of the transducer is overlaid for comparison (light grey line). Note the non-Gaussian error distribution.

Fig. 1.

Beam pattern can be accurately estimated using a linear array. Mean beam pattern estimate of a Reson TC2116 transducer emitting a 50 kHz signal (A) and a Reson TC2130 transducer emitting a 150 kHz signal (B) as measured with a 4-hydrophone array. The on-axis sound intensity and angle of incidence from the acoustically localised source to each receiver were estimated through a second-degree polynomial fit (see Materials and methods). The sound intensity relative to the on-axis intensity is plotted against the angle of incidence for each of four receivers recording the same click (squares, colour-coded according to click number). A circular piston model with an aperture minimising the root mean square (rms) error of received sound intensity on a logarithmic decibel scale (log method) was fitted to the data (dark grey line) and 95% confidence intervals for the fit were calculated using a bootstrap method with 2000 replicates (dark grey dashed lines). The known calibration curve of the transducer is overlaid for comparison (light grey line). Note the non-Gaussian error distribution.

Table 1.

Validation of composite beam width estimation

Validation of composite beam width estimation
Validation of composite beam width estimation

Composite beam pattern estimates were surprisingly robust to low sample sizes. Equivalent piston radius (EPR) confidence intervals were consistently wider during simulations with few on-axis clicks, but the mean EPR was highly stable (Fig. 2). The traditional error model for fitting the piston yielded consistently higher EPR estimates (narrower beam width) compared with the logarithmic error model (Table 1, Fig. 2). However, this bias was relatively small, of the order of less than half a degree. A similar evaluation of the impact of sample size on data from Atlantic spotted dolphins revealed that EPR estimates recorded with a 6-element hydrophone array were much more robust to low numbers of on-axis clicks, probably because each click was measured across a larger part of the biosonar beam (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2.

Beam pattern estimates are robust to low sample size. Estimated equivalent piston radius (mean ±95% bootstrap confidence intervals) as a function of the number of on-axis clicks (ranging from 2 to the total sample size N in increments of 2) included in the piston fitting procedure. Individual clicks recorded on a 4-channel (A) or 6-channel (B) hydrophone array were sampled with replacement from the total population of on-axis clicks (A: N=23, B: N=19) and a piston fitting procedure implemented as described in Materials and methods. Means and confidence intervals were calculated using 500 bootstrap replicates. Note that the baseline for the Stenella frontalis dataset (grey dashed line in B) is based on the best-fitting piston model using the full sample size and logarithmic error model, not on the actual (and unknown) equivalent piston radius (EPR).

Fig. 2.

Beam pattern estimates are robust to low sample size. Estimated equivalent piston radius (mean ±95% bootstrap confidence intervals) as a function of the number of on-axis clicks (ranging from 2 to the total sample size N in increments of 2) included in the piston fitting procedure. Individual clicks recorded on a 4-channel (A) or 6-channel (B) hydrophone array were sampled with replacement from the total population of on-axis clicks (A: N=23, B: N=19) and a piston fitting procedure implemented as described in Materials and methods. Means and confidence intervals were calculated using 500 bootstrap replicates. Note that the baseline for the Stenella frontalis dataset (grey dashed line in B) is based on the best-fitting piston model using the full sample size and logarithmic error model, not on the actual (and unknown) equivalent piston radius (EPR).

Source parameters of S. frontalis biosonar clicks

A total of 1035 clicks including 28 on-axis clicks were recorded from wild S. frontalis. Of these, 19 clicks were recorded within 20 m, with the dolphins milling around the array and often moving in to investigate it. The clicks were typical broadband delphinid echolocation signals (Au, 1993) characterised by short duration and high amplitude (Fig. 3A), with a high centroid frequency and broad bandwidth (Fig. 3B) that corresponds well with the short duration and dominant period in the signal waveform.

Fig. 3.

Stenella frontalis echolocation clicks. (A) Waveform of the four echolocation clicks of highest amplitude. Waveforms (sample rate 500 kHz) were upsampled (×10 low-pass interpolation), phase aligned and normalised to the largest pressure excursion for easier comparison. (B) Individual log-transformed power spectra (black lines) and mean S. frontalis energy distribution (grey dashed line) derived from all on-axis echolocation clicks. Power spectra were constructed using a 320-point fast Fourier transform based on a 32-point (64 µs) window (resulting in ×10 sinc interpolation) centred on the peak envelope of each click. Note that the flatness of the mean energy distribution is partly a result of differences in peak frequency between clicks, whereas individual power spectra exhibit much more spectral variation.

Fig. 3.

Stenella frontalis echolocation clicks. (A) Waveform of the four echolocation clicks of highest amplitude. Waveforms (sample rate 500 kHz) were upsampled (×10 low-pass interpolation), phase aligned and normalised to the largest pressure excursion for easier comparison. (B) Individual log-transformed power spectra (black lines) and mean S. frontalis energy distribution (grey dashed line) derived from all on-axis echolocation clicks. Power spectra were constructed using a 320-point fast Fourier transform based on a 32-point (64 µs) window (resulting in ×10 sinc interpolation) centred on the peak envelope of each click. Note that the flatness of the mean energy distribution is partly a result of differences in peak frequency between clicks, whereas individual power spectra exhibit much more spectral variation.

The source parameters of these oceanic dolphins were characterised by mean back-calculated apparent source level (±1 s.d.) of 209±4.7 dB re. 1 µPa peak–peak, corresponding to 200±4.6 dB re. 1 µPa root mean square (rms) over a −10 dB envelope time window. The maximum estimated source level was 216 dB re. 1 µPa peak–peak, corresponding to 207 dB re. 1 µPa rms (Table 2). The spectral parameters reflected the broadband nature of these biosonar clicks. Centroid frequency averaged 86±9.0 kHz and centralised rms bandwidth averaged 33±2.7 kHz, resulting in an average quality factor (Qrms) of 2.6 (Table 2).

Table 2.

Source properties of echolocation clicks from Atlantic (Stenella frontalis) and Pantropical (Stenella attenuata) spotted dolphins

Source properties of echolocation clicks from Atlantic (Stenella frontalis) and Pantropical (Stenella attenuata) spotted dolphins
Source properties of echolocation clicks from Atlantic (Stenella frontalis) and Pantropical (Stenella attenuata) spotted dolphins

Using 19 on-axis S. frontalis clicks recorded within 20 m, the logarithmic error model estimated an EPR of 5.0±0.2 cm (mean±s.e.m.) with confidence intervals of 4.6–5.4 cm (Table 1) for a −3 dB beam width of 10.3 deg in the vertical plane (assuming dolphins were swimming dorsal side up, which seemed to be the predominant swimming orientation for animals near the surface), and a composite DI of 25 dB (bootstrap confidence interval 24.4:25.9 dB) (Table 3). The composite vertical beam pattern and confidence intervals estimated using the logarithmic fitting procedure is shown in Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Composite vertical beam pattern of S. frontalis. The exact angle of incidence was estimated by fitting a second-degree polynomial to data points consisting of the hydrophone recording the highest source level and the two neighbouring hydrophones. (A) Apparent source level difference relative to the estimated on-axis source level is shown as a function of angle of incidence (black squares). A piston model (dark grey line) corresponding to an on-axis S. frontalis click convolved by the angle-specific impulse response of a circular piston with an equivalent piston radius of 5.2 cm was fitted to the data. The 95% confidence intervals of the estimated beam pattern (dark grey dashed lines) were calculated using a bootstrap method with 2000 replicates. (B) Polar plot of estimated vertical beam pattern and 95% bootstrap confidence intervals.

Fig. 4.

Composite vertical beam pattern of S. frontalis. The exact angle of incidence was estimated by fitting a second-degree polynomial to data points consisting of the hydrophone recording the highest source level and the two neighbouring hydrophones. (A) Apparent source level difference relative to the estimated on-axis source level is shown as a function of angle of incidence (black squares). A piston model (dark grey line) corresponding to an on-axis S. frontalis click convolved by the angle-specific impulse response of a circular piston with an equivalent piston radius of 5.2 cm was fitted to the data. The 95% confidence intervals of the estimated beam pattern (dark grey dashed lines) were calculated using a bootstrap method with 2000 replicates. (B) Polar plot of estimated vertical beam pattern and 95% bootstrap confidence intervals.

Table 3.

Directional properties of Atlantic spotted dolphin (Stenella frontalis) echolocation clicks

Directional properties of Atlantic spotted dolphin (Stenella frontalis) echolocation clicks
Directional properties of Atlantic spotted dolphin (Stenella frontalis) echolocation clicks

Estimates of beam width for individual clicks are necessary to understand whether free-ranging animals shape their biosonar beam to different needs. An estimate of the EPR for each click was derived from the parametric spectral fit (Fig. 5). The EPR was 5.0±0.3 cm (mean±s.e.m.), with 95% confidence intervals calculated using the percentile bootstrap method of 4.5–5.6 cm (Table 3). These results were cross-validated with the results from the composite beam pattern estimates, and the two methods corresponded well with each other (Table 3).

Fig. 5.

EPR estimated for an individual biosonar click using a parametric fit. (A) Modelled beam pattern for increasing aperture size (solid lines), relative power as a function of absolute angle measured over six receivers (red squares) and modelled beam pattern for a 4.57 cm piston (red dashed line). Receiver 3 (shown in B) is highlighted. (B) Parametric spectral fit. For each receiver, an observed 16-point amplitude spectrum (red squares) was calculated from the signal waveform. Expected amplitude spectra were calculated by convolving the on-axis signal waveform with the angle-specific impulse response of a circular piston (solid lines, colour coded according to modelled piston size, same colour scale as in A). (C) Individual sum of squared errors (SSE) for each receiver (coloured lines) and total SSE for a signal recorded at a range of 6.4 m and with an estimated EPR of 4.57 cm that minimises the total SSE across channels.

Fig. 5.

EPR estimated for an individual biosonar click using a parametric fit. (A) Modelled beam pattern for increasing aperture size (solid lines), relative power as a function of absolute angle measured over six receivers (red squares) and modelled beam pattern for a 4.57 cm piston (red dashed line). Receiver 3 (shown in B) is highlighted. (B) Parametric spectral fit. For each receiver, an observed 16-point amplitude spectrum (red squares) was calculated from the signal waveform. Expected amplitude spectra were calculated by convolving the on-axis signal waveform with the angle-specific impulse response of a circular piston (solid lines, colour coded according to modelled piston size, same colour scale as in A). (C) Individual sum of squared errors (SSE) for each receiver (coloured lines) and total SSE for a signal recorded at a range of 6.4 m and with an estimated EPR of 4.57 cm that minimises the total SSE across channels.

The parametric fit revealed that the directionality of the biosonar clicks produced by S. frontalis changed with range from the recording array. The EPR, and hence the beam width of the animal, correlated significantly with the range of the animal to the hydrophone array (linear regression: R2=0.31, F17=7.7, P=0.013, EPR=0.16r+3.15, where r is radius). There was also a significant negative relationship (best fitting slope of −0.06 Fc, where Fc is centroid frequency) between EPR and click Fc in kHz (linear regression: R2=0.35, F17=9.0, P=0.008) as would be expected from a relationship between directionality and frequency. We therefore calculated the difference between observed half-power beam width and expected half-power beam width (given constant EPR and measured Fc of each click), and a negative correlation with range persisted (linear regression: R2=0.26, F17=6.04, P=0.02).

Echolocating animals exercise a remarkable control over the spectral and temporal properties of their biosonar signals (Kalko and Schnitzler, 1993; Moore et al., 2008; Moore and Pawloski, 1990). Dynamic changes to the acoustic field of view (Jakobsen et al., 2013; Wisniewska et al., 2012) may help echolocating animals inspect their surroundings or lock on to specific targets, shaping the perception of their surroundings via changes in the acoustic gaze (Moss, 2010; Moss et al., 2011). Here, we show that wild Atlantic spotted dolphins seem to increase their vertical biosonar beam width by 50% over a fourfold decrease in range. Expanding the acoustic field of view during approach, and especially during prey capture, is likely to be important to ensure that prey remains within the acoustic field of view despite rapid prey avoidance reactions at close range.

Vertical arrays provide a robust quantification of the composite biosonar beam pattern and the beam width of individual clicks

Measuring the biosonar field of view of free-ranging echolocating animals is challenging and requires the use of extensive receiver arrays, acoustic localisation algorithms and conservative on-axis criteria (Madsen and Wahlberg, 2007; Surlykke et al., 2009). Composite beam patterns, defined as the mean beam pattern of a large series of clicks (Au et al., 1986), have been measured for multiple toothed whale species using linear vertical hydrophone arrays (Kyhn et al., 2010,, 2009; Wahlberg et al., 2011a,,b), but the errors inherent in this estimation procedure have never been addressed. We show here that the composite beam pattern of toothed whales, quantified as the mean EPR and corresponding biosonar beam width, can be reliably estimated using small sample sizes of on-axis biosonar signals derived from echolocation scans in the wild (Fig. 1) where clicks that are on-axis in the horizontal plane are identified using strict selection criteria. Using a modified fitting procedure from previous studies, beam pattern estimates using a vertical array are both accurate, with 1–4% mean errors compared with known source transducers (Fig. 1), and relatively precise, with 95% confidence intervals of the composite DI spanning 1.5–2.0 dB for the sample sizes used here (Table 1). Given the narrow sonar beam of most toothed whales, studies of beam pattern from wild animals often result in a small number of on-axis clicks (Jensen et al., 2013; Madsen et al., 2004; Wahlberg et al., 2011b). The beam pattern estimates were surprisingly robust to small sample sizes of on-axis clicks for artificial transducers (Fig. 2A) and quick convergence for delphinid signals (Fig. 2B), such that a small sample size will yield a realistic estimate of the biosonar beam width as long as the array covers a substantial part of the biosonar beam.

While composite beam pattern estimates may facilitate comparisons of biosonar field of view between species or populations, they are insufficient when addressing causes of variation in the biosonar beam within a dataset. To test whether free-ranging toothed whales such as Atlantic spotted dolphins modify their biosonar beam in the field, we derived an instantaneous estimate of the EPR for individual clicks based on predictable spectral changes (Au, 1993) at increasing off-axis angles (Fig. 5). Cross-validation with the composite beam pattern estimates obtained by fitting a circular piston model with the logarithmic error model indicates that the beam pattern for individual clicks reliably quantifies the biosonar field of view in the plane of the array (Table 3). Thus, using the methods developed here, it is possible to obtain estimates of the beam pattern of individual clicks, assuming axial symmetry, with a one-dimensional array, and to start teasing apart the underlying mechanisms for variations in the acoustic field of view. However, given the assumptions of axial symmetry and reliance on criteria to identify on-axis clicks in the horizontal plane, two-dimensional planar arrays should be employed where feasible to quantify close-range fine-scale beam patterns.

Free-ranging Atlantic spotted dolphins may increase biosonar field of view at close range

The directionality of biosonar signals allows echolocating animals to detect prey at greater range while reducing the impact of clutter from other nearby but off-axis objects. The broadband biosonar clicks produced by Atlantic spotted dolphins are characterised by a composite DI of 25 dB (Fig. 4), which is very similar to that reported for other similar-sized marine toothed whales (Au et al., 1978; Koblitz et al., 2012; Kyhn et al., 2010; Rasmussen et al., 2004; Wahlberg et al., 2011a). Echolocating toothed whales ranging in size across three orders of magnitude have all evolved highly directional biosonar signals with DIs of 23–32 dB (Koblitz et al., 2012; Madsen and Surlykke, 2013). It has been hypothesised that high directionality has been an important evolutionary driver for high echolocation frequencies in toothed whales (Koblitz et al., 2012), driven by the need for a long-range biosonar system in the marine environment (Jensen et al., 2013; Madsen and Surlykke, 2013). However, while a long biosonar detection range can be advantageous when searching for prey in the open ocean, it may pose other challenges when capturing prey at close range.

Two recent studies have measured changes in the echolocation beam shape and field of view as a function of target range, reaching very different conclusions. Kloepper et al. (2012) reported that a false killer whale, trained to discriminate between objects using echolocation, decreased its biosonar beam width by 8% when discriminating between targets at 2.5 m compared with those at 7 m. Even though this change seems counter-intuitive (decreasing SNR at long range where task discrimination is more difficult), the small magnitude of change is unlikely to have an impact on sensory performance. In contrast, harbour porpoises trained to approach and discriminate between two targets showed an increase in beam width at close range with more profound sensory implications (Wisniewska et al., 2012).

Here, we show that Atlantic spotted dolphins seem to increase their biosonar beam width by almost 50% (−3 dB beam width from 8 to 12 deg) when approaching the recording array with a fourfold decrease in range (Fig. 6). The sample size of our study was very low and it is likely that a simple linear regression is a poor approximation of how animals modify their acoustic gaze, especially when confronted by live, mobile prey rather than stationary recording arrays. Further lab and field experiments should be performed to verify these results and to tease apart the nature of the relationship between beam width and range under different environmental conditions and sensory challenges. However, the increased field of view at short range is comparable to the increasing field of view of trained harbour porpoises (Wisniewska et al., 2012) and bottlenose dolphins (Finneran et al., 2014). This indicates that both phocoenids (family Phocoenidae, using narrow-band high-frequency signals) and delphinids (family Delphinidae, using broadband biosonar signals) employ a dynamic biosonar beam that allows them to expand their field of view when approaching objects or prey animals, and that these sensory adaptations seem to be important for animals in the wild.

Fig. 6.

Dynamic changes in biosonar field of view for Atlantic spotted dolphins. (A) EPR estimated for each click through a parametric spectral fit (Fig. 5) and shown as a function of range. The black line represents a significant linear least squares regression (R2=0.31, F17=7.68, P=0.013) and the grey shaded area represents the 95% confidence interval of the linear regression. (B) The half-power beam width (HPBW) as a function of range. (C) Frequency-independent change in beam width. Observed HPBW divided by the HPBW that would be expected if beam width was determined by a constant EPR (the mean EPR estimated by the parametric fit method) and a changing centroid frequency (measured for each click). The black line represents a significant linear least squares regression (R2=0.26, F17=6.04, P=0.02) and the grey shaded area represents the 95% confidence interval of the linear regression. Data points are colour coded according to centroid frequency of the on-axis click.

Fig. 6.

Dynamic changes in biosonar field of view for Atlantic spotted dolphins. (A) EPR estimated for each click through a parametric spectral fit (Fig. 5) and shown as a function of range. The black line represents a significant linear least squares regression (R2=0.31, F17=7.68, P=0.013) and the grey shaded area represents the 95% confidence interval of the linear regression. (B) The half-power beam width (HPBW) as a function of range. (C) Frequency-independent change in beam width. Observed HPBW divided by the HPBW that would be expected if beam width was determined by a constant EPR (the mean EPR estimated by the parametric fit method) and a changing centroid frequency (measured for each click). The black line represents a significant linear least squares regression (R2=0.26, F17=6.04, P=0.02) and the grey shaded area represents the 95% confidence interval of the linear regression. Data points are colour coded according to centroid frequency of the on-axis click.

Conformational changes in the melon and surrounding air sacs may help modify the acoustic field of view independently of changes in biosonar frequency

The functional morphology of the structures associated with sound production in toothed whales is highly diverse (Cranford et al., 1996). Echolocation signals in delphinids seem to be produced at the right pair (Madsen et al., 2013b,, 2010) of sound-producing phonic lips (Norris and Harvey, 1972) and are then guided through the dorsal bursae and the fatty tissue of the melon (Cranford et al., 1996). Early studies suggested that the melon functioned as an acoustic lens to concentrate the sound beam (Wood, 1964). It has been suggested that there is an acoustic focal point in front of the melon where the acoustic rays converge (Kloepper et al., 2012) but this hypothesis does not take into account that the sound source itself is placed very close to the melon (Cranford et al., 1996). Finite element models based on computed tomography scans of delphinids (Cranford et al., 2013) instead show that the melon serves as an acoustic collimator. Indeed, several sound propagation simulations have revealed how the skull and associated air sacs provide the structural basis for the frequency-dependent directionality of toothed whale biosonar beams and simultaneously show that the melon may subsequently modify the shape of the biosonar beam (Aroyan et al., 1992; Cranford et al., 2013). While part of the change in beam width reported in our study could be explained by the effect of biosonar frequency, variation in biosonar frequency was limited (Table 2, Fig. 6) and changes in beam width after taking into account the effect of frequency were significant (Fig. 6C). Such changes could include modifying the geometry of the melon or surrounding air sacs, changing the position of the anterior and posterior bursae, or modifying the actuation of the phonic lips. Both the melon and surrounding air sacs are controlled by complicated epicranial musculature (Cranford et al., 1996; Huggenberger et al., 2009), which probably serves to modify directionality to some degree (Cranford et al., 2013). Similarly, the change in beam width that has been observed in the terminal part of prey capture in harbour porpoises also occurred without concurrent spectral changes and has been attributed to conformational changes in the soft structures of the nasal complex (Wisniewska et al., 2012). The extent to which the soft tissue structures in the odontocete forehead may serve to modify directionality defines how much echolocating animals are able to influence their sensory volume. The increase in beam width for Atlantic spotted dolphins exceeds 50% (Fig. 6) over a fourfold decrease in range, but the ranges tested do not include the very close target distances that are attained during prey capture attempts, suggesting that greater beam width variation is possible. In fact, trained porpoises readily increase their half-power beam width up to 90% during foraging buzzes (Wisniewska et al., 2015), demonstrating extensive control over acoustic gaze during prey capture in phocoenids.

An adaptable acoustic field of view may allow for long-range prey detection while facilitating prey capture at close range

Marine delphinids have probably evolved a highly directional biosonar beam to increase the on-axis source level and thus the detection range of possible prey items in the open ocean (Koblitz et al., 2012; Madsen and Surlykke, 2013) and only certain species of freshwater dolphins living in shallow river systems find prey using a short-range, broader biosonar beam (Jensen et al., 2013). Given the high biosonar update rate (typically 1–100 Hz) compared with swim speed (1–5 m s−1), it is likely to be more efficient for an echolocating toothed whale to scan a narrow beam gradually through an environment than it is to swim a greater distance with a shorter but wider biosonar. Blainville's beaked whales depend on significant head-scanning movements of up to ±10 deg at rates of 4 deg s−1 when searching for prey patches in the deep ocean (Madsen et al., 2013a; Shaffer et al., 2013), demonstrating how a narrow beam can be sequentially scanned through the environment to search a greater volume of water. However, a narrow beam can be a significant disadvantage when approaching and capturing prey items as rapid escape behaviours at close range might take the prey outside of the acoustic field of view of the approaching predator. Dynamic gaze adjustments, in contrast to a static biosonar beam, allow the approaching predator to increase the width of its field of view during this terminal capture phase, thereby decreasing the likelihood of prey escaping outside the biosonar beam. It is striking that wild delphinids may have comparable gaze adjustment behaviours to those of trained harbour porpoises (Wisniewska et al., 2012) and echolocating vespertillionid and phyllostomid bats (Brinkløv et al., 2011; Jakobsen and Surlykke, 2010) that all increase their field of view when approaching objects or prey animals. It is likely that shared eco-sensory requirements have led to similar biosonar behaviour in bats and toothed whales to increase the likelihood of successfully acquiring active prey using echolocation, supporting the case of convergent evolution of echolocation behaviour between these highly unrelated lineages.

Conclusions

Free-ranging Atlantic spotted dolphins seem to increase their beam width independently of Fc when approaching and investigating a recording array. This demonstrates that wild delphinids are capable of adjusting their outgoing sonar beam independently of frequency, probably using conformational changes of the soft tissue structures in the melon. Bats also expand their field of view when closing in on prey, though by changing frequency rather than aperture. An adaptable biosonar beam offers the benefits of long-range target detection with a narrow beam, while enhancing the capacity for tracking and capturing agile prey by increasing the field of view at close range.

Composite beam pattern estimation

Location

Ground-truth experiments were conducted at the Fjord & Bælt research facility in a net pen with a water depth of 3 m. An array of four Reson TC4034 hydrophones (Slangerup, Denmark) spaced 0.75 m apart was suspended horizontally from a floating pontoon at a depth of 1.5 m. Hydrophones were connected through a custom-made 4-channel amplification and filtering box (50 dB gain, 10 kHz high-pass filter, 200 kHz low-pass filter) to two synchronised 2-channel National Instruments (Hørsholm, Denmark) USB-6251 analog-to-digital converters (sampling rate 500 kHz, 16 bit) writing data to a laptop using custom-written LabView (National Instruments) sound acquisition software.

Calibration signals

Directional signals were transmitted 7.6 m from the axis of the horizontal array and at a depth of 1.5 m. Test signals were generated with an Agilent Technologies 33220A arbitrary waveform generator (Hørsholm, Denmark) and emitted through two circular piston transducers of different diameter. First, a 10-cycle, 50 kHz test signal was transmitted through an 18 cm diameter Reson TC2116 transducer at a rate of 10 pulses s−1. Afterwards, a 10-cycle, 150 kHz signal was transmitted through a 12 cm diameter Reson TC2130 transducer, also at a rate of 10 pulses s−1. In both cases, the transducer was positioned approximately in front of hydrophone 2 and turned gradually around the axis parallel to the axis of the hydrophone array so that the beam slowly passed back and forth across the array. Although depth constraints in the Fjord & Bælt facility required a horizontally deployed array, the rotation of the transducer around the axis of the array simulated a delphinid scanning its biosonar beam from side to side across a vertically deployed array in the field.

Analysis

Signals were analysed in Adobe Audition 3.0 (Adobe Systems, Inc.) and MatLab 7.0 (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) using the same metrics and definitions as for delphinid clicks recorded in the field (Madsen and Wahlberg, 2007). The highest click in each scan was assumed to be pointing towards the axis of the array. The received level on each hydrophone was then calculated as a rms sound pressure level. The angle of incidence was counted as being within the array aperture if the highest received level was found on one of the inner hydrophones, and the click was discarded from further analysis if this was not the case. Subsequently, the source of the click was localised acoustically using time-of-arrival differences (Spiesberger and Fristrup, 1990) following previous studies (Jensen et al., 2009; Kyhn et al., 2010,, 2009), after which an initial angle of incidence was calculated to each receiver by assuming that the click was focused on the hydrophone with the highest received level. Then, the theoretical on-axis amplitude and exact angle of incidence relative to the on-axis hydrophone were calculated by fitting a second-degree polynomial through the three points of angle and amplitude, corresponding to the hydrophone with the highest received level and its two neighbouring hydrophones. The peak of the resulting polynomial located between the three hydrophones was defined as the on-axis direction and amplitude of the biosonar beam. Finally, the angles and received levels for all hydrophones were calculated relative to the on-axis angle and amplitude.

Beam pattern estimation

The sonar system of bats and toothed whales is often modelled, for mathematical simplicity, as a flat, circular piston oscillating in an infinite baffle (Au et al., 1978; Strother and Mogus, 1970). Building on this model, the transmission beam pattern was estimated numerically using a parametric intensity fit. First, a waveform of an on-axis signal was identified; here, we used the signal with the highest back-calculated source level and no apparent reflections. This model on-axis signal was convolved with the angle-specific impulse response of a circular piston with an EPR from 0.5 cm up to 10 cm in 0.05 cm steps. For each step, the expected sound intensity relative to peak on-axis sound intensity was estimated for off-axis values up to the maximum angle of incidence recorded in the dataset, resulting in a modelled beam pattern for each piston size. These modelled values of relative sound intensity were compared with the estimated angle of incidence and measured sound intensity recorded across all hydrophones (see Kyhn et al., 2010). Two variants of the fitting procedure were tested. In the traditional error model, the best-fitting EPR was estimated as the piston model minimising the sum of squared errors between the modelled sound intensity and the measured sound intensity values for all recorded clicks. This reflects the method used in previous studies of odontocete beam patterns using linear arrays (Kyhn et al., 2010,, 2013,, 2009; Wahlberg et al., 2011a,,b). In the logarithmic error model, the modelled and measured sound intensity values were transformed to a decibel scale [10 log10(I/I0) where I0 is the on-axis intensity] and the best-fitting EPR was estimated as the piston model minimising the sum of squared errors between the log-transformed modelled and measured sound intensity values.

Beam pattern confidence intervals

A non-parametric bootstrap method (Efron, 1979) was constructed to evaluate the variation around the beam pattern estimate. Given that on-axis clicks were derived from different scans, on-axis clicks were assumed to be independent. For a sample size containing N on-axis clicks, individual bootstrap replicates were constructed by randomly sampling N clicks with replacement from the original recorded clicks. In this way, each randomly sampled click included the sound levels recorded across all hydrophones, meaning that this resampling technique is similar to the resampling techniques used for bootstrapping regression. The best-fitting EPR was calculated for each bootstrap as described above. Bootstrap 95% confidence intervals (Efron, 1981) were calculated as the 2.5 and 97.5 percentile of the bootstrap distribution of equivalent piston radii and were confirmed to be similar to the confidence intervals based on a normal distribution (Efron, 1981,, 1982). Confidence intervals for final estimates were based on 2000 bootstrap iterations to facilitate percentile confidence intervals (Manly, 1997).

Effects of sample size

We evaluated the effects of sample size on beam width estimates of original datasets using a similar bootstrap method by randomly selecting n clicks out of the available N clicks (sampled with replacement), where n was varied between 2 on-axis clicks up to the total sample size (N), in steps of two. For each sample size, 500 bootstrap iterations were made, and the average (as well as confidence intervals) of the estimated EPR was evaluated from the resulting distribution as described above.

Source parameters of S. frontalis biosonar clicks

Recording habitat

Biosonar clicks of spotted dolphins were recorded under a research permit from the Canary Islands Government granted to La Laguna University. Recordings of Atlantic spotted dolphins (S. frontalis) were obtained in May 2008 off the west coast of Tenerife, Canary Islands, Spain. Equipment was deployed when encountering groups of spotted dolphins. In several cases, dolphins remained close to the boat for half an hour after the vessel had stopped, circling and investigating the vessel and recording array throughout the recording period.

Recording equipment

An array of six Reson TC4034 hydrophones fixed in a hollow PVC tube was suspended vertically between a surface buoy and a 2 kg lead weight. Regular holes in the PVC tube allowed it to fill with water when submerged. The acoustic impedance of the PVC is fairly close to the acoustic impedance of seawater to minimise shadowing and reflections. The top two hydrophones were separated by 1.50 m whereas the remaining hydrophones were separated by 0.75 m. The top and bottom hydrophones were located at approximately 2 and 6.5 m depth. A diagram of this recording setup can be found in Kyhn et al. (2010). Hydrophones were connected through two 4-channel amplifier and filtering boxes (1 kHz high-pass, 200 kHz low-pass filter, 40 dB gain) to three synchronised 2-channel National Instrument USB-6251 multifunction devices with analog-to-digital converters running at a sampling rate of 500 kHz, 16-bit per channel. Data were written through USB to a Dell laptop with custom-made LabView data acquisition software. Hydrophones were calibrated before and after the field experiments using a Brüel & Kjær 4228 piston-phone calibrator (Nærum, Denmark). The frequency response of the recording chain was flat (±3 dB) from 1 to 200 kHz, with a clipping level of 194 dB re. 1 µPa (peak). Data acquisition was initiated and terminated manually, and files were stored approximately every minute.

On-axis criteria

Sound files were analysed with custom-written scripts in MatLab 7.0. An automated click extractor isolated echolocation clicks from each recording and displayed the click amplitudes as a function of time. Given the one-dimensional nature of the array, a set of on-axis criteria following Jensen et al. (2009) was employed to minimise the amount of clicks recorded away from the centre of the biosonar beam. A click was analysed only if it fulfilled the following criteria: (i) the click had the highest received level in a scan, i.e. a short series of clicks closely spaced in time and resembling a delphinid moving its beam across the array (normally with increasing and then decreasing signal amplitude); (ii) the highest received level of the click was recorded on one of the four central hydrophones; and (iii) the direct path of the click was stronger than any surface reflections present.

Acoustic localisation

The source of signals fulfilling these on-axis criteria was then acoustically localised using time-of-arrival differences of the same click to the six receivers (Wahlberg et al., 2001). The signal recorded on the third hydrophone (near the centre of the array), excluding any surface reflections, was cross-correlated with the signals recorded on the remaining hydrophones. The time-of-arrival differences were then found by taking the time of the cross-correlation peak relative to the cross-correlation peak of the first hydrophone, so that time-of-arrival localisation (Spiesberger and Fristrup, 1990) was made with respect to the top hydrophone. An average sound speed of 1524 m s−1 within the first 40 m water depth was measured with a CTD (RBR Data Logger model XR-620 CTD, RBR Global, ON, Canada). A two-dimensional acoustic localisation (rotationally symmetric around the axis of the array) was obtained as the least-squared solution to the hyperbola equations formed by each time-of-arrival difference and the corresponding difference in receiver coordinates following equations in Madsen and Wahlberg (2007). Signals that could not be localised were dismissed from further analysis. Clicks that were localised more than 50 m away from the array were removed from the analysis following calibration of localisation accuracy (Kyhn et al., 2010) to ensure a localisation error of less than 3 dB in transmission loss (Jensen et al., 2009).

Source parameter estimation

The range from the sound source to each hydrophone was calculated from source coordinates with the Pythagorean equation. The received levels at the hydrophones were calculated as the peak–peak and rms sound pressure levels within a time window given by the −10 dB end points relative to the peak of the amplitude envelope (Au, 1993; Madsen, 2005), which is reasonable given the high signal-to-noise ratio of the on-axis clicks. The click duration was defined as the time interval between −10 dB end points. An energy flux density measure of click amplitude was calculated as the sum of squared sound pressure values within the −10 dB analysis window (Madsen, 2005). The time between the peak of each click and the previous click was defined as the inter-click interval (Au, 1993). Subsequently, the click amplitude spectrum was calculated as the 3200-point discrete Fourier transform of a 32-point window centred on the peak envelope of each signal. The amplitude spectrum was squared and divided by its peak value to get the normalised power spectrum. The peak frequency, centroid frequency Fc (defined as the frequency separating the power spectrum into two halves of equal energy) and signal bandwidth (centralised rms bandwidth, −3 dB power and −10 dB power bandwidth) were calculated from this power spectrum, and the quality factor (Qrms) defined as Fc divided by the centralised rms bandwidth (Madsen and Wahlberg, 2007). The apparent source level, peak–peak (ASLpp) was defined as the back-calculated sound pressure level 1 m from the source at an unknown angle from the acoustic axis (Madsen and Wahlberg, 2007; Møhl et al., 2000) and calculated according to previous studies (e.g. Madsen et al., 2004) by compensating for the transmission loss between source and receiver. Transmission loss was estimated as the sum of spherical spreading (20 log10r) and frequency-dependent absorption (αr) over the range r, using a sound absorption coefficient α of 0.02 dB m−1 at 85 kHz. To quantify the biosonar beam pattern, we then restricted analysis to signals localised closer than 20 m to ensure high localisation accuracy (s.d. of less than 2% of range) (Kyhn et al., 2010) and we estimated the composite vertical beam pattern as described above.

Single-click beam pattern

Biosonar clicks exhibit predictable spectral changes when recorded off the acoustic axis (Au, 1993; Au et al., 2012; Wahlberg et al., 2011b). Here, we used these changes to estimate the instantaneous EPR from individual clicks using a parametric spectral fit based on a circular piston model. To do this, we extracted the click waveform recorded on all receivers in a 32-point window centred on the peak of the envelope. The click with the highest received level was taken as our best measure of the true on-axis click waveform. We estimated the corrected angle of incidence in the vertical axis using a second-degree polynomial fit as described above, and then calculated the angle of incidence for each receiver. Then, the expected click waveform was modelled for all receivers over a range of simulated circular piston apertures (EPR of 1–10 cm in steps of 0.005 cm). For each piston aperture, the on-axis waveform was convolved with the angle-specific impulse response of a circular piston (Eqn 1) at the angle of incidence estimated for each receiver, and the modelled amplitude spectrum obtained through a fast Fourier transform.

The angle-specific, far-field impulse response of a circular piston was defined (Beedholm and Møhl, 2006) as:
(1)
where:
and defined within |t|<T/2. Here, c is the sound speed of the medium, a is the piston radius (EPR) and θ is the off-axis angle of each receiver.

As a measure of the goodness-of-fit of each piston size, we calculated the residual sum of squared error (SSE) between the observed amplitude spectrum and the modelled amplitude spectrum for each receiver. Finally, the best-fitting EPR was estimated as the piston size minimising the total SSE across receivers (Fig. 5). When calculating total SSE, only receivers at angles between 2 and 25 deg were used to avoid potential frequency-dependent side-lobes, but this proved to have a negligible effect on the final fit.

The half-power beam width (HPBW) was then approximated for each click following Zimmer et al. (2005):
(2)
where 185 a constant angle, k is the wave number, Fc is the centroid frequency of the click, and a is the radius of a circular piston, approximated here as EPR.
The parametric fit procedure assumes that the piston is flat and circular. Systematic deviations from this assumption might therefore confound results. To account for this, we also estimated the instantaneous aperture size from predictable spectral changes in biosonar signals recorded off the acoustic axis at a known angle (Au, 1993). Clicks recorded off the acoustic axis are expected to have interference dips in the power spectrum as a function of off-axis angle (lower frequency for greater angles) and aperture dimensions (lower frequency for larger aperture) (Beedholm and Møhl, 2006; Wahlberg et al., 2011b). To avoid the circular piston assumption, we assumed only that the sound emitter was finite along the horizontal axis. For a signal transmitted from a line array with length given by 2a (in m) recorded at an angle θ (in deg) off the acoustic axis of the array, negative interference will occur at a frequency where the difference in travel distance between signals from the edge and centre of the array equals half the wavelength of the signal. For each receiver, we calculated the one-sided amplitude spectrum (16 points) and then extracted the frequency of the first spectral notch (a local minimum of −1 dB or greater) occurring after the peak frequency Fp (supplementary material Fig. S1). The EPR was then calculated using the frequency fn of the first spectral notch, recorded at an off-axis angle θ (estimated for each hydrophone relative to the peak of the polynomial) in a medium with sound speed c0 (1524 m s−1) as:
(3)

Only clicks with S/N ratio (where S is the signal intensity and N is the noise intensity) greater than 10 dB and recorded at angles greater than 2 deg and less than 25 deg were used for this analysis. For each individual click, the estimated EPR was taken as the average estimate across hydrophones. This approach yielded very similar results (supplementary material Fig. S2) compared with the parametric fit, and results are therefore included only in supplementary material Figs S1–S3.

We would like to acknowledge I. Domínguez, F. Díaz, L. Martinez, C. Aparicio, P. Arranz, C. Gonzalez and P. Aspas for their help during fieldwork. Calibrations were performed with the support of the Fjord & Bælt research facility in Kerteminde, Denmark. We would like to thank four anonymous reviewers for their constructive feedback on the manuscript.

Author contributions

F.H.J., M.W. and P.T.M. designed experiments and calibrations. F.H.J., K.B. and M.J. developed analytical methods. F.H.J., N.A.d.S., M.J. and P.T.M. acquired funding and conducted fieldwork. F.H.J. and M.W. performed method validation experiments. F.H.J., M.W., K.B., N.A.d.S., M.J. and P.T.M. drafted the manuscript.

Funding

The study was funded by frame grants from the Danish Natural Science Foundation to P.T.M. and M.W., and by the National Oceanographic Partnership Programme via a research agreement between La Laguna University (N.A.d.S.) and the Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution (M.J.). F.H.J. was supported by the Danish Council for Independent Research | Natural Sciences, and is currently funded by a postdoctoral fellowship from the Carlsberg Foundation.

Aroyan
,
J. L.
,
Cranford
,
T. W.
,
Kent
,
J.
and
Norris
,
K. S.
(
1992
).
Computer modeling of acoustic beam formation in Delphinus delphis
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
92
,
2539
-
2545
.
Au
,
W. W. L.
(
1993
).
The Sonar of Dolphins
.
New York
:
Springer Verlag
.
Au
,
W. W. L.
and
Herzing
,
D. L.
(
2003
).
Echolocation signals of wild Atlantic spotted dolphin (Stenella frontalis)
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
113
,
598
-
604
.
Au
,
W. W. L.
,
Floyd
,
R. W.
and
Haun
,
J. E.
(
1978
).
Propagation of Atlantic bottlenose dolphin echolocation signals
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
64
,
411
-
422
.
Au
,
W. W. L.
,
Moore
,
P. W. B.
and
Pawloski
,
D.
(
1986
).
Echolocation transmitting beam of the Atlantic bottlenose dolphin
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
80
,
688
-
691
.
Au
,
W. W. L.
,
Pawloski
,
J. L.
,
Nachtigall
,
P. E.
,
Blonz
,
M.
and
Gisner
,
R. C.
(
1995
).
Echolocation signals and transmission beam pattern of a false killer whale (Pseudorca crassidens)
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
98
,
51
-
59
.
Au
,
W. W. L.
,
Branstetter
,
B.
,
Moore
,
P. W.
and
Finneran
,
J. J.
(
2012
).
The biosonar field around an Atlantic bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
131
,
569
-
576
.
Beedholm
,
K.
and
Møhl
,
B.
(
2006
).
Directionality of sperm whale sonar clicks and its relation to piston radiation theory
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
119
,
EL14
-
EL19
.
Brinkløv
,
S.
,
Jakobsen
,
L.
,
Ratcliffe
,
J. M.
,
Kalko
,
E. K. V.
and
Surlykke
,
A.
(
2011
).
Echolocation call intensity and directionality in flying short-tailed fruit bats, Carollia perspicillata (Phyllostomidae)
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
129
,
427
-
435
.
Cranford
,
T. W.
,
Amundin
,
M.
and
Norris
,
K. S.
(
1996
).
Functional morphology and homology in the odontocete nasal complex: implications for sound generation
.
J. Morphol.
228
,
223
-
285
.
Cranford
,
T. W.
,
Trijoulet
,
V.
,
Smith
,
C. R.
and
Krysl
,
P.
(
2013
).
Validation of a vibroacoustic finite element model using bottlenose dolphin simulations: the dolphin biosonar beam is focused in stages
.
Bioacoustics
23
,
1
-
34
.
Efron
,
B.
(
1979
).
Bootstrap methods: another look at the Jackknife
.
Ann. Stat.
7
,
1
-
26
.
Efron
,
B.
(
1981
).
Nonparametric standard errors and confidence intervals
.
Can. J. Stat.
9
,
139
-
158
.
Efron
,
B.
(
1982
).
The Jackknife, the Bootstrap, and Other Resampling Plans
.
CBMS-NSF Regional Conference Series in Applied Mathematics
, Vol.
38
.
VT, USA
:
Capital City Press
.
Finneran
,
J. J.
,
Branstetter
,
B. K.
,
Houser
,
D. S.
,
Moore
,
P. W.
,
Mulsow
,
J.
,
Martin
,
C.
and
Perisho
,
S.
(
2014
).
High-resolution measurement of a bottlenose dolphin's (Tursiops truncatus) biosonar transmission beam pattern in the horizontal plane
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
136
,
2025
-
2038
.
Griffin
,
D. R.
(
1958
).
Listening in the Dark: The Acoustic Orientation of Bats and Men
.
New Haven, CT
:
Yale University Press
.
Huggenberger
,
S.
,
Rauschmann
,
M. A.
,
Vogl
,
T. J.
and
Oelschläger
,
H. H. A.
(
2009
).
Functional morphology of the nasal complex in the harbor porpoise (Phocoena phocoena L.)
.
Anat. Rec.
292
,
902
-
920
.
Jakobsen
,
L.
and
Surlykke
,
A.
(
2010
).
Vespertilionid bats control the width of their biosonar sound beam dynamically during prey pursuit
.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
107
,
13930
-
13935
.
Jakobsen
,
L.
,
Kalko
,
E. K. V.
and
Surlykke
,
A.
(
2012
).
Echolocation beam shape in emballonurid bats, Saccopteryx bilineata and Cormura brevirostris
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
66
,
1493
-
1502
.
Jakobsen
,
L.
,
Ratcliffe
,
J. M.
and
Surlykke
,
A.
(
2013
).
Convergent acoustic field of view in echolocating bats
.
Nature
493
,
93
-
96
.
Jensen
,
F. H.
,
Bejder
,
L.
,
Wahlberg
,
M.
and
Madsen
,
P. T.
(
2009
).
Biosonar adjustments to target range of echolocating bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.) in the wild
.
J. Exp. Biol.
212
,
1078
-
1086
.
Jensen
,
F. H.
,
Rocco
,
A.
,
Mansur
,
R. M.
,
Smith
,
B. D.
,
Janik
,
V. M.
and
Madsen
,
P. T.
(
2013
).
Clicking in shallow rivers: short-range echolocation of Irrawaddy and Ganges river dolphins in a shallow, acoustically complex habitat
.
PLoS ONE
8
,
e59284
.
Johnson
,
M.
,
Hickmott
,
L. S.
,
Soto
,
N. A.
and
Madsen
,
P. T.
(
2008
).
Echolocation behaviour adapted to prey in foraging Blainville's beaked whale (Mesoplodon densirostris)
.
Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.
275
,
133
-
139
.
Kalko
,
E. K. V.
(
1995
).
Insect pursuit, prey capture and echolocation in pipistrelle bats (Microchiroptera)
.
Anim. Behav.
50
,
861
-
880
.
Kalko
,
E. K. V.
and
Schnitzler
,
H.-U.
(
1993
).
Plasticity in echolocation signals of European pipistrelle bats in search flight: implications for habitat use and prey detection
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
33
,
415
-
428
.
Kloepper
,
L. N.
,
Nachtigall
,
P. E.
,
Donahue
,
M. J.
and
Breese
,
M.
(
2012
).
Active echolocation beam focusing in the false killer whale, Pseudorca crassidens
.
J. Exp. Biol.
215
,
1306
-
1312
.
Koblitz
,
J. C.
,
Wahlberg
,
M.
,
Stilz
,
P.
,
Madsen
,
P. T.
,
Beedholm
,
K.
and
Schnitzler
,
H.-U.
(
2012
).
Asymmetry and dynamics of a narrow sonar beam in an echolocating harbor porpoise
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
131
,
2315
-
2324
.
Kyhn
,
L. A.
,
Tougaard
,
J.
,
Jensen
,
F.
,
Wahlberg
,
M.
,
Stone
,
G.
,
Yoshinaga
,
A.
,
Beedholm
,
K.
and
Madsen
,
P. T.
(
2009
).
Feeding at a high pitch: source parameters of narrow band, high-frequency clicks from echolocating off-shore hourglass dolphins and coastal Hector's dolphins
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
125
,
1783
-
1791
.
Kyhn
,
L. A.
,
Jensen
,
F. H.
,
Beedholm
,
K.
,
Tougaard
,
J.
,
Hansen
,
M.
and
Madsen
,
P. T.
(
2010
).
Echolocation in sympatric Peale's dolphins (Lagenorhynchus australis) and Commerson's dolphins (Cephalorhynchus commersonii) producing narrow-band high-frequency clicks
.
J. Exp. Biol.
213
,
1940
-
1949
.
Kyhn
,
L. A.
,
Tougaard
,
J.
,
Beedholm
,
K.
,
Jensen
,
F. H.
,
Ashe
,
E.
,
Williams
,
R.
and
Madsen
,
P. T.
(
2013
).
Clicking in a killer whale habitat: narrow-band, high-frequency biosonar clicks of harbour porpoise (Phocoena phocoena) and Dall's porpoise (Phocoenoides dalli)
.
PLoS ONE
8
,
e63763
.
Madsen
,
P. T.
(
2005
).
Marine mammals and noise: problems with root mean square sound pressure levels for transients
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
117
,
3952
-
3957
.
Madsen
,
P. T.
and
Surlykke
,
A.
(
2013
).
Functional convergence in bat and toothed whale biosonars
.
Physiology
28
,
276
-
283
.
Madsen
,
P. T.
and
Wahlberg
,
M.
(
2007
).
Recording and quantification of ultrasonic echolocation clicks from free-ranging toothed whales
.
Deep Sea Res.
54
,
1421
-
1444
.
Madsen
,
P. T.
,
Kerr
,
I.
and
Payne
,
R.
(
2004
).
Echolocation clicks of two free-ranging, oceanic delphinids with different food preferences: false killer whales Pseudorca crassidens and Risso's dolphins Grampus griseus
.
J. Exp. Biol.
207
,
1811
-
1823
.
Madsen
,
P. T.
,
Wilson
,
M.
,
Johnson
,
M.
,
Hanlon
,
R. T.
,
Bocconcelli
,
A.
,
de Soto
,
N. A.
and
Tyack
,
P. L.
(
2007
).
Clicking for calamari: toothed whales can echolocate squid Loligo pealeii
.
Aquat. Biol.
1
,
141
-
150
.
Madsen
,
P. T.
,
Wisniewska
,
D.
and
Beedholm
,
K.
(
2010
).
Single source sound production and dynamic beam formation in echolocating harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena)
.
J. Exp. Biol.
213
,
3105
-
3110
.
Madsen
,
P. T.
,
de Soto
,
N. A.
,
Arranz
,
P.
and
Johnson
,
M.
(
2013a
).
Echolocation in Blainville's beaked whales (Mesoplodon densirostris)
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol.
199
,
451
-
469
.
Madsen
,
P. T.
,
Lammers
,
M.
,
Wisniewska
,
D.
and
Beedholm
,
K.
(
2013b
).
Nasal sound production in echolocating delphinids (Tursiops truncatus and Pseudorca crassidens) is dynamic, but unilateral: clicking on the right side and whistling on the left side
.
J. Exp. Biol.
216
,
4091
-
4102
.
Manly
,
B. F. J.
(
1997
).
Randomization, Bootstrap and Monte Carlo Methods in Biology
.
Boca Raton
:
Chapman & Hall
.
Møhl
,
B.
,
Wahlberg
,
M.
,
Madsen
,
P. T.
,
Miller
,
L. A.
and
Surlykke
,
A.
(
2000
).
Sperm whale clicks: directionality and source level revisited
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
107
,
638
-
648
.
Moore
,
P.
and
Patterson
,
S.
(
1983
).
Behavior control of echolocation source level in the dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)
.
Proceedings of the Fifth Biennial Conference on the Biology of Marine Mammals
.
Boston, MA
:
The Society for Marine Mammalogy
.
Moore
,
P. W. B.
and
Pawloski
,
D. A.
(
1990
).
Investigations on the control of echolocation pulses in the dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)
. In
Sensory Abilities of Cetaceans: Laboratory and Field Evidence
(ed.
J. A.
Thomas
and
R. A.
Kastelein
), pp.
305
-
316
.
New York
:
Plenum Press
.
Moore
,
P. W.
,
Dankiewicz
,
L. A.
and
Houser
,
D. S.
(
2008
).
Beamwidth control and angular target detection in an echolocating bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
124
,
3324
-
3332
.
Moss
,
C. F.
(
2010
).
Probing the natural scene by echolocation in bats
.
Front. Behav. Neurosci.
4
,
33
.
Moss
,
C. F.
and
Surlykke
,
A.
(
2001
).
Auditory scene analysis by echolocation in bats
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
110
,
2207
-
2226
.
Moss
,
C. F.
,
Chiu
,
C.
and
Surlykke
,
A.
(
2011
).
Adaptive vocal behavior drives perception by echolocation in bats
.
Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.
21
,
645
-
652
.
Norris
,
K. S.
and
Harvey
,
G. W.
(
1972
).
A theory for the function of the spermaceti organ of the sperm whale (Physeter catodon L.)
. In
Animal Orientation and Navigation
(ed.
Galler
,
S. R.
,
Schmidt-Koenig
,
K.
,
Jacobs
,
G. J.
and
elleville
R. E.
), pp.
397
-
417
.
Washington, DC
:
Science and Technology office, NASA
.
Rasmussen
,
M. H.
,
Wahlberg
,
M.
and
Miller
,
L. A.
(
2004
).
Estimated transmission beam pattern of clicks recorded from free-ranging white-beaked dolphins (Lagenorhynchus albirostris)
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
116
,
1826
-
1831
.
Schevill
,
W. E.
and
McBride
,
A. F.
(
1956
).
Evidence for echolocation by cetaceans
.
Deep Sea Res.
3
,
153
-
154
.
Schotten
,
M.
,
Au
,
W. W. L.
,
Lammers
,
M. O.
and
Aubauer
,
R.
(
2004
).
Echolocation recordings and localization of wild spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris) and pantropical spotted dolphins (S. attenuata) using a four hydrophone array
. In
Echolocation in Bats and Dolphins
(ed.
J. A.
Thomas
,
C. F.
Moss
and
M. M.
Vater
), pp.
393
-
400
.
Chicago
:
University of Chicago Press
.
Shaffer
,
J. W.
,
Moretti
,
D.
,
Jarvis
,
S.
,
Tyack
,
P.
and
Johnson
,
M.
(
2013
).
Effective beam pattern of the Blainville's beaked whale (Mesoplodon densirostris) and implications for passive acoustic monitoring
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
133
,
1770
-
1784
.
Spiesberger
,
J. L.
and
Fristrup
,
K. M.
(
1990
).
Passive localization of calling animals and sensing of their acoustic environment using acoustic tomography
.
Am. Nat.
135
,
107
-
153
.
Strother
,
G. K.
and
Mogus
,
M.
(
1970
).
Acoustical beam patterns for bats: some theoretical considerations
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
48
,
1430
-
1432
.
Surlykke
,
A.
,
Boel Pedersen
,
S.
and
Jakobsen
,
L.
(
2009
).
Echolocating bats emit a highly directional sonar sound beam in the field
.
Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.
276
,
853
-
860
.
Urick
,
R. J.
(
1983
).
Principles of Underwater Sound
.
Los Altos
:
Peninsula
.
Wahlberg
,
M.
,
Møhl
,
B.
and
Madsen
,
P. T.
(
2001
).
Estimating source position accuracy of a large-aperture hydrophone array for bioacoustics
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
109
,
397
-
406
.
Wahlberg
,
M.
,
Beedholm
,
K.
,
Heerfordt
,
A.
and
Møhl
,
B.
(
2011a
).
Characteristics of biosonar signals from the northern bottlenose whale, Hyperoodon ampullatus
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
130
,
3077
-
3084
.
Wahlberg
,
M.
,
Jensen
,
F. H.
,
Soto
,
N. A.
,
Beedholm
,
K.
,
Bejder
,
L.
,
Oliveira
,
C.
,
Rasmussen
,
M.
,
Simon
,
M.
,
Villadsgaard
,
A.
and
Madsen
,
P. T.
(
2011b
).
Source parameters of echolocation clicks from wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus and Tursiops truncatus)
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
130
,
2263
-
2274
.
Wisniewska
,
D. M.
,
Johnson
,
M.
,
Beedholm
,
K.
,
Wahlberg
,
M.
and
Madsen
,
P. T.
(
2012
).
Acoustic gaze adjustments during active target selection in echolocating porpoises
.
J. Exp. Biol.
215
,
4358
-
4373
.
Wisniewska
,
D. M.
,
Ratcliffe
,
J. M.
,
Beedholm
,
K.
,
Christensen
,
C. B.
,
Johnson
,
M.
,
Koblitz
,
J. C.
,
Wahlberg
,
M.
and
Madsen
,
P. T.
(
2015
).
Range-dependent flexibility in the acoustic field of view of echolocating porpoises (Phocoena phocoena)
.
eLife
2015
; doi:10.7554/eLife.05651
Wood
,
F. G.
(
1964
).
Discussion
. In
Marine Bio-acoustics
. Vol.
II
(ed.
W.
Tavolga
), pp.
395
-
396
.
Oxford
:
Pergamon
.
Zimmer
,
W. M. X.
,
Johnson
,
M. P.
,
Madsen
,
P. T.
and
Tyack
,
P. L.
(
2005
).
Echolocation clicks of free-ranging Cuvier's beaked whales (Ziphius cavirostris)
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
117
,
3919
-
3927
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

Supplementary information