ABSTRACT
Glyphosate (GLY) is a herbicide that is widely used in agriculture for weed control. Although reports about the impact of GLY in snails, crustaceans and amphibians exist, few studies have investigated its sublethal effects in non-target organisms such as the honeybee Apis mellifera, the main pollen vector in commercial crops. Here, we tested whether exposure to three sublethal concentrations of GLY (2.5, 5 and 10 mg l−1: corresponding to 0.125, 0.250 and 0.500 μg per animal) affects the homeward flight path of honeybees in an open field. We performed an experiment in which forager honeybees were trained to an artificial feeder, and then captured, fed with sugar solution containing traces of GLY and released from a novel site either once or twice. Their homeward trajectories were tracked using harmonic radar technology. We found that honeybees that had been fed with solution containing 10 mg l−1 GLY spent more time performing homeward flights than control bees or bees treated with lower concentrations. They also performed more indirect homing flights. Moreover, the proportion of direct homeward flights performed after a second release from the same site increased in control bees but not in treated bees. These results suggest that, in honeybees, exposure to levels of GLY commonly found in agricultural settings impairs the cognitive capacities needed to retrieve and integrate spatial information for a successful return to the hive. Therefore, honeybee navigation is affected by ingesting traces of the most widely used herbicide worldwide, with potential long-term negative consequences for colony foraging success.
INTRODUCTION
Honeybees (Apis mellifera) are the main pollinators in agricultural settings (Aizen et al., 2009) and as such are highly exposed to any perturbation occurring in the surroundings of crop fields. Consequently, this eusocial insect can serve as a biosensor to accurately determine environmental pollutants (Devillers and Pham-Delègue, 2002). Any foreign substance present in gathered resources (i.e. pollen and nectar) may also be stored and accumulated inside the nest for long periods, potentially affecting nest mates of all stages (Devillers and Pham-Delègue, 2002). This applies in particular to highly water-soluble agrochemicals such as the herbicide glyphosate N-(phosphonomethyl) glycine, which may remain on crops after application for long periods (Zhang et al., 2011). Any subsequent accumulation of agrochemicals inside the hive could have negative effects which are often inconspicuous in the short term (Giesy et al., 2000), but which could impair individual behaviors and social organization in the long term (Kirchner, 1999).
The use of glyphosate (GLY) as a broad-spectrum post-emergent herbicide for weed control has spread rapidly in the last few decades (Goldsborough and Brown, 1988) to become one of the most commonly used agrochemicals worldwide (Zhang et al., 2011). The typical methods of administration involve spraying it directly onto foliage and aerial application (Giesy et al., 2000). As a consequence, traces of the herbicide can also be found in the surroundings of fields cultivated with the target crop. GLY deters plant growth by inhibiting an aromatic amino acid pathway that is apparently present only in plants, microorganisms and fungi, not animals (Amrhein et al., 1980; Carlisle and Trevors, 1988; Duke et al., 1989; Franz et al., 1997).
Several studies have reported negative effects of this herbicide on vertebrates and invertebrates. GLY doses between 0.1 and 10 mg acid equivalent l−1 have been found to reduce growth in the earthworm Aporrectoden caliginom (Springett and Gray, 1992) and affect reproduction and development in the freshwater snail Pseudosuccinea columella (Tate et al., 1997). A negative effect has also been reported in amphibians after chronic exposure to different concentrations of glyphosate (3.8–18 mg l−1; Howe et al., 2004; Relyea, 2005a,b). Despite these findings and others that report negative and lethal effects on invertebrates such as amphipods (Dutra et al., 2011), the sublethal impacts of GLY on non-target organisms such as insect pollinators have so far been poorly evaluated (Herbert et al., 2014; Thompson et al., 2014). In this study, we used sublethal concentrations of GLY ranging from 2.5 to 10 mg l−1.
Honeybees show a behavioral repertoire that allows the evaluation of perturbations in well-known stereotypical responses. The behavior in which bees protrude their probosces after being stimulated by applying sucrose solution to their antennae is one of these responses, and it can be used to test the effects of environmental pollutants on appetitive behavior (Devillers and Pham-Delègue, 2002). A recent study found that a concentration of glyphosate (2.5 mg l−1), within the recommended range for aquatic and terrestrial weed control (Giesy et al., 2000), affects gustatory responsiveness and learning performance in harnessed bees [tested with proboscis extension response (PER) assays]. However, no effect was observed on locomotive activity when foragers collected sucrose solution contaminated with the herbicide at an artificial feeder, suggesting that GLY may accumulate inside the hive (Herbert et al., 2014). Also, Herbert and co-workers (2014) found that an acute exposure to sublethal GLY concentrations offered during olfactory PER conditioning decreased short-term memory and impaired more complex forms of associative learning in foragers.
Studies have already shown that other agrochemical compounds used for pest control, such as neonicotinoids, negatively affect honeybee gustatory sensitivity and even their dance maneuvers (Eiri and Nieh, 2012). Non-lethal doses of imidacloprid (75–1000 ppb), a neonicotinoid insecticide, which acts on cholinergic pathways of insect synaptic transmission (Gauthier, 2010), affect homing abilities (Bortolotti et al., 2003) and impair the retrieval of memory acquired during exploratory orientation flights (Fischer et al., 2014).
Honeybees are well established as a model for studies on animal navigation (von Frisch, 1967; Menzel et al., 2012; Menzel, 2012). In a typical catch-and-release experiment, bees are displaced within a previously explored area to evaluate their homing behavior using different tracking technologies (Decourtye et al., 2011; Schneider et al., 2012; Fischer et al., 2014). Exploration during orientation flights familiarizes bees with the sun compass, their distance measure (odometer) and the landmarks in the environment (Menzel et al., 2005, 2012). Further information about the landscape is added during flights between the hive and the feeding sites. Integration of the multiple sources of spatial information leads to a reference memory that allows bees to perform shortcuts between important locations (hive, feeding sites and release sites). As a result, honeybees are able to refer to a common frame of spatial reference that allows them to return to the hive even from an unfamiliar location by taking novel shortcuts (Menzel et al., 1998).
Although the GLY concentrations recommended for weed control as well as those previously detected in aquatic and agricultural systems are within the range of 1.4 mg l−1 to 3.7 mg l−1 (Couture et al., 1995; Giesy et al., 2000; Solomon and Thompson, 2003), intensive use of the herbicide during the last two decades has led to an exponential increase in the doses of GLY present in genetically modified (GM) crops (USDA data source, NASS). This situation implies that GLY concentrations found in close proximity to GM crops today should be much higher than the range previously reported. In the present study we propose that honeybees foraging on ‘nectar’ containing traces of GLY may have difficulty integrating complex information from their environment which they need for navigation. To evaluate whether sublethal doses of glyphosate affect Apis mellifera orientation and navigation, we performed a catch-and-release experiment in which honeybees flying to the hive were displaced during foraging trips.
RESULTS
In a catch-and-release experiment as performed here, we expect that bees captured at the feeder and then released from the release site (RS) are motivated to return to the hive. After ingesting food contaminated with glyphosate, we expected that these treated bees would perform irregular homeward flights or at least take more time than untreated control bees to return to the hive. Our results show that animals either start immediately with a straight flight from the release site (Fig. 1A,B) or they perform less regular flights (Fig. 1C). Some of the straight flights follow the vector the bees would have taken if they had not been relocated to the release site. These flights were either directed towards the hive and finished at the hive, or they were directed towards the feeder and then followed the trained route from the feeder to the hive (Fig. 1A). Some of these initially straight flights at the beginning of their homing behavior were followed by a single loop before the bees return to the hive (Fig. 1B). Therefore, we distinguish between two major flight categories: direct flights (straight flight with or without one loop, Fig. 1A,B) and indirect flights (flights with loops Fig. 1C).
First release
Fig. 2 shows the proportion of bees performing different homeward flights after being relocated from the feeder to the RS and released from the RS for the first time. As already mentioned, these homeward paths involve: (1) straight and rapid flights directly to the hive, with or without a single loop before returning to the hive (‘direct flights’); or (2) irregular flights, in which bees changed direction frequently (‘indirect flights’). Both control and treated bees showed similar proportions of direct flights to the hive (test of heterogeneity: χ2=2.604; P=0.457; N=79). However, we found statistical differences in the time spent performing direct flights between treatments (Fig. 3A; Kruskal–Wallis test: H=10.008, P=0.019, d.f.=3, N=50). Specifically, bees that had ingested sucrose solution containing 10 mg l−1 GLY spent more time flying from the RS to the hive than control bees or bees that had ingested 2.5 or 5 mg l−1 GLY (Mann–Whitney test: 0 mg l−1 versus 10 mg l−1: U=28.5, P=0.004; 2.5 mg l−1 versus 10 mg l−1: U=13.5, P=0.016; 5 mg l−1 versus 10 mg l−1: U=8.0, P=0.003). No statistical difference in the flight time was found between control and treated bees performing indirect flights (Fig. 3B; Kruskal–Wallis test: H=5.197, P=0.158, d.f.=3, N=29).
We observed that during some homeward flights a small number of bees passed through the feeder area. The proportion of bees that flew via the feeder was higher among control bees and bees that ingested sucrose solution with 2.5 mg l−1 GLY than among bees treated with 5 or 10 mg l−1 (see Table 1). After flying close to the feeder, those bees followed the trained flight route to the hive.
Second release
Bees learn to improve their homing flights during sequential releases from the same site (Menzel et al., 2005). Therefore, we next asked whether this form of learning is compromised in bees that have been exposed to the herbicide. To test this, bees were captured at the feeder, relocated to the RS, and released for a second time: these bees were therefore exposed twice to the same amount of GLY.
Control bees and bees that were exposed to 2.5 or 5 mg l−1 GLY showed a tendency to perform direct flights more frequently than indirect flights (Fig. 4). Conversely, bees that had ingested sucrose solution with 10 mg l−1 GLY showed the inverse tendency, with more bees performing indirect flights. Nevertheless, no statistical differences for the time spent in direct flights were found between control and treated bees (Fig. 5A; Kruskal–Wallis test: H=3.332, P=0.343, d.f.=3, N=27). It was not possible to perform a statistical analysis of data for indirect flights (Fig. 5B) because the sample size was too small (0 mg l−1: N=4; 2.5 mg l−1: N=1; 5 mg l−1: N=2; 10 mg l−1: N=3). When we compared the proportion of control and treated bees that performed direct and indirect flights after the first and second release, we found statistical differences between control bees released once or twice, but not between treated bees (Fig. 6A; Fisher's exact test: control bees, χ2=10.80; P=0.001; treated bees, χ2=1.07; P=0.245, N=32). Control bees modified their tendency to perform more indirect flights after the first release than after the second one, whereas the proportion of treated bees performing direct or indirect flights after one or two releases was similar. Furthermore, when studying the transitions (or lack thereof) from direct or indirect flights (or vice versa) performed after the first release to direct or indirect flights performed after the second release (direct–direct: D–D, direct–indirect: D–I, indirect–direct: I–D and indirect–indirect: I–I), we observed a tendency for control bees to perform more I–D transitions than treated bees. Interestingly, bees that had ingested the higher GLY concentration showed a tendency to perform more transitions to indirect flights (D–I, I–I) after the second release (Fig. 6B).
DISCUSSION
We evaluated the effect of recommended concentrations of glyphosate (GLY) used in agricultural settings on honeybee navigation (up to 3.7 mg l−1 GLY; Giesy et al., 2000) and two additional concentrations that are reported to be sublethal (5 and 10 mg l−1). Our results show that a single exposure to a concentration of GLY within this range delays the return of the foraging honeybee to the hive. In some cases, the flight trajectories were also affected after successive exposure to the herbicide, suggesting that the spatial learning process is impaired by ingestion of the herbicide during feeding. This impairment of navigation in the explored area increased when the concentration of GLY ingested was higher. Indeed, bees fed with 10 mg l−1 GLY took more time to perform direct homeward flights and performed more indirect flights after the second release than bees treated with lower GLY concentrations. Bees that had ingested low concentrations of GLY (2.5 or 5 mg l−1) and showed indirect flight trajectories after the first release performed direct flights after the second release. Accordingly, more experimental honeybees found the hive regardless of the herbicide concentration ingested. However, subtle effects on the homing behavior within this concentration range were seen, indicating that the GLY concentrations used in this study caused only sublethal effects on honeybees.
Regarding the kind of flight trajectories performed, we found that honeybees treated with GLY exhibited more indirect homing flights after the second release than the control bees. As reported by Menzel et al. (2005), we expected that the bees released more than once from the same location improve their homeward flights. This means that we expected a lower proportion of bees to execute indirect flights from RS to H after the second release. Our results show that a higher proportion of control bees did indeed perform indirect flights during the first release and changed to direct flights during the second one, whereas animals treated with the highest dose of GLY were impaired in terms of improving their navigation performance. Bees released twice from the RS have fed on the contaminated food twice, a fact that might promote physiological stress and/or learning impairment. We propose that both a single exposure and repeated exposures to GLY have an effect on the retrieval and formation of memory. The effect of GLY on memory retrieval is indicated by the reduced probability of bees taking a shortcut to the hive or the feeder, longer search flights and a lack of improvement of homing behavior after experience.
A recent study using the PER paradigm showed that acute exposure to GLY (2.5 mg l−1) affect the retention of olfactory memory in honeybees evaluated in both simple and complex associative learning tasks. The learning process for both kinds of paradigm is faster for untreated bees and, specifically for a kind of negative pattern learning, in the presence of GLY in the reward (Herbert et al., 2014). Navigation requires several rather complex cognitive capacities during memory formation and retrieval that allow them to integrate current and previously acquired environmental information. These processes would be compromised by the uptake of higher concentrations of GLY used, as we show for 5 (Fig. 6) and 10 mg l−1 (Figs 3, 4 and 6) concentrations. A plausible explanation for this response is that the herbicide impairs appetitive behaviors, disturbing not only those processes involved in acquiring and associating chemosensory information, as proposed in a previous study (Herbert et al., 2014), but also the use of stored information about the environment acquired during the exploratory orientation flights of foragers and the experience gained from homing flights over the course of the experiment. Thus, feeding on nectar containing traces of GLY might affect the learning and retrieval of memory relevant for the recognition of food sources and for navigating between those food sources and the hive.
The ingestion of specific insecticides in sublethal concentrations increases sugar response thresholds (Eiri and Nieh, 2012) and affects homing in honeybees (Henry et al., 2012; Fischer et al., 2014). Herbert and co-workers (2014) have shown that chronic exposure to traces of GLY reduces responsiveness to sucrose and learning performance during olfactory PER conditioning. Furthermore, when honeybees were exposed to high levels of this herbicide they showed impaired associative learning, but no clear effects on their dancing behavior were observed (Herbert et al., 2014). These data support the view that exposure to GLY, even at low concentrations, negatively affects gustatory responsiveness and thus also the motivation to forage for food in free-flying honeybees in the experiments reported here. This motivational effect, however, was not strong enough to eliminate homing behavior but appeared to reduce the acquisition of new navigational memory.
The experiment performed here focused on the action of GLY over a short period of time (hours) but chronic exposure to the herbicide could have additional effects and may affect the general performance of the entire colony. Usually, genetically modified herbicide-tolerant crop fields are surrounded by native flora (Bohan et al., 2005). As we mentioned above, honeybees are the main pollinator in agricultural ecosystems, but they also play a key role in pollination of native flora (Aizen et al., 2009). As a consequence of GLY application in those agricultural crops and its drift (Chang et al., 2011) to neighboring areas, the native species in the surrounding areas could be affected (Matthews, 2006), as well as their pollinators. Moreover, in countries that have introduced glyphosate-resistant GM crops, traces of GLY were detected in honey (Chile: CIAP, 2012; Rubio et al., 2014), air particles and rain samples (USA: Chang et al., 2011; Argentina: Alonso et al., 2014) and in the surface of bodies of water located close to treated fields that could be visited by honeybees (Canada: CCME, 1989). In addition, we focus on agricultural settings and their surroundings – a system that includes the wild flora. The presence of GM crops in some countries where monoculture is common is often linked to the use of aerial spraying to inoculate pesticides, a situation that promotes drift of agrochemicals to non-target areas (Matthews, 2006; Chang et al., 2011). Moreover, herbicides are used beyond the surroundings of commercial crops; nowadays, its scope has reached domestic use in homes and gardens (Matthews, 2006), where honeybees can potentially collect food resources.
As the resistance of organisms to agrochemicals increases, higher concentrations are used to treat agricultural crops (ARMS, 2014), and pollinators like the honeybee will be exposed to higher concentrations. Thus, higher proportions of ‘disoriented’ foragers could decrease foraging efficiency, leading to a reduction in the honeybee population. Such effects have been seen in neonicotinoid treatments (Henry et al., 2012; Schneider et al., 2012; Fischer et al., 2014). Schneider and co-workers (2012) recorded a significant reduction in the number of honeybees visiting the food source and returning to the hive after the exposure to imidacloprid and clothianidin. Moreover, bees spent longer periods inside the hive before restarting the foraging process to the food source. As a consequence of this impairment, the foraging efficiency of the colony as a whole might be affected.
The concentrations of herbicide used in our study were based on recommended levels for spraying fields and levels measured in natural environments (0 to 3.7 mg l−1 range; Couture et al., 1995; Mann and Bidwell, 1999; Giesy et al., 2000; Perkins et al., 2000; Solomon and Thompson, 2003), even though higher concentrations have not been previously measured in the environment, they were selected to represent a potential worst-case exposure scenario that a pollinator could encounter while foraging in flowers located within or outside the GM crops (Chang et al., 2011). Interestingly, the locomotive activity of bees tested in our study was not impaired after the incubation phase and they did not reject the sucrose solution offered, whether with or without GLY. As a result, honeybees continued foraging at our feeding station and thus also on plants that expose bees to similar GLY concentrations, and the contaminated nectar or pollen could be brought back by honeybees to the hive and would then accumulate there. Rubio and co-workers (2014) found traces of glyphosate in both organic (26–93 ppb, mean 50 ppb) and non-organic (17–163 ppb, mean 66 ppb) honey samples from several countries. Moreover, they found the presence of GLY traces in honey samples made by bees feeding on wild and melliferous flora. Although the amounts they reported are lower than the GLY concentration that we used in this study, it does not mean that this was representative of those concentrations the forager bees are exposed to in the field. We expect that some bees could find the concentrations of GLY that we used in our experiment in food and would then take it back to the hive. With this in mind, further studies in GLY-exposed commercial crops and their surroundings are necessary to evaluate the actual exposure of forager bees to the herbicide, and the relationship between the concentration of GLY collected by the honeybees in exposed environments and the traces found in the stored honey or pollen.
Despite the lack of data on the actual level of GLY that forager honeybees are exposed to in the field, present results show that exposure to non-lethal concentrations of glyphosate causes sub-lethal effects, which modify the bees’ foraging behavior. However, further studies are necessary to evaluate to what extent this chemical influences foraging behavior of honeybees in a natural environment and whether prolonged exposure to this herbicide might contribute to worsen the health status of beehives. Since GM herbicide-tolerant crop fields are usually surrounded by native flora that is visited by honeybees, it would also be necessary to analyze traces of glyphosate present in collected and stored honey and pollen, as well as in larvae and adult bees from hives located in the surroundings of agricultural crops treated with GLY, before and after the herbicide application.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Animals and study site
We used bees from a colony of approximately 30,000 bees (Apis mellifera Linnaeus 1758). The experiment was conducted from August to September of 2013 in an open field (N 50°48′53.01″, E 8°52′21.36″) located close to the village of Großseelheim (Hessen), Germany.
Experimental procedure
A group of forager bees was trained to collect unscented 0.5 mol l−1 sucrose solution from an artificial feeder located 400 m north of the hive and fitted with colored number tags on the thorax for individual identification. At 15 min intervals, numbered bees were captured individually at the feeder before they began to ingest the sucrose solution offered and were immediately confined in plastic tubes, and transported to the release site (RS) located 460 m east of the feeder location. The RS was located within the area explored during orientation flights, but otherwise it was novel for the trained bees. Each plastic tube contained a small feeder providing 50 µl of unscented 2 mol l−1 sucrose solution, either with or without glyphosate. The tube was kept in a dark box for 1 hour (incubation), allowing bees to ingest all the solution offered. Three different concentrations of GLY were used (diluted in 2 mol l−1 sucrose solution, see next section for more details): 2.5 mg l−1, 5 mg l−1 and 10 mg l−1. Control bees were handled in the same way but were fed the solution without herbicide (0 mg l−1).
After incubation, a radar transponder was glued to the number tag fixed on the thorax of each bee and the homeward flight trajectory (from the RS to the hive) was tracked with harmonic radar. Bees were released at 15 min intervals to ensure the same incubation time for all the individuals. One experimenter at the radar station passed on information about the flight trajectories of the released bees by walkie-talkie. Once the bee arrived at the hive, it was caught, the transponder was removed, and then the honeybee was allowed to enter the hive. Whenever possible, these bees were captured at the feeder and released from the RS once more (‘second release’) in order to test whether learning during homing flights was compromised. The total number of bees tested was 108 for the first release, and 44 for the second. The number of flight trajectories obtained was 79 for first release and 37 for the second (see Tables 1 and 2).
We measured the following variables: capture time, release time, arrival time at the hive and the flight trajectory recorded with the harmonic radar. If a bee was observed on the radar but then disappeared from the radar range and was not seen arriving at the hive, it was classified as a non-arriving bee.
Herbicide
A stock solution of glyphosate (Glyphosate PESTANAL, Sigma-Aldrich, Steinheim, Germany) at a concentration of 100 mg acid equivalent l−1 was prepared with distilled water and kept refrigerated. New stock solution was prepared every 7 days. The stock solution was diluted in sucrose solution 2 mol l−1 to obtain the different GLY concentrations used in the experimental procedure. The concentrations of herbicide used were: 0 mg (control), 2.5 mg, 5 mg and 10 mg of glyphosate per liter of sucrose solution. Each bee ingested 50 µl of 2 mol l−1 sucrose solution with or without GLY, so the concentrations used were equivalent to the following doses: 0 ng, 125 ng, 250 ng and 500 ng of glyphosate per bee.
Harmonic radar tracking
Tracking bees with a harmonic radar system is described in Riley et al. (1996, 2005), Menzel et al. (2011) and Scheiner et al. (2013). We used a system with a sending unit which consisted of a 9.4 GHz radar transceiver (Raytheon Marine GmbH, Kiel, NSC 2525/7 XU) combined with a parabolic antenna of ∼44 dBi that provided a signal from the transponder on the bee thorax every 3 s. The transponder consisted of a dipole antenna with a Low Barrier Schottky Diode HSCH-5340 of centered inductivity. The second harmonic component of the signal (18.8 GHz) was the target for the radar. The receiving unit consisted of an 18.8 GHz parabolic antenna with a low-noise preamplifier directly coupled to a mixer (18.8 GHz oscillator) and a downstream amplifier with a 90 MHz ZF-Filter. The transponder was made of a silver wire with a diameter of 0.3 mm, a length of 11 mm, a weight of 10.5 mg and a loop inductance of 1.3 nH. The range of the harmonic radar had a radius of 900 m.
Statistical analysis
A heterogeneity chi-square analysis was used to compare the proportion of bees performing direct or indirect flights from the release site back to the hive. A Kruskal–Wallis test was performed to compare the time bees spent between the RS and the hive, according to the treatments (control bees and bees exposed to GLY: 2.5 mg l−1, 5 mg l−1 and 10 mg l−1). To compare the proportion of bees performing direct or indirect flights according to whether bees were released once or twice, we applied Fisher's exact test (Sokal and Rohlf, 1995).
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to Hanna Zwaka for her help during the field assays, and Anne Carney for her corrections on the final version of the manuscript. We also thank K. Lukowiak and two anonymous referees for their valuable comments and suggestions on an earlier version of this manuscript.
Footnotes
Author contributions
M.S.B., R.M. and W.M.F. conceived and designed the experiments. M.S.B., U.G. M.-L.H., L.T. and R.M. performed the experiments. M.S.B., R.M. and W.M.F. performed data analysis. M.S.B., R.M. and W.M.F. drafted the manuscript. All authors revised and commented on the manuscript.
Funding
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.
References
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing or financial interests.