The capacity of animals to dissipate heat may constrain sustained energy intake during lactation. We examined these constraints at peak lactation in MF1 mice that had ad libitum access to food, or that had to run a pre-set target on running wheels to obtain ad libitum access to food. The voluntary distance run decreased sharply during pregnancy and peak lactation. When lactating females were provided with 80% of their estimated food requirements, and had to run pre-set distances of 2, 4 or 6 km before given access to additional ad libitum food, most of them did not complete the running target during late lactation and the mice with the highest targets failed to reach their targets earlier in lactation. There were consequently significant group differences in asymptotic food intake (2 km, 16.97±0.40 g day−1; 4 km, 14.29±0.72 g day−1; and 6 km, 12.65±0.45 g day−1) and weaned litter masses (2 km, 71.11±2.39 g; 4 km, 54.63±4.28 g and 6 km, 47.18±2.46 g). When the females did run sufficiently to gain ad libitum food access, their intake did not differ between the different distance groups or from controls that were not required to run. Thus, despite being physically capable of running the distances, mice could not exercise sufficiently in lactation to gain regular ad libitum access to food, probably because of the risks of hyperthermia when combining heat production from exercise with thermogenesis from lactation.

The maximal rates of energy metabolism that animals can sustain for protracted periods provide an upper limit constraining many aspects of animal performance (Drent and Daan, 1980; Hammond and Diamond, 1997; Piersma and van Gils, 2011; Ruf and Grafl, 2010; Speakman, 2000; Speakman, 2008; Thompson, 1992; Weiner, 1992). Maximal rates of energy intake and expenditure are probably limited by aspects of an animal's physiology (Hammond and Diamond, 1997; Kirkwood, 1983; Koteja, 1996; Perrigo, 1987; Speakman et al., 2004; Speakman and Król, 2005; Speakman and Król, 2010a). Late lactation is the most energetically demanding period during the life cycle of altricial mammals (Gittleman and Thompson, 1988; Millar, 1977; Thompson, 1992) and has been increasingly used as a model for testing potential factors that limit sustained energy intake and expenditure (Hammond and Diamond, 1992; Hammond et al., 1994; Hammond et al., 1996; Hammond and Diamond, 1997; Johnson and Speakman, 2001; Johnson et al., 2001a; Johnson et al., 2001b; Johnson et al., 2001c; Król and Speakman, 2003a; Król and Speakman, 2003b; Król et al., 2003; Król et al., 2007; Piersma and van Gils 2011; Speakman and McQueenie, 1996; Speakman et al., 2001; Valencak and Ruf, 2009; Valencak et al., 2010; Wu et al., 2009; Zhang and Wang, 2007; Zhao and Cao, 2009a; Zhao et al., 2010). Sustained energy intake (SusEI) during late lactation has been previously suggested to be limited centrally by the energy-supplying machinery, i.e. the alimentary tract and associated organs, which has been called the ‘central limitation hypothesis’ (Denis et al., 2003; Drent and Daan, 1980; Drummond et al., 2000; Hackländer et al., 2002; Hammond and Diamond, 1992; Hammond and Diamond, 1994; Hammond et al., 1996; Hammond and Diamond, 1997; Johnson et al., 2001a; Johnson et al., 2001c; Kirkwood, 1983; Knight et al., 1986; Koteja, 1996; Meyer et al., 1985; Perrigo, 1987; Rogowitz, 1998; Schubert et al., 2009; Speakman et al., 2001; Hammond and Diamond, 1997; Kirkwood, 1983; Weiner, 1992). This idea was consistent with observations that mice given additional tasks to perform during peak lactation, such as running for their food (Perrigo, 1987; Schubert et al., 2009), being simultaneously pregnant when lactating (Johnson et al., 2001c) and being given extra pups to raise (Hammond et al., 1996; Johnson et al., 2001a; Knight et al., 1986; Zhao and Cao, 2009a) did not elevate their food intake or milk production above the levels observed in unmanipulated females: perhaps reflecting the central processing capacity of the alimentary tract. Failure to increase milk production when given more pups to raise has also been reported in laboratory rats (Denis et al., 2003), cotton rats (Sigmodon hispidus) (Rogowitz, 1998), rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) (Drummond et al., 2000) and dogs (Canis familiaris) (Meyer et al., 1985). Moreover, manipulations of the energy density of the food were also consistent with a central processing limit in the alimentary tract (Hackländer et al., 2002; Speakman et al., 2001).

The notion of a central processing limit, however, has been disproved by repeated observations that when lactating rodents are exposed to the cold they are able to elevate their food intake above levels that at room temperature appeared to be limiting on performance (Hammond et al., 1994; Hammond and Kristan, 2000; Johnson and Speakman, 2001; Rogowitz, 1998; Zhang and Wang, 2007). It was consequently suggested that the sustained energy intake may rather reflect the summed requirements of the energy-consuming machinery, each of which may be working to capacity in different circumstances. Hence, in lactation the mammary glands may be working at capacity. Manipulations, such as increasing the number of pups, therefore did not result in elevated food intake because the female could not translate this extra intake into more milk. However, when lactating females were exposed to cold they did increase their intake because they could utilise the extra ingested energy to facilitate thermoregulation. This idea was called the ‘peripheral limitation hypothesis’. Hammond and colleagues (Hammond et al., 1996) provided strong experimental support for this hypothesis by surgically reducing the number of mammary glands in lactating mice, and demonstrating that the residual tissue was unable to compensate by increasing milk production, confirming the mammary glands were indeed working at capacity. This hypothesis was further confirmed by observations that in the cold some lactating rodents did not significantly elevate their milk production (Rogowitz, 1998; Zhao and Cao, 2009a; Zhao et al., 2010; Zhao, 2011).

Other data, however, are inconsistent with the peripheral limitation hypothesis. For example, it is not clear why rodents that have to run to obtain food did not increase their intake in late lactation above those of non-runners, but rather culled their offspring to reduce demand, or postponed weaning their pups (Perrigo, 1987; Schubert et al., 2009). In addition it was shown that MF1 mice did elevate their milk production when temperatures were reduced (Król and Speakman, 2003a; Król and Speakman, 2003b; Speakman and Król, 2005), even though they would not elevate their milk production when given additional pups to raise (Johnson et al., 2001a). To explain these anomalous data it was suggested that the key limit on performance is the capacity of an animal to dissipate body heat (Król and Speakman, 2003a; Speakman and Król, 2010a; Speakman and Król, 2010b). Hence, manipulations made at a fixed ambient temperature (such as forcing animals to run for their food, making them simultaneously pregnant or giving them more pups to raise) did not result in elevated intake because animals were still constrained by their heat dissipation capacity. In contrast, when animals were exposed to the cold, this relaxed the heat dissipation limit, allowing the animals to elevate both their food intake and energy output as milk production (Johnson and Speakman, 2001; Król and Speakman, 2003a; Król and Speakman, 2003b; Speakman and Król, 2005). Król and colleagues (Król et al., 2007) showed that removing the dorsal fur from MF1 female mice during lactation significantly increased food intake beyond that observed in normal lactation, allowing the animals to generate more milk and raise heavier litters, strongly supporting the heat dissipation limit (HDL) theory (Król et al., 2007). However, levels of milk production in cotton rats (Rogowitz, 1998), Swiss mice (Zhao et al., 2010) and striped hamsters (Zhao, 2011) exposed to the cold are not elevated, consistent with the mammary glands working at maximal capacity. Moreover, removal of the dorsal fur increased food intake at peak lactation in Swiss mice (Zhao and Cao, 2009a) and in Siberian hamsters (Paul et al., 2010) but had no significant effect on the weaned litter masses. These data are not consistent with the HDL theory but rather support the peripheral limitation hypothesis [but see Speakman and Król (Speakman and Król, 2011) for alternative explanations of the Paul et al. (Paul et al., 2010) results]. The current data are consequently inconsistent.

One data set that does not support the peripheral limitation hypothesis, but is consistent with the HDL theory, is the observation that mice forced to run for their food do not elevate their intake to accommodate the extra demands of running, but rather scale back their demands by reducing the number of pups they are raising, or slowing their growth and extending lactation (Perrigo, 1987; Schubert et al., 2009). One potential reason why this result does not support the peripheral limitation hypothesis might be the experimental design. In these experiments mice must run a certain distance on a running wheel to obtain small pellets of food. There is consequently a fixed relationship between the amount of running an animal does, and the amount of food it can obtain. It is not possible in this design for the mouse to elevate its intake without performing more running. This link between intake and expenditure may be a realistic reflection of natural foraging behaviour (Schubert et al., 2009), but may make it impossible for the mice to elevate their intake sufficiently to cover both lactation and running demands because they reach a peripheral limit in their muscle capacity to perform exercise.

To explore this phenomenon further, we designed a protocol that dissociates the tight linkage between how much running is performed and how much food is obtained. In detail, we gave lactating mice 80% of their estimated total daily energy requirements and then required them to run a fixed distance that was lower than the distance they could run when not lactating (hence below any peripheral limit in their running capacity) to gain access to ad libitum food supplies. In this design, therefore, we broke the tight linkage between exercise level and intake that has characterised the previous experiments in this area (Perrigo, 1987; Schubert et al., 2009). In this situation we predicted that if the peripheral limitation hypothesis was correct, the mice would run to the fixed criterion level to obtain ad libitum food supplies, and then they would eat whatever food was necessary to cover both the costs of lactation and the costs of running. In contrast, if the heat dissipation limit theory was correct (and mice do not modulate their thermal conductance when forced to run), the mice running during lactation would have significant problems reaching the criterion distances to obtain ad libitum food because of the additional heat burden of running combined with lactation (assuming thermal conductance of the pelage is not modulated). Hence, we predicted the mice would often fail to obtain ad libitum food, and even when they did so, they would still face a heat dissipation limit and might be unable to ingest more food than that observed in mice that were not forced to exercise for food. Consequently, they would be obliged to reduce their milk production and raise lighter litters (comprising smaller individual offspring or smaller litter sizes). One assumption we made when deriving the predictions of the different hypotheses was that the thermal conductance of the fur was not altered in animals that had to run for their food. In the present study we also tested the effect of running on the thermal conductance of the fur.

We performed four different experimental studies. In the first experiment we observed the pattern of activity in reproducing mice given access to running wheels, but not required to run to obtain access to food. This experiment allowed us to determine the levels of activity that the mice could sustain and hence the criterion distances to obtain access to food during lactation. In the second experiment we gave mice running wheels but this time during lactation we required them to run a fixed distance (either 2, 4 or 6 km each day) to obtain ad libitum access to food above 80% of their requirements, which we provided whether they ran or not. One assumption we made when deriving the predictions of the different hypotheses was that the thermal conductance of the fur was not altered in animals that had to run for their food. The third experiment was designed to directly test this assumption by investigating the effect of wheel-running on conductance of the fur. Finally, the effects of the running behaviour on the food intake of the mice might come about not because of limits reflecting heat dissipation but because mice that have to run for a substantial period each day are unable to do anything else, like feeding, or suckling their pups, during the time that they are running (about 3 h per day). To investigate this possibility, we performed a fourth experiment where we manipulated different groups of lactating female mice by preventing them from (a) feeding, (b) suckling, (c) feeding and suckling and (d) feeding, suckling and sleeping, for 3 h each day (six half-hour periods). In these groups, we compared the asymptotic food intake at peak lactation, and growth of the offspring, with that of unmanipulated mice.

Animals

Virgin female mice (outbred MF1), 8–10 weeks of age, were housed individually in plastic cages with clean sawdust and paper bedding and maintained at 21±1°C on a 12 h:12 h light:dark cycle (lights on at 07:00 h). In experiment 1, rodent chow (CRM, Pelleted Rat and Mouse Breeder and Grower Diet, Special Diets Services, BP Nutrition, Witham, Essex, UK) and water were available ad libitum. In experiments 2, 3 and 4, food (16.17 kJ g−1, 67.3% carbohydrate, 19.2% protein, 4.3% fat; produced by Research Diets, Inc., New Brunswick, NJ, USA: open source diet code D12450B) and water were available in relation to running performance (experiment 2) or ad libitum (other experiments).

Experiment 1

To evaluate changes in wheel-running activity of female mice during the different stages of reproduction, 15 females were placed separately in cages with access to a running wheel that was linked to a computer system, which monitored the number of wheel revolutions and the times at which these occurred in the daily cycle. In detail, a wheel (15 cm diameter and 10 cm width) was fixed using an axle on the wall inside the cage. The lower side of the wheel was 1 cm above the bottom of the cage, giving females easy to access to the wheel. The wheel carried a small magnet that operated a counting device. All the wheel revolution counters were linked into a computer that monitored the wheel-running activity on a minute by minute basis (number of wheel revolutions). This computer also operated the hoppers based on the number of recorded revolutions. After 1 week of baseline measurements, a male was introduced to the cage for mating and was removed 11 days later. This period is normally sufficient to ensure the female becomes pregnant (Król and Speakman, 2003). After parturition (day 0 of lactation), litter size and mass were measured first on day 3 of lactation and then daily until weaning on day 21 of lactation. In our previous work on this strain, we have normally weaned mice at age 18 or 19 days. Because we were unsure how they would respond to having a wheel in the cage and one potential response would be to extend lactation, we used 21 days as the weaning date in experiment 1. Based on the data from experiment 1, it was clear the mice did not extend lactation and hence in the following experiments we reverted to a weaning date of day 19. The females were then monitored for an additional 8 days after lactation. Over the baseline, mating, pregnant, lactating and post-lactating periods, wheel-running behaviour was monitored at 1 min intervals, and summary data on activity time (minutes containing wheel activity) and distance run were summarised each day (09:00 h to 09:00 h).

Experiment 2

To test the effects of wheel running on maternal body mass, food intake, litter mass and litter size in lactating mice, 57 females were transferred into cages with monitored running wheels. Body mass, food intake and wheel-running activity were measured for 3 days. Then the females were provided with 80% of their individual ad libitum food intake. The running wheels were connected to a computer-controlled food dispenser. When the mice reached a target distance the computer would open the food dispenser, giving the animals ad libitum access to additional food. The target distance that the mice had to run to obtain the food was 3.5 km day−1 (equal 7500 revolutions day−1) for the first 2 days, 5.2 km day−1 (11,000 revolutions day−1) for the next 2 days and 7.1 km day−1 (15,000 revolutions day−1) for the next 2 days. From experiment 1, we knew that non-reproductive mice ran on average 7.25 km day−1 so the criteria distances to obtain access to ad libitum food were within their potential performance limits. We defined this period as the baseline training stage. After this period, 57 males were transferred into the cages to mate with the females and were removed after 11 days during which both females and males had free access to food and water and the running wheel. After the males were removed, females were given 80% of their estimated food requirements with a target distance to run to obtain additional ad libitum food of 0.4 km day−1 until the day of parturition. This very low target was designed to be easily achievable by pregnant mice but kept the mice aware of the requirement to run to obtain ad libitum food. The measurements in this stage were termed ‘pregnancy’. We provided 80% of their required food calculated from a regression equation linking food intake and body mass of pregnant MF1 mice derived from data in the same strain of mouse (Gamo et al., 2013) (N=112, y=0.096x+1.110, R2=0.316, P<0.001). No measurements were carried out from parturition (day 0 of lactation) until day 2 of lactation. On day 3 of lactation, all lactating females were randomly divided into three groups, balanced for litter size. Females in the three groups were given 80% of their estimated food requirement and then required to run a pre-set 0.2, 0.4 and 0.6 km day−1, respectively, to obtain additional food. The running target of these three groups was increased by 0.2, 0.4 and 0.6 km per day, and reached 2, 4 and 6 km day−1, respectively, on day 12 of lactation. It was then maintained at this same level until weaning. These groups are hereafter referred to as 2 km, 4 km and 6 km mice, respectively. Eleven females did not become pregnant and were set the same running targets as the 6 km mice. On day 3 until day 19 of weaning, body mass and food intake of females and litter mass and size were measured daily. The 80% food requirement on days 3–5 of lactation was calculated according to the regression equation between food intake and body mass (N=40, y=0.509x−9.230, R2=0.407, P<0.001), and the 80% food requirement on days 6–18 was calculated based on the regression equation between food intake and litter mass derived from the data in a previous study (Gamo et al., 2013) (N=234, y=0.114x+8.486, R2=0.547, P<0.001). In addition to monitoring food intake and body mass of the mother each day, we also measured the mass and number of offspring. If the number of offspring was discrepant between days we assumed that mortality had occurred. Because we did not record actual deaths we do not know whether offspring died or were killed by the mother. The mothers often ate the missing pups so there was no trace of them. The raw data on the daily pattern of running activity for mice are available for collaborative exploitation.

Experiment 3

To test for wheel-running effects on the thermal conductance of fur, 10 females were housed individually in cages with free access to running wheels. Another 10 animals were housed individually in cages without running wheels. Both running and non-running females had ad libitum access to food and water. After 3 weeks these animals were killed by CO2 overdose, and the whole pelage except for the head, limbs and tail was immediately removed. The pelage was stitched around a 20 ml glass pot containing water. The pot was warmed to 45°C and then transferred to an incubator at 4°C. Water temperature change was monitored using a transmitter (Vital view transponder, Mini Mitter, Bend, OR, USA) in the water pot and automatically recorded and stored in the computer at 30 s intervals. Ambient temperature outside the pot was also continuously monitored. After the temperature of the pot declined to around 19°C, the pelage was removed, carefully shaved and then replaced on the pot. The pot was re-warmed to 45°C then transferred to the 4°C incubator and allowed to cool again. Finally, the temperature changes of the water pot alone, without any surrounding pelage were monitored as it cooled from 45°C in the 4°C environment. The intact pelage, shaved skin and removed fur were all weighed (to 0.0001 g).

Experiment 4

To test for time restriction effects on food intake and litter mass in lactating mice, 33 MF1 female mice were randomly assigned to one of the following groups: control mice (N=5) underwent a normal lactation; group 1 (N=6), females were prevented from feeding for 3 h day−1 (food deprived: FD); group 2 (N=6), females were separated from their pups but retained access to food for 3 h day−1 (pup deprived: PD); group 3 (N=8), females were separated from the pups and at the same time prevented from feeding for 3 h day−1 (food and pup deprived: FPD); or group 4 (N=8), as group 3 but additionally the females were not allowed to sleep when the food and pups were removed (food, pup and sleep deprived: FPSD). To prevent sleep, the females were continuously observed and if they attempted to settle down and rest, the cage was gently tapped to prevent them so doing. In all these manipulations except for group 4, the female could sleep if she chose to. The mice in groups 1–4 were manipulated for six half-hour periods during the day (07:30 h–08:00 h, 09:30 h–10:00 h, 11:30 h–12:00 h, 13:30 h–14:00 h, 15:30 h–16:00 h, 17:30 h–18:00 h) making a total of 3 h of disruption, which was the same time spent running by the long distance (6 km) group in experiment 2. Females were paired with males for 11 days to become pregnant. The above manipulations were performed daily from day 3 of lactation until day 19 when the offspring were weaned. Food intake and litter mass were measured on a daily basis over the entire lactation period.

Statistics

Data analysis was carried out using SPSS package (13.0). Distributions of all variables were tested for normality using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. In experiment 1, the change in wheel-running activity was analysed using repeated measures ANOVA. The relationship between wheel-running activity and litter mass or size was tested by Pearson's correlation. In experiment 2, the changes in wheel-running activity, food intake, litter mass and pup mortality were analysed by repeated measures ANOVA. The difference in wheel-running activity, food intake, litter mass and pup mortality between the 2 km, 4 km and 6 km groups was tested using one way ANOVA followed by Tukey's HSD post hoc tests. Pearson's correlation was performed to determine the correlation between litter mass and percentage of animals successfully opening the food hoppers. In experiment 3, the differences in mass of the intact pelage, shaved skin and removed fur as well as cooling slope between non-running and running groups were examined using independent t-tests. In experiment 4, the restriction effects on female food intake and litter mass were tested using one-way ANCOVA with litter size as a covariate. Statistical significance was determined at P<0.05. Values are means ± s.e.m.

All procedures concerning animal care and treatment were approved by the ethical committee for the use of experimental animals of the University of Aberdeen, and were licensed by the UK Home Office and performed under PPL 60/3705.

Experiment 1

Running activity and the time spent wheel running

When given free access to the running wheel, the females showed significant changes in running activity over the first 4 days (days 1–4 of baseline, F3,39=7.059, P<0.01; Fig. 1) as they became used to the wheels, and then had a relatively stable activity, running on average 7.25±0.54 km day−1 on days 5–7 of baseline (days 5–7, F2,28=0.581, P>0.05; Fig. 1) and for 330.62±15.60 min. The time and distance spent wheel running increased on the day that the male was placed in the cage. For the remainder of the time the male was present, the running time was approximately double that when the female was in the cage alone (averaging 548.64±19.04 min) but the total distance run each day was no different from when only the female was in the cage. At this time there were two mice in the cage and it was not possible to identify the separate contributions to this running activity. After the male was removed, the distance run immediately dropped to 3.58±0.26 km day−1 (on day −8 of pregnancy) and the time spent running similarly fell to 203.10±7.87 min day−1. Both these parameters continued to decrease significantly as pregnancy progressed and reached a minimum on day −2 of pregnancy that averaged 0.35±0.10 km day−1 and 50.70±7.36 min day−1 (days −8 to 0 of pregnancy: distance, F8,176=29.35, P<0.001; time, F8,176=22.49, P<0.001). The females ran 1.10±0.25 km day−1 on the day of parturition and ran less during the whole of lactation, averaging 0.51±0.08 km day−1 on day 15 of lactation (days 0–15 of lactation, F15,210=3.095, P<0.001) taking on average 63.13±7.37 min. On day 18, and thereafter, wheel-running activity slightly increased and reached 2.14±0.28 km day−1 on day 20 of lactation (Fig. 1). After the offspring were weaned, wheel-running activity of females increased significantly to 5.01±0.55 km day−1 on day 1 of post-lactation, and reached a plateau (5.38±0.51 km day−1) that was similar to that of pre-pregnancy (days 1–8 of post-lactation, F7,84=1.781, P>0.05). The time spent running similarly recovered to 318.03±25.89 min day−1. The detailed daily patterns of wheel-running activity averaged across individuals in the five different reproductive periods are included in supplementary material Fig. S1. The raw data used to compile these plots are also available for collaborative use (contact the corresponding author). Seven representative days for the baseline, mating, parturition, pregnancy, lactation and post-lactation periods are included in Fig. 2. These figures clearly illustrate the precipitous decline in wheel running that occurred in pregnancy and lactation. During baseline, the running activity was mostly nocturnal and involved a peak of activity after lights out of between 40 and 60 r.p.m.

Fig. 1.

Wheel-running activity over the pre-pregnancy, mating, pregnancy, parturition and lactation period (days) in voluntarily running MF1 mice. The distance run averaged 7.25±0.54 km day−1 in pre-pregnant mice, decreased sharply during pregnancy and reached a minimum of 0.51±0.08 km day−1 at peak lactation. The arrow indicates parturition day. The third, sixth and ninth days when the male was present are indicated as m3, m6 and m9, respectively. Values are means ± s.e.m.

Fig. 1.

Wheel-running activity over the pre-pregnancy, mating, pregnancy, parturition and lactation period (days) in voluntarily running MF1 mice. The distance run averaged 7.25±0.54 km day−1 in pre-pregnant mice, decreased sharply during pregnancy and reached a minimum of 0.51±0.08 km day−1 at peak lactation. The arrow indicates parturition day. The third, sixth and ninth days when the male was present are indicated as m3, m6 and m9, respectively. Values are means ± s.e.m.

Fig. 2.

Daily pattern of voluntary wheel running activity of MF1 mice on seven representative days for the baseline, mating, pregnancy, parturition, lactation and post-lactation periods. Values are means.

Fig. 2.

Daily pattern of voluntary wheel running activity of MF1 mice on seven representative days for the baseline, mating, pregnancy, parturition, lactation and post-lactation periods. Values are means.

Experiment 2

Running distance

Prior to mating, the distance run did not differ significantly between non-reproductive mice and reproductive mice allocated to the 2 km, 4 km or 6 km groups; the distance run during the baseline period was 7.70±0.50, 8.23±0.82, 7.52±0.67 and 7.31±0.58 km day−1 in non-reproductive, 2 km, 4 km and 6 km groups, respectively (F3,48=0.356, P>0.05; Fig. 3, Table 1). When the mice were on the restricted diet (80% of food intake on days 1–3) and were required to run 3.5, 5.2 and then 7.1 km day−1 to obtain access to ad libitum food, wheel-running distance increased significantly (days 4–9, F5,210=11.062, P<0.001; Table 1) and averaged 9.58±0.90, 11.53±1.03, 10.25±0.55 and 9.52±0.73 km day−1 for non-reproductive, 2 km, 4 km and 6 km groups, respectively, on day 9 of baseline.

Fig. 3.

Voluntary wheel-running activity of MF1 mice during pre-pregnancy, pregnancy, parturition and lactation (days). Mice were non-reproductive or had to run 2, 4 and 6 km day−1 at peak lactation to obtain access to ad libitum food (2 km, 4 km and 6 km groups). Arrow indicates parturition day. Values are means ± s.e.m.

Fig. 3.

Voluntary wheel-running activity of MF1 mice during pre-pregnancy, pregnancy, parturition and lactation (days). Mice were non-reproductive or had to run 2, 4 and 6 km day−1 at peak lactation to obtain access to ad libitum food (2 km, 4 km and 6 km groups). Arrow indicates parturition day. Values are means ± s.e.m.

During pregnancy, wheel-running distances of 2 km, 4 km and 6 km mice decreased significantly (days −8 to −1, F7,259=82.56, P<0.001) and reached a minimum on day −1 (Table 1) that averaged 1.25±0.13, 0.97±0.15 and 1.28±0.24 km day−1, respectively. This was again substantially more than the 0.4 km day−1 they were required to run to get ad libitum food access. On day 3 of lactation and thereafter, wheel-running activity of both 4 km and 6 km mice increased in line with the increasing daily target (4 km, F15,180=2.538, P<0.01 and 6 km, F15,150=3.424, P<0.001; Fig. 3, Table 1). The distance run averaged 2.04±0.34 and 2.05±0.31 km day−1 on day 3, increasing to a maximum on day 11 that averaged 3.15±0.27 and 4.26±0.36 km day−1 for 4 km and 6 km mice, respectively. However, wheel-running distance of the 2 km mice did not show a significant increase over the lactation period (F15,165=1.601, P>0.05) and the average distance run was 1.95±0.24 km day−1. The wheel-running distance was significantly greater in 6 km mice than in 2 km mice on day 9, 10, 13 and 15 of lactation (day 9, F3,48=64.572, P<0.001). Among the four groups, non-reproductive mice ran far more than the pregnant and lactating mice on day −7 of pregnancy to day 17 of lactation (day −7, F3,48=9.572, P<0.01 and day 17, F3,48=16.485, P<0.001; Fig. 3).

Food intake

Mean food intake was not significantly different between non-reproductive mice and 2 km, 4 km and 6 km mice (4.94±0.23, 4.66±0.23, 4.63±0.11 and 4.30±0.18 g day−1, respectively) at the start of the experiment during baseline (F3,49=1.784, P>0.05; Fig. 4, Table 1). After 6 days of running training with 80% food, the food intake of all mice increased significantly by 15%, 21%, 14% and 14% and averaged 5.67±0.32, 5.64±0.22, 5.30±0.28 and 4.91±0.28 g day−1 in non-reproductive controls and 2 km, 4 km and 6 km mice (F11,539=8.716, P<0.01), but no group differences were found at the end of the baseline training period (day 12 of baseline, F3,49=2.003, P>0.05; Table 1). During pregnancy, the food intake of pregnant mice increased significantly and was higher than that of non-reproductive mice on days −5 to −2 (day −2, F3,49=3.360, P<0.05). After parturition, all the lactating mice ate more food than non-reproductive mice on days 2–18 of lactation (day 18 of lactation, F3,49=81.226, P<0.001; Fig. 4).

Table 1.

Summary of mean data for experiment 2

Summary of mean data for experiment 2
Summary of mean data for experiment 2

Daily food intake of the three lactating groups increased significantly over time in lactation (days 3–18 of lactation, F15,690=8.057, P<0.001). The maximum intake averaged 17.20±0.63 g day−1 in 2 km mice on day 13, 15.13±1.21 g day−1 in 4 km mice on day 12 and 13.86±0.76 g day−1 in 6 km mice on day 8. Among the three lactating groups, the food intake of 6 km mice was significantly lower than that of 2 km mice on days 9–12, and days 15–18 of lactation (day 9, F2,38=8.506, P<0.01; day 18, F2,38=15.918, P<0.01). The asymptotic food intake (averaged over days 9–14) was 16.97±0.40, 14.29±0.72 and 12.65±0.45 g day−1 in 2 km, 4 km and 6 km mice, respectively. The asymptotic food intake of the 6 km mice was significantly lower by 25% than that of the 2 km mice (F2,38=14.800, P<0.001; Fig. 4, Table 1). On the days when the mice ran sufficient distance to gain access to ad libitum food, the asymptotic food intake (daily food intake between days 11 and 18) was not significantly different between the females that had to run 2, 4 or 6 km to obtain food (18.07±0.36, 17.60±0.60 and 16.80±0.58 g day−1, P<0.05; Fig. 5, Table 1).

Fig. 4.

Food intake of MF1 mice during pre-pregnancy, pregnancy, parturition and lactation (days). Mice were non-reproductive or had to run 2, 4 and 6 km day−1 at peak lactation to obtain access to ad libitum food (2 km, 4 km and 6 km groups). *P<0.05, **P<0.01. Values are means ± s.e.m.

Fig. 4.

Food intake of MF1 mice during pre-pregnancy, pregnancy, parturition and lactation (days). Mice were non-reproductive or had to run 2, 4 and 6 km day−1 at peak lactation to obtain access to ad libitum food (2 km, 4 km and 6 km groups). *P<0.05, **P<0.01. Values are means ± s.e.m.

Percentage of open hoppers

The 2 km mice were able to run the pre-set target distance and open hoppers every day until day 11 of lactation. The percentage of mice that ran the target distance was 100% before day 9 in the 4 km mice, and 100% before day 6 in the 6 km mice. The decreasing percentage of mice that ran sufficiently to open the hoppers (Fig. 6A) indicated that more and more mice in both the 4 km and 6 km groups were unable to run the target distance as lactation progressed. On the day of weaning (day 19), the percentage of open hoppers was 50%, 30% and 0% for the 2 km, 4 km and 6 km mice, respectively. There were positive correlations between weaned litter mass and the percentage of opened hoppers for 4 km mice (y=0.59x+17.52, P<0.05) and 6 km mice (y=0.59x+20.30, P<0.001), but not for 2 km mice (y=0.23x+53.81, P>0.05; Fig. 6B). No significant relationships (P>0.05) were observed between female body mass on day 2 of lactation and the accumulated days on which the mice ran sufficiently to open the hoppers during mid- and late lactation (days 8–17) for the 2 km, 4 km or 6 km group.

Fig. 5.

Food intake of female mice on the days that they ran to target throughout lactation. Mice were non-reproductive or had to run 2, 4 and 6 km day−1 at peak lactation to obtain access to ad libitum food (2 km, 4 km and 6 km groups). **P<0.01. Values are means ± s.e.m.

Fig. 5.

Food intake of female mice on the days that they ran to target throughout lactation. Mice were non-reproductive or had to run 2, 4 and 6 km day−1 at peak lactation to obtain access to ad libitum food (2 km, 4 km and 6 km groups). **P<0.01. Values are means ± s.e.m.

Daily pattern of running activity

The average daily pattern of running activity for mice in the three distance groups for all days are available as supplementary material Figs S2, S3 and S4 for 2km, 4km and 6km mice, respectively. The raw data on which these plots are based are also available for collaborative use. Example days for early (day 5) and late (day 18) lactation for all three groups are presented in Fig. 7. The most striking difference between these patterns and the patterns of voluntary running during baseline (see Fig. 2 and supplementary material Fig. S1) is that when lactating mice had to run to get access to ad libitum food, they distributed their activity much more equally between the light and dark cycles. Moreover, although there was a noticeable increase in activity after lights out, the maximum running distance was always less than 40 r.p.m., contrasting the much greater running speeds (40–60 r.p.m.) at baseline.

Litter mass

Litter mass averaged 18.47±1.37, 17.67±1.81 and 19.19±1.47 g for 2 km, 4 km and 6 km mice on day 3 of lactation (F2,38=0.230, P>0.05; Fig. 8A) and increased significantly over the lactation period (days 3–19, F16,608=141.163, P<0.001). From day 13 until weaning, litter mass in the 2 km group was significantly greater than that in the other two groups (day 13, F2,38=3.455, P<0.01). The weaned litter mass of the 2 km mice was 30.4% and 48.9% heavier than the mass of the weaned litters of the 4 km and 6 km mice, respectively (day 19, 72.31±2.40, 55.46±4.26 and 48.57±2.52 g for 2 km, 4 km and 6 km mice, F2,38=13.531, P<0.001; Fig. 8A). Pup mortality of 2 km mice averaged 2.17±1.14% by day 4 of lactation, and averaged 4.48±2.70% on day 19 of weaning (F15,165=1.397, P>0.05; Fig. 8B). However, both 4 km and 6 km mice showed an increasing mortality between days 4 and 13 (4 km mice, F9,108=6.291, P<0.001 and 6 km mice, F9,99=9.489, P<0.001) and then a stable mortality between days 14 and 19 (4 km, F5,60=1.000, P>0.05; 6 km, F5,55=1.730, P>0.05). During the increasing phase on days 4–13, pup mortality increased from 2.88±1.59% to 24.17±5.78% and 0 to 26.20±6.50% in 4 km and 6 km mice, respectively. Mortality of 4 km and 6 km mice was 5.7 and 6.2 times higher than that of 2 km mice during days 14–19, which was significant from day 12 onwards (day 12, F2,36=3.658, P<0.001; Fig. 8B).

Experiment 3

Mass and cooling rate of pelage and fur

There was no significant difference in the mass of the intact pelage between non-running and running mice (Table 2). In addition, the shaved pelage and removed fur showed no significant group differences in mass. Between 39°C and 18°C, the cooling rate of the pots surrounded by the intact pelage from non-running mice was not significantly different to that when surrounded by the pelage of running mice. The slopes of the exponential cooling curves were −0.037±0.001 and −0.037±0.001 in non-runners and runners, respectively (t18=0.474, P>0.05). The cooling rate of the pots alone was not significantly faster than when surrounded by the pelage and also did not show any group difference (non-runners, −0.072±0.003; runners, −0.077±0.005; t18=0.904, P>0.05). As expected, shaved skin retarded heat loss less efficiently than the intact pelage. The cooling rate of shaved skin from the runners was significantly faster than that of non-runners (slope of non-runner, −0.058±0.002; runner, −0.063±0.001; t18=2.109, P<0.05; Fig. 9). These data suggest that 37–40% of the resistance to heat flow of the intact pelage was provided by the skin and 60–63% by the fur.

Fig. 6.

(A) The percentage of opening hoppers over the lactation period and (B) the correlation between litter mass on weaning day and percentage of open hoppers in MF1 mice voluntarily running to obtain ad libitum access to food; 2 km, 4 km and 6 km indicate mice whose targets were set at 2, 4 and 6 km day−1, respectively. There was a positive correlation between litter mass and percentage of open hoppers in 4 km mice (N=13, y=0.59x+17.52, P<0.05) and 6 km mice (N=12, y=0.59x+20.30, P<0.001).

Fig. 6.

(A) The percentage of opening hoppers over the lactation period and (B) the correlation between litter mass on weaning day and percentage of open hoppers in MF1 mice voluntarily running to obtain ad libitum access to food; 2 km, 4 km and 6 km indicate mice whose targets were set at 2, 4 and 6 km day−1, respectively. There was a positive correlation between litter mass and percentage of open hoppers in 4 km mice (N=13, y=0.59x+17.52, P<0.05) and 6 km mice (N=12, y=0.59x+20.30, P<0.001).

Experiment 4

Food intake

Mean food intake on day 3 of lactation (the day the manipulations began) was 11.01±1.19 g day−1 in controls, 9.69±1.24 g day−1 in the FD group, 10.05±0.77 g day−1 in the PD group, 11.12±0.64 g day−1 in the FPD group and 11.06±0.57 g day−1 in the FPSD group and no significant differences were found between these groups (F4,25=0.973, P>0.05; Fig. 10). The group differences on any day throughout lactation were not significant when litter size was included as a covariate in the analysis (e.g. day 20: control, 21.98±2.17 g day−1; FD, 16.25±1.89 g day−1; PD, 16.27±1.67 g day−1; FPD, 19.07±1.08 g day−1; and FPSD, 19.72±0.90 g day−1; F4,25=2.715, P>0.05).

Litter size

Over the lactation period (days 3–20), litter mass increased from 24.67±4.17 g on day 3 to 100.46±10.15 g on day 20 in controls, 15.60±2.69 g to 75.83±8.93 g in the FD group, 18.10±3.71 g to 77.33±10.47 g in the PD group, 23.87±2.04 g to 85.45±6.10 g in the FPD group and 22.87±1.08 g to 93.84±2.77 g in the FPSD group. There were no significant differences between these groups in litter mass on any day in lactation (e.g. day 3, F4,25=1.380, P>0.05; day 20, F4,25=2.122, P>0.05; Fig. 10).

Fig. 7.

Daily pattern of voluntary wheel-running activity during early (day 5, A) and late (day 18, B) lactation in MF1 mice that had to run a set target of 2, 4 or 6 km to obtain ad libitum access to food. Values are means.

Fig. 7.

Daily pattern of voluntary wheel-running activity during early (day 5, A) and late (day 18, B) lactation in MF1 mice that had to run a set target of 2, 4 or 6 km to obtain ad libitum access to food. Values are means.

During the baseline period, prior to reproduction, the mice voluntarily ran on average 7.25 km day−1. This is at the lower end of the distances that rodents have been reported to voluntarily run in wheels. For example, the distance run per day can be as far as 43 km for rats (Richter, 1927), 31 km for white footed mice Peromyscus leucopus (Kavanau and Brant, 1965; Kavanau, 1967), 19 km for lemmings Lemmus lemmus (de Kock and Rohn, 1971), 16 km for laboratory mice (Festing and Greenwood, 1976; Morgan et al., 2003), 9 km for golden hamsters Mesocricetus auratus (Richards, 1966) and 8 km for Mongolian gerbils Meriones unguiculatus (Roper, 1976; Roper, 1978). When mice had to run pre-set revolutions per pellet, they increased wheel-running activity from an average of 10–20 km day−1 (Vaanholt et al., 2007). The maximum distance that laboratory mice could run under this training was around 23 km day−1 (Vaanholt et al., 2007). Similarly, when the mice in our study were food restricted to 80% of their normal intake and required to run to obtain ad libitum access to food, they greatly increased their wheel-running distances from on average 7.25 to around 11.5 km day−1. This was despite the fact that they first needed to run only 3.5 km and later 7.1 km to obtain ad libitum food access, distances that they had covered voluntarily prior to any restriction. Consequently, the animals had no need to increase their activity but they did so anyway. Increased physical activity of mice under caloric restriction is a well-established phenomenon (reviewed in Speakman and Mitchell, 2011) and is presumed to reflect a drive to search for food when under restriction, or anticipatory behaviour waiting for the daily food delivery (called food anticipatory activity) (Zhao and Cao, 2009b). Paradoxically, this leads to an exacerbated energy deficit, which for rats maintained under restriction can ultimately be fatal (Even and Nicolaïdis, 1993; Selman et al., 2005). In the case of our mice, the consequence of the increased activity was only that their food intake increased to cover the costs of the elevated activity.

Fig. 8.

Changes in litter mass (A) and pup mortality (B) throughout lactation in MF1 mice that had to run a set target of 2, 4 or 6 km to obtain ad libitum access to food. Values are means ± s.e.m. *P<0.05, **P<0.01.

Fig. 8.

Changes in litter mass (A) and pup mortality (B) throughout lactation in MF1 mice that had to run a set target of 2, 4 or 6 km to obtain ad libitum access to food. Values are means ± s.e.m. *P<0.05, **P<0.01.

Table 2.

Mass of skin with fur, shaved skin and fur in non-running and running MF1 mice

Mass of skin with fur, shaved skin and fur in non-running and running MF1 mice
Mass of skin with fur, shaved skin and fur in non-running and running MF1 mice
Fig. 9.

Cooling time and slopes of cooling curves (inset) measured in pots surrounded by fur and skin (F+S) or shaved skin (S), and for the pot only (P) in non-running (NR) and running (R) MF1 females. Tp–a indicates the temperature of the pot water minus ambient temperature (in the incubator). Values are means ± s.e.m. There was no significant effect of wheel running on the slope of cooling time of the fur and skin (t18=0.474, P>0.05). *The cooling rate of the shaved skin from the runners was significantly greater than that of non-runners (P<0.05).

Fig. 9.

Cooling time and slopes of cooling curves (inset) measured in pots surrounded by fur and skin (F+S) or shaved skin (S), and for the pot only (P) in non-running (NR) and running (R) MF1 females. Tp–a indicates the temperature of the pot water minus ambient temperature (in the incubator). Values are means ± s.e.m. There was no significant effect of wheel running on the slope of cooling time of the fur and skin (t18=0.474, P>0.05). *The cooling rate of the shaved skin from the runners was significantly greater than that of non-runners (P<0.05).

During pregnancy and lactation, voluntary wheel running activity fell enormously. This was similar to the decline in wheel running reported previously in pregnant and lactating rats (Slonaker, 1924; Wang, 1924) and also the decline in general activity in this strain of mouse during reproduction (Speakman et al., 2001). The pattern of wheel-running distance (and time) revealed here is an exaggerated version of the general activity patterns. This decline in wheel-running activity during lactation may have several contributory causes. First, the time available to run may be reduced because the animals spend large amounts of time suckling their offspring and also an increased time spent feeding (Speakman et al., 2001). Second, the energy that would have been devoted to physical activity may be diverted to support the export of milk. Finally, by reducing activity the animals can avoid hyperthermia, which might occur for animals combining the high heat load of lactation (Król and Speakman, 2003a; Król and Speakman, 2003b) with the heat generated by physical activity, in a situation where their total capacity to dissipate heat is limited. The large decline in the levels of wheel-running activity in lactation was consequently consistent with both the HDL theory and the resource allocation hypothesis that mice prioritise energy use between activities when under energetic stress.

When mice had to run during lactation to obtain ad libitum access to food, they significantly increased their levels of activity above the voluntary levels observed in experiment 1, but these levels remained much lower than those of non-reproductive individuals on the same reward schedule, and lower than their own activity prior to reproduction. As lactation progressed, the females increasingly failed to reach the pre-set target that would allow them ad libitum access to the food. This was despite the fact that the target for all three groups was well below the levels that the mice were clearly capable of running based on their own performances prior to mating, and the simultaneous activity of non-reproducing mice. The failure to run these distances was therefore not because the mice were being asked to perform a task beyond their physiological capabilities. We suggest the most likely reason for the failure to run the required distance to open the food hoppers was that the mice were limited in their capacity to run because of the risks of hyperthermia when combining physical activity with lactation. The greater the required distance, the less likely the mice were to be able to run this distance without overheating. Supporting this interpretation, the body temperatures of mice with implanted transmitters were higher when they were physically active than when at rest (Gamo et al., 2013).

Fig. 10.

Food intake (A) and litter mass (B) during lactation in the MF1 females deprived of food (FD), pups (PD), both food and pups (FPD) or food, pups and sleep (FPSD). Values are means ± s.e.m.

Fig. 10.

Food intake (A) and litter mass (B) during lactation in the MF1 females deprived of food (FD), pups (PD), both food and pups (FPD) or food, pups and sleep (FPSD). Values are means ± s.e.m.

The detailed daily patterns of activity of the running animals (Fig. 7) strongly support this interpretation, in that the mice that had to run in lactation never ran at the same high revolution rates that were observed in non-reproductive mice (Fig. 2), and their running was more spread out across the day and more fragmented in nature. This is consistent with mice attempting to run the target distance but limiting their exposure to hyperthermia risk by restricting their maximum running speed, making shorter running bouts and therefore extending their activity over a longer period.

The direct consequence of failing to reach the target running distance was that the asymptotic level of food intake of mice at peak lactation was inversely related to the target distance. The further direct consequence of these asymptotic intakes was that the weaned litter mass in the 2 km, 4 km and 6 km females was lower by 18%, 37% and 46% than the previously reported 86.7±1.41 g litter mass in normal lactating females (Johnson et al., 2001a). A major contributory factor to the low food intake and hence impaired litter mass accumulation was the fact the mice failed to open the hoppers and therefore only obtained 80% of their estimated food energy requirements on these days. On the days that the mice did open the hoppers they had ad libitum access to food. In theory, therefore, they could have ingested food on these days to cover not only the costs of lactation and running on those days, but also to compensate for the energy shortfalls on the days that the hoppers were not opened. The peripheral limitation hypothesis predicted that on these days, when the hoppers were opened, the mice would ingest food in direct proportion to the summed demands of their peripheral tissues. Hence, it would be predicted that the food intake would increase from 2 km to 4 km and 6 km mice. However, this was not the case, and the actual intake did not differ significantly across the groups. Between days 11 and 18 the average ad libitum intake on days that the hoppers were opened was 18.1 g for the 2 km mice, 17.3 g for the 4 km mice and 16.5 g for the 6 km mice. These were all significantly lower than the previously reported 23.1 g day−1 food intake of MF1 mice that had ad libitum access to food during lactation (Johnson et al., 2001a). However, this previous mass of food intake refers to a different diet (CRM pellets) which has a lower absorption efficiency – hence, a greater mass of food must be ingested to obtain the same energy. The average asymptotic food intake of lactating MF1 mice fed ad libitum in lactation, feeding on the same diet as used here was 18.2 g day−1 (Gamo et al., 2013). This was not significantly different from the intake reported here on the days the mice ran sufficiently to successfully gain access to ad libitum food. Hence, the data do not support the predictions of the peripheral limitation hypothesis, but strongly support the heat dissipation limitation hypothesis, which predicts a constraint imposed on the food intake of the mice by the capacity to dissipate heat. Our direct measurements of the pelages of mice that ran, and mice that did not run, indicated that the heat dissipation capacity of the pelage was not affected by whether the mice ran or not. However, runners had shaved skin that conducted more heat than for non-runners, suggesting some acclimation at this level to improve heat dissipation. Moreover, we cannot eliminate the possibility that mice were able to modulate their heat loss in other ways – by, for example, vasodilation.

Our data are similar to those reported previously using a different method linking food intake to physical activity (Perrigo, 1987), in which forced running deer mice and house mice did not exhibit higher food intake compared with control females fed ad libitum. Both maternal energy intake and expenditure declined with increasing foraging costs in house mice, in which milk energy output showed a relative decline with rising foraging costs, both at peak lactation and over the entire reproductive period (Schubert et al., 2009).

In the present study, the females that had to run to obtain ad libitum access to food spent about 3 h in running wheels, which was about six times higher than voluntary running females during lactation. This required time spent running may have resulted in the mice spending less and more fragmented time suckling their young (or feeding and sleeping). It has been well established that suckling is one of the primary factors stimulating oxytocin and prolactin release and milk let down, thereby influencing milk production (Speakman and Król, 2005). As running activity might interfere with the suckling stimulus, this could lead to the reduced milk production and consequently the lower litter mass in females that had to run a pre-set distance to obtain access to food. Thus, the failure to open the hoppers might not be because of a heat constraint but because, by running, the suckling stimulus was disrupted and the demands of the pups were therefore reduced, and hence the females had less need to run. However, when lactating females were prevented from accessing their pups, or from feeding and sleeping for the same duration as the time spent on running wheels in 6 km runners, they did not have significantly altered food intake or litter mass from each other, or from non-manipulated control females. Consequently, it is unlikely that the effect of wheel running on the time–activity distribution during lactation was the primary factor mediating the limitation on sustained energy intake.

Although the present data are consistent with the heat dissipation limit theory, it is important to recognise that other factors may also be important in the decision making of the female about how hard she will work in lactation. Additional factors may be the consequences of reproduction for oxidative stress (Costantini, 2008; Speakman, 2008; Dowling and Simmons, 2009; Monaghan et al., 2009; Selman et al., 2012) (but see Speakman and Selman, 2011) and immune function (Christe et al., 2000; Christe et al., 2011; Drazen et al., 2003). Among mammals, studies of the impact of reproduction on oxidative stress have generated mixed results, with some field studies, using serum or plasma, indicating increased damage as a consequence of reproduction (Bergeron et al., 2011; Fletcher et al., 2013). In contrast, laboratory-based studies have indicated that in some tissues, notably the liver, oxidative damage is actually reduced in reproducing compared with non-reproducing females (Garratt et al., 2011; Ołdakowski et al., 2012). The cause of this difference between studies remains unclear. Perhaps in the laboratory environment where animals are fed high-quality foods with abundant antioxidants they are able to offset any increase in oxygen radical production, but in the field such foods are not available, exposing the trade-off between reproduction and oxidative damage. Alternatively, the difference between laboratory and field studies may be confounded by the different tissues used for analysis and this may then be an analytical artifact. Current data are insufficient to separate these ideas.

In summary, this study has shown that the voluntary wheel-running activity significantly decreased during pregnancy and was maintained at a low level during lactation, before recovering during post-lactation to baseline pre-reproduction levels. This could reflect avoidance of hyperthermia or reallocation of energy devoted to exercise into milk production. When the females were provided with 80% of their estimated food requirements and had to run a pre-set 2, 4 or 6 km distance before being given ad libitum access to additional food, most of them did not complete the running target during late lactation. Food intake and litter mass were significantly lower in the females running to obtain ad libitum access to food compared with those reported previously (Johnson and Speakman, 2001; Johnson et al., 2001a; Johnson et al., 2001b; Johnson et al., 2001c; Speakman et al., 2001). The running distance attained before reproduction (and in non-reproductive animals on the same reward schedule) were much greater than the target distances required to obtain ad libitum food access, suggesting that the failure to reach the target was not due the limited capacity of the leg muscles to perform work. The detailed patterns of activity in running females in lactation compared with baseline activity strongly suggested that the primary reason for the failure to reach the target was that the animals were avoiding the risk of hyperthermia, due to the combined heat stress of running and lactation. When the females did run sufficiently to gain ad libitum food access, their intake did not differ between the different distance groups or from controls that were not required to run. These last data also support the heat dissipation limit theory idea but are inconsistent with the peripheral limitation hypothesis.

We are enormously grateful to Dr Phil Bagley who constructed and programmed the wheel running and hopper equipment used in this experiment. We thank theanimal house staff for their invaluable help at various stages throughout the project, and C. Hambly and L. Vaanholt and three anonymous referees for excellent comments on previous versions of the manuscript that substantially improved it.

FUNDING

This work was funded in part by an International Incoming Fellowship from the Royal Society (London) to Z.-J.Z., and in part by a grant from the Natural Environmental Research Council [grant no. NE/C004159/1] to J.R.S. Y.G. was funded by a scholarship from the Rotary foundation and S.M. was funded by an Erasmus exchange scholarship.

Bergeron
P.
,
Careau
V.
,
Humphries
M. M.
,
Reale
D.
,
Speakman
J. R.
,
Garant
D.
(
2011
).
The energetic and oxidative costs of reproduction in a free-ranging rodent
.
Funct. Ecol.
25
,
1063
-
1071
.
Christe
P.
,
Arlettaz
R.
,
Vogel
P.
(
2000
).
Variation in intensity of a parasitic mite (Spinturnix myoti) in relation to the reproductive cycle and immunocompetence of its bat host (Myotis myotis)
.
Ecol. Lett.
3
,
207
-
212
.
Christe
P.
,
Glaizot
O.
,
Strepparava
N.
,
Devevey
G.
,
Fumagalli
L.
(
2011
).
Twofold cost of reproduction: an increase in parental effort leads to higher malarial parasitaemia and to a decrease in resistance to oxidative stress
.
Proc. Biol. Sci.
279
,
1142
-
1149
.
Costantini
D.
(
2008
).
Oxidative stress in ecology and evolution: lessons from avian studies
.
Ecol. Lett.
11
,
1238
-
1251
.
de Kock
L. L.
,
Rohn
I.
(
1971
).
Observations on the use of the exercise-wheel in relation to the social rank and hormonal conditions in the bank vole (Clethrionomys glareolus), and the Norway lemming (Lemmus lemmus)
.
Z. Tierpsychol.
29
,
180
-
195
.
Denis
R. G. P.
,
Bing
C.
,
Naderali
E. K.
,
Vernon
R. G.
,
Williams
G.
(
2003
).
Lactation modulates diurnal expression profiles of specific leptin receptor isoforms in the rat hypothalamus
.
J. Endocrinol.
178
,
225
-
232
.
Dowling
D. K.
,
Simmons
L. W.
(
2009
).
Reactive oxygen species as universal constraints in life-history evolution
.
Proc. R. Soc. B
276
,
1737
-
1745
.
Drazen
D. L.
,
Trasy
A.
,
Nelson
R. J.
(
2003
).
Photoperiod differentially affects energetics of immunity in pregnant and lactating Siberian hamsters (Phodopus sungorus)
.
Can. J. Zool.
81
,
1406
-
1413
.
Drent
R.
,
Daan
S.
(
1980
).
The prudent parent: energetic adjustments in avian breeding
.
Ardea
68
,
225
-
252
.
Drummond
H.
,
Vazquez
E.
,
Sanchez-Colon
S.
,
Martinez-Gomez
M.
,
Hudson
R.
(
2000
).
Competition for milk in the domestic rabbit: survivors benefit from littermate deaths
.
Ethology
106
,
511
-
526
.
Even
P. C.
,
Nicolaïdis
S.
(
1993
).
Adaptive changes in energy expenditure during mild and severe feed restriction in the rat
.
Br. J. Nutr.
70
,
421
-
431
.
Festing
M. F. W.
,
Greenwood
R.
(
1976
).
Home-cage wheel activity recording in mice
.
Lab. Anim.
10
,
81
-
85
.
Fletcher
Q. E.
,
Selman
C.
,
Boutin
S.
,
McAdam
A. G.
,
Woods
S. B.
,
Seo
A. Y.
,
Leeuwenburgh
C.
,
Speakman
J. R.
,
Humphries
M. M.
(
2013
).
Oxidative damage increases with reproductive energy expenditure and is reduced by food supplementation
.
Evolution
67
,
1527
-
1536
.
Gamo
Y.
,
Bernard
A.
,
Mitchell
S. E.
,
Hambly
C.
,
Al Jothery
A.
,
Vaanholt
L. M.
,
Król
E.
,
Speakman
J. R.
(
2013
).
Limits to sustained energy intake. XVI. Body temperature and physical activity of female mice during pregnancy
.
J. Exp. Biol.
216
,
2328
-
2338
.
Garratt
M.
,
Vasilaki
A.
,
Stockley
P.
,
McArdle
F.
,
Jackson
M.
,
Hurst
J. L.
(
2011
).
Is oxidative stress a physiological cost of reproduction? An experimental test in house mice
.
Proc. Biol. Sci.
278
,
1098
-
1106
.
Gittleman
J. L.
,
Thompson
S. D.
(
1988
).
Energy allocation in mammalian reproduction
.
Am. Zool.
28
,
863
-
875
.
Hackländer
K.
,
Tataruch
F.
,
Ruf
T.
(
2002
).
The effect of dietary fat content on lactation energetics in the European hare (Lepus europaeus)
.
Physiol. Biochem. Zool.
75
,
19
-
28
.
Hammond
K. A.
,
Diamond
J.
(
1992
).
An experimental test for a ceiling on sustained metabolic rate in lactating mice
.
Physiol. Zool.
65
,
952
-
977
.
Hammond
K. A.
,
Diamond
J.
(
1994
).
Limits to dietary nutrient intake and intestinal nutrient uptake in lactating mice
.
Physiol. Zool.
67
,
282
-
303
.
Hammond
K. A.
,
Diamond
J.
(
1997
).
Maximal sustained energy budgets in humans and animals
.
Nature
386
,
457
-
462
.
Hammond
K. A.
,
Kristan
D. M.
(
2000
).
Responses to lactation and cold exposure by deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus)
.
Physiol. Biochem. Zool.
73
,
547
-
556
.
Hammond
K. A.
,
Konarzewski
M.
,
Torres
R. M.
,
Diamond
J.
(
1994
).
Metabolic ceilings under a combination of peak energy demands
.
Physiol. Zool.
67
,
1479
-
1506
.
Hammond
K. A.
,
Lloyd
K. C. K.
,
Diamond
J.
(
1996
).
Is mammary output capacity limiting to lactational performance in mice?
J. Exp. Biol.
199
,
337
-
349
.
Johnson
M. S.
,
Speakman
J. R.
(
2001
).
Limits to sustained energy intake. V. Effect of cold-exposure during lactation in Mus musculus
.
J. Exp. Biol.
204
,
1967
-
1977
.
Johnson
M. S.
,
Thomson
S. C.
,
Speakman
J. R.
(
2001a
).
Limits to sustained energy intake. I. Lactation in the laboratory mouse Mus musculus
.
J. Exp. Biol.
204
,
1925
-
1935
.
Johnson
M. S.
,
Thomson
S. C.
,
Speakman
J. R.
(
2001b
).
Limits to sustained energy intake. II. Inter-relationships between resting metabolic rate, life-history traits and morphology in Mus musculus
.
J. Exp. Biol.
204
,
1937
-
1946
.
Johnson
M. S.
,
Thomson
S. C.
,
Speakman
J. R.
(
2001c
).
Limits to sustained energy intake. III. Effects of concurrent pregnancy and lactation in Mus musculus
.
J. Exp. Biol.
204
,
1947
-
1956
.
Kavanau
J. L.
(
1967
).
Behavior of captive white-footed mice
.
Science
155
,
1623
-
1639
.
Kavanau
J. L.
,
Brant
D. H.
(
1965
).
Wheel running preferences of Peromyscus
.
Nature
208
,
597
-
598
.
Kirkwood
J. K.
(
1983
).
A limit to metabolisable energy intake in mammals and birds
.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol.
75A
,
1
-
3
.
Knight
C. H.
,
Maltz
E.
,
Docherty
A. H.
(
1986
).
Milk yield and composition in mice: effects of litter size and lactation number
.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol.
84A
,
127
-
133
.
Koteja
P.
(
1996
).
Measuring energy metabolism with open-flow respirometric systems? Which design to choose?
Funct. Ecol.
10
,
675
-
677
.
Król
E.
,
Speakman
J. R.
(
2003a
).
Limits to sustained energy intake. VI. Energetics of lactation in laboratory mice at thermoneutrality
.
J. Exp. Biol.
206
,
4255
-
4266
.
Król
E.
,
Speakman
J. R.
(
2003b
).
Limits to sustained energy intake. VII. Milk energy output in laboratory mice at thermoneutrality
.
J. Exp. Biol.
206
,
4267
-
4281
.
Król
E.
,
Johnson
M. S.
,
Speakman
J. R.
(
2003
).
Limits to sustained energy intake. VIII. Resting metabolic rate and organ morphology of laboratory mice lactating at thermoneutrality
.
J. Exp. Biol.
206
,
4283
-
4291
.
Król
E.
,
Murphy
M.
,
Speakman
J. R.
(
2007
).
Limits to sustained energy intake. X. Effects of fur removal on reproductive performance in laboratory mice
.
J. Exp. Biol.
210
,
4233
-
4243
.
Meyer
H.
,
Dammers
C.
,
Kienzle
E.
(
1985
).
Body composition of newborn puppies and nutrient requirement of pregnant bitches
. In
Investigations on Nutrient Requirements in Breeding Bitches and Suckling Pups
(ed.
Meyer
H.
), pp.
7
-
25
.
Advances in Animal Physiology and Animal Nutrition (supplements to Journal of Animal Physiology and Animal Nutrition)
.
Hamburg, Berlin
:
Verlag Paul Parey
.
Millar
J. S.
(
1977
).
Adaptive features of mammalian reproduction
.
Evolution
31
,
370
-
386
.
Monaghan
P.
,
Metcalfe
N. B.
,
Torres
R.
(
2009
).
Oxidative stress as a mediator of life history trade-offs: mechanisms, measurements and interpretation
.
Ecol. Lett.
12
,
75
-
92
.
Morgan
T. J.
,
Garland
T.
 Jr
,
Carter
P. A.
(
2003
).
Ontogenies in mice selected for high voluntary wheel-running activity. I. Mean ontogenies
.
Evolution
57
,
646
-
657
.
Ołdakowski
Ł.
,
Piotrowska
Ż.
,
Chrzaácik
K. M.
,
Sadowska
E. T.
,
Koteja
P.
,
Taylor
J. R. E.
(
2012
).
Is reproduction costly? No increase of oxidative damage in breeding bank voles
.
J. Exp. Biol.
215
,
1799
-
1805
.
Paul
M. J.
,
Tuthill
C.
,
Kauffman
A. S.
,
Zucker
I.
(
2010
).
Pelage insulation, litter size, and ambient temperature impact maternal energy intake and offspring development during lactation
.
Physiol. Behav.
100
,
128
-
134
.
Perrigo
G.
(
1987
).
Breeding and feeding strategies in deer mice and house mice when females are challenged to work for their food
.
Anim. Behav.
35
,
1298
-
1316
.
Piersma
T.
,
van Gils
J. A.
(
2011
)
The Flexible Phenotype: a Body Centred Integration of Ecology, Physiology and Behaviour
.
Oxford
:
Oxford University Press
.
Richards
M. P. M.
(
1966
).
Activity measured by running wheels and observation during the oestrous cycle, pregnancy and pseudopregnancy in the golden hamster
.
Anim. Behav.
14
,
450
-
458
.
Richter
C. P.
(
1927
).
Animal behavior and internal drives
.
Q. Rev. Biol.
2
,
307
-
343
.
Rogowitz
G. L.
(
1998
).
Limits to milk flow and energy allocation during lactation of the hispid cotton rat (Sigmodon hispidus)
.
Physiol. Zool.
71
,
312
-
320
.
Roper
T. J.
(
1976
).
Sex differences in circadian wheel running rhythms in the Mongolian gerbil
.
Physiol. Behav.
17
,
549
-
551
.
Roper
T. J.
(
1978
).
The effect of food deprivation on drinking and running in Mongolian gerbils
.
Anim. Behav.
26
,
1264
-
1272
.
Ruf
T.
,
Grafl
B.
(
2010
).
Maximum rates of sustained metabolic rate in cold-exposed Djungarian hamsters (Phodopus sungorus): the second wind
.
J. Comp. Physiol. B
180
,
1089
-
1098
.
Schubert
K. A.
,
de Vries
G.
,
Vaanholt
L. M.
,
Meijer
H. A. J.
,
Daan
S.
,
Verhulst
S.
(
2009
).
Maternal energy allocation to offspring increases with environmental quality in house mice
.
Am. Nat.
173
,
831
-
840
.
Selman
C.
,
Phillips
T.
,
Staib
J. L.
,
Duncan
J. S.
,
Leeuwenburgh
C.
,
Speakman
J. R.
(
2005
).
Energy expenditure of calorically restricted rats is higher than predicted from their altered body composition
.
Mech. Ageing Dev.
126
,
783
-
793
.
Selman
C.
,
Blount
J. D.
,
Nussey
D. H.
,
Speakman
J. R.
(
2012
).
Oxidative damage, ageing, and life-history evolution: where now?
Trends Ecol. Evol.
27
,
570
-
577
.
Slonaker
J. R.
(
1924
).
The effect of pubescence, oestruation, and menopause on the voluntary activity in the albino rat
.
Am. J. Physiol.
68
,
294
-
315
.
Speakman
J. R.
(
2000
).
The cost of living: field metabolic rates of small mammals
.
Adv. Ecol. Res.
30
,
177
-
297
.
Speakman
J. R.
(
2008
).
The physiological cost of reproduction in small mammals
.
Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B
363
,
375
-
398
.
Speakman
J. R.
,
Król
E.
(
2005
).
Limits to sustained energy intake IX: a review of hypotheses
.
J. Comp. Physiol. B
175
,
375
-
394
.
Speakman
J. R.
,
Król
E.
(
2010a
).
Maximal heat dissipation capacity and hyperthermia risk: neglected key factors in the ecology of endotherms
.
J. Anim. Ecol.
79
,
726
-
746
.
Speakman
J. R.
,
Król
E.
(
2010b
).
The heat dissipation limit theory and evolution of life histories in endotherms – time to dispose of the disposable soma theory?
Integr. Comp. Biol.
50
,
793
-
807
.
Speakman
J. R.
,
Król
E.
(
2011
).
Limits to sustained energy intake. XIII. Recent progress and future perspectives
.
J. Exp. Biol.
214
,
230
-
241
.
Speakman
J. R.
,
McQueenie
J.
(
1996
).
Limits to sustained metabolic rate: the link between food intake, basal metabolic rate and morphology in reproducing mice, Mus musculus
.
Physiol. Zool.
69
,
746
-
769
.
Speakman
J. R.
,
Mitchell
S. E.
(
2011
).
Caloric restriction
.
Mol. Aspects Med.
32
,
159
-
221
.
Speakman
J. R.
,
Selman
C.
(
2011
).
The free-radical damage theory: accumulating evidence against a simple link of oxidative stress to ageing and lifespan
.
Bioessays
33
,
255
-
259
.
Speakman
J. R.
,
Gidney
A.
,
Bett
J.
,
Mitchell
I. P.
,
Johnson
M. S.
(
2001
).
Limits to sustained energy intake. IV. Effect of variation in food quality on lactating mice Mus musculus
.
J. Exp. Biol.
204
,
1957
-
1965
.
Speakman
J. R.
,
Król
E.
,
Johnson
M. S.
(
2004
).
The functional significance of individual variation in basal metabolic rate
.
Physiol. Biochem. Zool.
77
,
900
-
915
.
Thompson
S. D.
(
1992
).
Gestation and lactation in small mammals: basal metabolic rate and the limits of energy use
. In
Mammalian Energetics. Interdisciplinary Views of Metabolism and Reproduction
,
Chap. 10
(ed.
Tomasi
T. E.
,
Horton
T. H.
), pp.
213
-
259
.
Ithaca, NY
:
Comstock
.
Vaanholt
L. M.
,
De Jong
B.
,
Garland
T.
 Jr
,
Daan
S.
,
Visser
G. H.
(
2007
).
Behavioural and physiological responses to increased foraging effort in male mice
.
J. Exp. Biol.
210
,
2013
-
2024
.
Valencak
T. G.
,
Ruf
T.
(
2009
).
Energy turnover in European hares is centrally limited during early, but not during peak lactation
.
J. Comp. Physiol. B
179
,
933
-
943
.
Valencak
T. G.
,
Hackländer
K.
,
Ruf
T.
(
2010
).
Peak energy turnover in lactating European hares: a test of the heat dissipation limitation hypothesis
.
J. Exp. Biol.
213
,
2832
-
2839
.
Wang
G. H.
(
1925
).
The changes in the amount of daily food intake of the albino rat during pregnancy and lactation
.
Am. J. Phys.
71
,
736
-
741
.
Weiner
J.
(
1992
).
Physiological limits to sustainable energy budgets in birds and mammals: ecological implications
.
Trends Ecol. Evol.
7
,
384
-
388
.
Wu
S. H.
,
Zhang
L. N.
,
Speakman
J. R.
,
Wang
D. H.
(
2009
).
Limits to sustained energy intake. XI. A test of the heat dissipation limitation hypothesis in lactating Brandt's voles (Lasiopodomys brandtii)
.
J. Exp. Biol.
212
,
3455
-
3465
.
Zhang
X. Y.
,
Wang
D. H.
(
2007
).
Thermogenesis, food intake and serum leptin in cold-exposed lactating Brandt's voles Lasiopodomys brandtii
.
J. Exp. Biol.
210
,
512
-
521
.
Zhao
Z. J.
(
2011
).
Energy budget during lactation in striped hamsters at different ambient temperatures
.
J. Exp. Biol.
214
,
988
-
995
.
Zhao
Z. J.
,
Cao
J.
(
2009a
).
Effect of fur removal on the thermal conductance and energy budget in lactating Swiss mice
.
J. Exp. Biol.
212
,
2541
-
2549
.
Zhao
Z. J.
,
Cao
J.
(
2009b
).
Plasticity in energy budget and behavior in Swiss mice and striped hamsters under stochastic food deprivation and refeeding
.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol.
154A
,
84
-
91
.
Zhao
Z. J.
,
Chi
Q. S.
,
Cao
J.
(
2010
).
Milk energy output during peak lactation in shaved Swiss mice
.
Physiol. Behav.
101
,
59
-
66
.

COMPETING INTERESTS

No competing interests declared.

Supplementary information