In the blue crab, Callinectes sapidus, claw color varies by sex, sexual maturity and individual. Males rely in part on color cues to select appropriate mates, and these chromatic cues may be perceived through an opponent interaction between two photoreceptors with maximum wavelength sensitivities at 440 and 508 nm. The range of color discrimination of this dichromatic visual system may be limited, however, and it is unclear whether male blue crabs are capable of discriminating the natural variations in claw color that may be important in mate choice. By testing males’ innate color preferences in binary choice tests between photographs of red-clawed females and six variations of orange-clawed females, we examined both the chromatic (opponent interaction) and achromatic (relative luminance) cues used in male mate choice. Males significantly preferred red-clawed females to orange-clawed females, except when the test colors were similar in both opponency and relative luminance. Our results are unusual in that they indicate that male mate choice in the blue crab is not guided solely by achromatic or chromatic mechanisms, suggesting that both color and intensity are used to evaluate female claw color.

Bright, conspicuous colors are often investigated within the context of sexual communication and sexual selection. Animals use color to communicate information such as sex (Butcher and Rohwer, 1989; Marquez and Verrell, 1991), reproductive readiness (McLennan and McPhail, 1990; Sköld et al., 2008), individual or species identity (Losos, 1985; Detto et al., 2006), social status (Rohwer, 1975; Watt, 1986) and individual quality (Endler, 1980; Hill and Montgomerie, 1994). However, colors can only be effective signals or cues if they can be perceived by the intended receiver (Rowland, 1979). It is necessary then, when investigating the role of an animal’s coloration, to also consider the receiver’s visual system, which varies widely across animal taxa (reviewed by Briscoe and Chittka, 2001; Osorio and Vorobyev, 2008; Kelber and Osorio, 2010).

The blue crab, Callinectes sapidus, is an excellent system for simultaneously investigating the perception of color cues and their role in sexual signaling. The blue crab is a colorful portunid species endemic to coastal Atlantic waters along North, Central and South America. Male and female blue crabs have sexually dimorphic claw coloration; adult males have white and blue claws, and adult females have orange or red claws (Fig. 1). The claw coloration of immature males and females ranges from pale blue or violet to orange (Fig. 1), and qualitative evidence suggests that claw coloration changes with sexual maturity in both males and females. These differences in claw color between sexes and between sexually immature and mature crabs suggest that claw color may act as a cue of sex and/or sexual maturity, while individual variation within each group invites speculation regarding individual quality or fitness.

Hypotheses regarding the signaling function of claw coloration must be evaluated within the parameters of the blue crab visual system. Blue crabs likely have a dichromatic visual system. Electroretinogram (ERG) data suggest the presence of photoreceptors with a peak sensitivity of 508 nm (green) over the entire eye and a second set of photoreceptors in the ventral portion of the eye with a peak sensitivity at about 440 nm (blue) (Martin and Mote, 1982). Thus, the detection of chromatic cues may occur via opponency between the green and blue photoreceptors in the ventral portion of the eye. Achromatic cues, those that are based on signal intensity alone, are likely detected via the 508 nm photoreceptor given that it is the predominant receptor in the eye (Martin and Mote, 1982).

Color vision was suggested in behavioral experiments where C. sapidus showed distinct responses to yellow, red and blue approaching objects (Bursey, 1984). However, these experiments did not control for the intensity of the colors used; thus, the responses may have been based on signal intensity rather than color. A more recent behavioral study demonstrated the blue crab’s use of chromatic cues when males displayed a preference for images of red-clawed females over those of gray-clawed females that had the same luminance (as perceived by the blue crab eye) (Baldwin and Johnsen, 2009). Although males preferred red-clawed females to those with isoluminant gray claws, it was not determined how or whether male blue crabs can distinguish naturally occurring variations in the red and orange claw coloration of females. The blue crab’s blue–green dichromatic visual system may have a restricted range of color discrimination because of its limited sensitivity to long-wavelength light. Thus, the nuances of orange and red colors that are evident to humans may not be perceived by the blue crab eye.

Here, we investigated natural variation in claw coloration in male and female blue crabs, documenting color differences between sexually immature and mature individuals of both sexes. We then estimated the appearance of claw color to the blue crab visual system by modeling perceived luminance (achromatic cues) and opponency (chromatic cues) using ERG data. Then, in binary choice tests using photographs of females with claws colored red and six variations of orange, we behaviorally tested the ability of male blue crabs to discriminate between various long-wavelength-dominated colors. These behavioral assays examine both chromatic and achromatic mechanisms of male mate choice and the likelihood of whether males are capable of discriminating between naturally occurring claw colors.

Study species

Males and females of the blue crab, C. sapidus Rathbun 1896, were captured from Jarrett Bay near Smyrna, NC, USA (34°45′31″N, 76°30′44″W) in April 2011. Sex and sexual maturity were determined by visually examining claw color, abdomen shape and overall size. In males, sexual maturity is associated with size and individuals over 100 mm were assumed to be sexually mature (Milikin and Williams, 1984). In females, abdomen shape is a reliable indicator of sexual maturity. Immature females have triangular shaped abdomens while mature females have wider, rounder abdomens (Newcombe et al., 1949; Jivoff et al., 2007). Prepubertal female crabs are in a transitional stage of sexual maturity, meaning that they will become sexually receptive just prior to their next molt; these can also be identified by their abdominal shape and coloration.

Measuring spectral reflectance of claw color

Blue crab claw color was measured in April 2011, during the first peak of the commercial soft crabbing season in North Carolina. During this time, juvenile and adult male and female blue crabs were available in running seawater enclosures at a commercial crab fishing facility in Smyrna. Crabs were separated into five groups based on sex and sexual maturity: immature males, mature males, immature females, prepubertal females and mature females. Spectral reflectance measurements of claws were taken in a darkened room. Prior to measurements, crabs were chilled on ice for 15–30 min to facilitate handling.

Spectral reflectance data were collected using methods outlined previously (Johnsen, 2005), using a fiber optic reflectance probe (R400-7 reflection probe, Ocean Optics Inc., Dunedin, FL, USA) coupled with a pulsed xenon light source (PX-2, Ocean Optics) and a multi-channel spectrometer (USB2000, Ocean Optics). The reflectance probe contained seven 400 μm diameter optical fibers in a six-around-one arrangement. The six outer fibers were coupled to the light source and illuminated the specimen. The central fiber collected the light reflected from the specimen and was coupled to the spectrometer. The end of the reflectance probe was held next to the claw surface at a 45 deg angle using a rigid optical mount, which illuminated and collected the back-reflection of the claw surface. This approach reduced the collection of specularly reflected light from the shiny claw surface and instead collected the diffuse reflectance, which is relatively independent of the angles of illumination and measurement (Palmer, 1995). The reflectance measurements were calibrated using Spectralon™, a diffuse reflectance standard that diffusely reflects nearly 100% of light from 200 to 800 nm (WS-1 Diffuse Reflection Standard, Ocean Optics). Spectral reflectance was taken at the center of the dactyls (moveable fingers) of each side of the claws, referred to as the interior and exterior claw faces relative to the crab. Measurements were taken from the right claws of 18–29 individuals of each group.

Calculation of relative luminance and opponency to the blue crab eye

Claw reflectance spectra, illumination spectra and the spectral sensitivity of the visual system were used to model the perceived chromatic and achromatic signals. The luminance (L) of reflected light perceived by the blue crab eye is given by:
where R(λ) is the diffuse spectral reflectance of the object being viewed, I(λ) is the downwelling spectral irradiance, S(λ) is the spectral sensitivity of the crab eye, and C is a constant that includes factors such as eye size, etc., that are independent of wavelength and factor out when comparing different samples. The irradiance was chosen to be that of the test tank used in the behavioral experiments described later in this paper. The spectral sensitivity of C. sapidus was based on ERG data (Martin and Mote, 1982), and perceived luminance was found for both the blue photoreceptor (λmax=440 nm) and the green photoreceptor (λmax=508 nm). In many species, the medium wavelength photopigment functions as the achromatic channel, and in the blue crab the green photoreceptor is the predominant one found throughout the eye (Martin and Mote, 1982; Cronin and Forward, 1988). Therefore, the achromatic cues we report on here are based on the quantum catch of the green photoreceptor. In addition, for easier comparison we normalized all crab-perceived luminance by the perceived luminance of the red test color used in the behavioral experiments described below, referring to this ratio as ‘relative luminance’.
To determine color as perceived by the blue crab eye, we assumed that color vision occurred via an opponency mechanism between the putative 440 nm and a 508 nm pigment in the blue crab eye (Martin and Mote, 1982; Baldwin and Johnsen, 2009). We used a simple model that assumes that the influence of the two pigments is balanced over the wavelength range of 400 to 700 nm, i.e.:
where Lλ is the perceived luminance calculated in Eqn 1 for a pigment with a peak wavelength of λ. The relative gain of the two channels was adjusted to produce an opponency value of zero for an achromatic object.

While the blue crab may be sensitive to ultraviolet light, we report here only on the visible spectrum of light as a result of limitations of the known spectral sensitivity of the blue crab. In addition, preliminary analyses that extrapolated the visual sensitivity of the crab eye into the ultraviolet did not give significantly different results.

Behavioral experiments evaluating male color preference

Male blue crabs were collected from June to November 2009 and April to August 2010. Crabs were immediately placed into individual buckets with a shallow layer of water and transported to Duke University’s central campus in Durham, NC, USA. There, crabs were kept in individual compartments within a 700 l recirculating artificial seawater system (salinity 29–31‰, temperature 25–26°C, natural light cycle). Compartment walls were opaque to minimize stress and agonistic behavior. Crabs were fed pieces of fish, shrimp or scallop every 2 days, and kept for at least 48 h before being used in experiments.

Fig. 1.

Mean ± s.e.m. spectral reflectance against wavelength (λ), and photographs of both the exterior (top) and interior (bottom) claw surfaces of sexually mature males (N=28), immature males (N=30), immature females (N=27), prepubertal females (N=21) and sexually mature females (N=25).

Fig. 1.

Mean ± s.e.m. spectral reflectance against wavelength (λ), and photographs of both the exterior (top) and interior (bottom) claw surfaces of sexually mature males (N=28), immature males (N=30), immature females (N=27), prepubertal females (N=21) and sexually mature females (N=25).

Binary choice experiments were conducted in three 100 l glass aquariums (32×91×46 cm) with gravel bottoms. During acclimation periods, water was filtered and at all times maintained at the same salinity and temperature as the holding tank. Experimental tanks were kept in a separate room and visually isolated on all sides by blue cloth. The tanks were observed via a video camera and lit using overhead fluorescent and incandescent lamps, resulting in a downwelling irradiance of ∼8×1014 photons cm–2 s–1 (integrated from 400 to 700 nm).

Photographs of a sexually mature female in a receptive posture on a solid gray background were used in place of live females to limit confounding variables, such as chemical, tactile or motion cues. Photos were printed on Staples™ brand recycled copy paper and mounted on white foam boards for stability. A photograph of a red-clawed female was tested against photographs with orange claws of varied relative luminance. The red color tested was selected from a photograph of a sexually mature female blue crab’s claws using the Color Picker tool of Photoshop CS (Adobe Inc., San Jose, CA, USA). After selecting the red test color, we chose an orange color similar in relative luminance under the given illumination spectrum of the test arena. This color is described as orange 0.85, with the number corresponding to the relative luminance. The spectral reflectance of the red and orange colors was measured using a reflecting probe coupled with a light source and spectrometer. From orange 0.85, five other oranges were produced by increasing and decreasing the relative luminance of the color using the Brightness function in the Color Picker tool: one darker (orange 0.48) and four lighter variations (orange 2.6, 5.1, 7.7 and 12). These orange variations had similarly shaped reflectance curves, but varied in mean reflectance (Fig. 2). Thus, a total of six versions of the photograph were used: one image with red-colored claws (Fig. 3) and five images with orange-colored claws. The gray background used in the photographs had a relatively constant spectral reflectance (not shown), with a relative luminance of 3.8 and an opponency of 0.007.

All experimental trials were conducted between 07:00 h and 19:00 h local time from May to November 2009 and April to August 2010. Prior to the start of each experiment, one male was placed in an experimental tank and allowed to adjust to the surroundings for 3 h. Then, two photographs were presented to the crab – one at each end of the tank. Photograph positions were assigned randomly. Over the next hour, the male’s behavior was recorded on video. Videos were later watched and scored blind to the experimental conditions. Most crabs made multiple stereotypic sexual displays that were unambiguously directed towards (and occurred within 5 cm of) one of the two images. During these 5–30 s displays, the crabs rose up on their walking legs, extended their claws, and waved their paddles while facing the photograph [see Baldwin and Johnsen (Baldwin and Johnsen, 2009) for further details]. The total number of sexual displays made towards each image over 1 h was counted. Choice was assigned to the image that received the greater number of displays. The results of each binary choice test, including first display and number of displays, are given in supplementary material Table S1. We evaluated the statistical significance of choices for each variation of orange tested using two-tailed exact binomial tests. The statistical issues inherent in multiple testing were regulated using the Benjamini and Hochberg procedure, which controlled for false discovery rate (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995).

Fig. 2.

Mean ± s.e.m. spectral reflectance of the red and orange colors used in behavioral experiments. N=20 for each color.

Fig. 2.

Mean ± s.e.m. spectral reflectance of the red and orange colors used in behavioral experiments. N=20 for each color.

We used 126 male crabs during these binary choice tests. Because these males were wild-caught, it was not known how recently they had molted or mated. Both of these factors may have affected their sexual receptivity. If a male did not display mate preference during the 1 h trial, the trial was discarded and the male was tested again 5–7 days later. No males were used more than once in a 5 day period and males were not reused for the same test colors. After testing, the males were returned to the re-circulating seawater system.

Spectral reflectance of the claws of C. sapidus

Measurements of claw coloration revealed clear differences in the spectral reflectance of claws between male and female blue crabs and also between sexually immature and mature crabs of each sex (Fig. 1). In most individuals, the average reflectance of the exterior of the claw was greater than that of the interior of the claw. Spectral reflectance varied between claws of sexually mature males and females, with males reflecting more light at shorter (bluer) wavelengths and females reflecting more light at longer (redder) wavelengths. The spectral reflectance of both male and female blue crabs changed with sexual maturity (Fig. 1).

Fig. 3.

One of the six photographs used in binary choice tests, pictured here with claws colored red. The crab is in a known sexually receptive posture.

Fig. 3.

One of the six photographs used in binary choice tests, pictured here with claws colored red. The crab is in a known sexually receptive posture.

Relative luminance and opponency of natural claw colors

Based on our model of blue crab visual perception, the relative luminance and opponency values of claws differed between sexually immature males, mature males, immature females and mature females (Fig. 4). The perceived claw coloration appeared to be distinct between groups, although there was considerable individual variation. Claw coloration of prepubertal females overlapped with both immature and mature females, which is not surprising given that this is an intermediate molt stage that transitions females from sexually immature to mature.

Behavioral experiments

Male color preference did not appear to be mediated solely by chromatic or achromatic cues but rather appeared to have a specific range of preferred color and intensity. Males significantly preferred red-clawed females (with an opponency value of 0.33) to those with claws colored with orange 0.48 (opponency value 0.38; P≤0.004), orange 5.1 (0.43; P≤0.013), orange 7.7 (0.44; P≤0.001) and orange 12 (0.16; P≤0.041) (Figs 5 and 6). Males significantly preferred red-clawed females over females with orange claws with both lower opponency values (orange 12, gray 5.7 and gray 0.85) and higher opponency values (orange 0.48, orange 5.1 and orange 7.7; Figs 5 and 6). However, males were either not able or not motivated to choose between red and orange claws similar in both opponency and relative luminance as perceived by the blue crab visual system. In the trials testing for preference between red and orange 0.85 and orange 2.6, males chose red slightly more often, but these preferences were not statistically significant.

Fig. 4.

Achromatic and chromatic cues of natural crab claws with 95% confidence intervals. Achromatic cues are shown as mean relative luminance, the perceived luminance of each color relative to red. Chromatic cues are reported as opponency values based on an opponent interaction between the blue (λmax=440 nm) and green (λmax=508 nm) channels. Data are reported for each side of the claw relative to the crab, referred to as the interior and exterior claw surface. (A) Interior claw surface. MM-I, mature male interior claws (N=28); IM-I, immature male interior claws (N=30); IF-I, immature female interior claws (N=27); PF-I, prepubertal female interior claws (N=21); and MF-I, mature female interior claws (N=25). (B) Exterior claw surface. MM-E, mature male exterior claws (N=28); IM-E, immature male exterior claws (N=30); IF-E, immature female exterior claws (N=27); PF-E, prepubertal female exterior claws (N=21); and MF-E, sexually mature female exterior claws (N=25).

Fig. 4.

Achromatic and chromatic cues of natural crab claws with 95% confidence intervals. Achromatic cues are shown as mean relative luminance, the perceived luminance of each color relative to red. Chromatic cues are reported as opponency values based on an opponent interaction between the blue (λmax=440 nm) and green (λmax=508 nm) channels. Data are reported for each side of the claw relative to the crab, referred to as the interior and exterior claw surface. (A) Interior claw surface. MM-I, mature male interior claws (N=28); IM-I, immature male interior claws (N=30); IF-I, immature female interior claws (N=27); PF-I, prepubertal female interior claws (N=21); and MF-I, mature female interior claws (N=25). (B) Exterior claw surface. MM-E, mature male exterior claws (N=28); IM-E, immature male exterior claws (N=30); IF-E, immature female exterior claws (N=27); PF-E, prepubertal female exterior claws (N=21); and MF-E, sexually mature female exterior claws (N=25).

In the present study, models of visual perception and behavioral tests of color vision were used to evaluate the ability of the blue crab to perceive differences in natural claw colors. Previous behavioral experiments have indicated that male blue crabs use chromatic cues when choosing a female mate by demonstrating male preference for red-clawed females over those with gray claws matched in relative luminance (Fig. 6B) (Baldwin and Johnsen, 2009). It remained unclear, however, whether males would be capable of discriminating between female claw colors found in nature (Fig. 1). The behavioral trials presented here were intended to probe the ability of the blue crab to choose between similar long-wavelength colors during mate choice. Unexpectedly, our results suggest that male blue crabs use a mixture of chromatic and achromatic cues to discriminate between long-wavelength colors. Additionally, males’ preference for red-clawed females and their ability to discriminate red over variations of orange support the possibility that claw color may function as a sexual signal or cue.

We found that male blue crabs could distinguish between red and orange coloration, except when the test shades were similar in both relative luminance and opponency. The results suggest that both chromatic and achromatic cues function in the discrimination of colors dominated by long-wavelength light. Further, there may be a particular range of relative luminance (0.85–2.6) and opponency (0.3–0.45) values that stimulates male courtship behavior (Fig. 6). Additional studies using multiple shades of gray and other colors may help in understanding the limits of color vision in this species. Alternative models of relative luminance suggest that if the blue crab possessed a visual pigment with a λmax between 540 and 600 nm, the choices we observed could be based solely on achromatic cues. However, no such photoreceptor has been detected through ERG or microspectrophotometry (MSP) (Martin and Mote, 1982; Cronin and Forward, 1988).

In invertebrate species, achromatic and chromatic cues are often used for different tasks. Achromatic vision may be more useful for tasks involving motion detection and shape or object recognition (Lythgoe, 1979; Kelber et al., 2003), while chromatic cues are often used when identifying and classifying objects, such as food, oviposition sites or potential mates (Vorobyev and Osorio, 1998; Sumner and Mollon, 2000). Most studies investigating innate visual behaviors of invertebrates show evidence of only chromatic cues or only achromatic cues being used during specific tasks, and the simultaneous use of both is at present unclear (Kelber and Osorio, 2010). However, experiments that involve color learning have shown evidence of the use of both. For example, studies on the honeybee, Apis mellifera, provide evidence that either chromatic or achromatic cues can be used for object identification, depending on the size of the object (Giurfa and Vorobyev, 1997; Giurfa and Vorobyev, 1998; Giurfa et al., 1997). The hawkmoth, Macroglossum stellatarum, can learn to associate rewards with either the chromatic or the achromatic aspect of a color, but appears to more readily learn the chromatic aspect (Kelber, 2005).

Fig. 5.

(A) One-dimensional plots of relative luminance and opponency of the experimental colors used during binary choice trials. (B) Relative luminance and opponency plotted together to represent how the blue crab may perceive both achromatic and chromatic cues. In both, the red cross shows the value of the red test color. Orange colors that were chosen significantly less often than red are represented by minus signs. Circles represent orange colors that were not chosen significantly less often than red. Data in B are offset for clarity. R, red; G, gray; O, orange; numbers indicate the luminance relative to the red test color.

Fig. 5.

(A) One-dimensional plots of relative luminance and opponency of the experimental colors used during binary choice trials. (B) Relative luminance and opponency plotted together to represent how the blue crab may perceive both achromatic and chromatic cues. In both, the red cross shows the value of the red test color. Orange colors that were chosen significantly less often than red are represented by minus signs. Circles represent orange colors that were not chosen significantly less often than red. Data in B are offset for clarity. R, red; G, gray; O, orange; numbers indicate the luminance relative to the red test color.

True color vision has been infrequently documented in crustaceans. Certainly, spectral sensitivity and the presence of different spectral classes of photoreceptors have been widely documented across crustacean species (Cronin and Forward, 1988; Frank and Widder, 1999; Rajkumar et al., 2010), but the behavioral evidence needed to confirm color vision (and discount confounding achromatic cues) is not common. Aside from tests in C. sapidus, color vision has been demonstrated in a fiddler crab, Uca mjoebergi, where females chose males with painted yellow claws over those with claws painted various shades of gray (Detto, 2007). Color vision has also been shown in a stomatopod, Odontodactylus scyllarus (Marshall et al., 1996). This mantis shrimp was capable of discriminating red, green and yellow from different grays. However, individuals of O. scyllarus were not able to discriminate blue from gray, possibly because of similarities between the blue and gray stimulation profiles (Marshall et al., 1996). The authors suggest that there may be a threshold value between photoreceptor catch ratios for color detection to occur.

Role of claw color

Some crustaceans, most notably crabs and stomatopods, have colorful displays paired with well-developed visual systems, and visual cues can play a role in their communication and social behavior (Schöne, 1968; Marshall et al., 2006). Fiddler crabs (Uca spp.) have been shown to use color cues in sex recognition, individual recognition and mate choice (Detto et al., 2006; Detto, 2007). In brachyuran crabs, the claws, in particular, are commonly used in sexual and agonistic communication (reviewed by Schöne, 1968; Christy, 1987). Claw color in the semaphore crab, Heloecius cordiformis, corresponds with sex and age (Detto et al., 2004), and may indicate sexual maturity. Given these examples, it is reasonable to assume that claw color may act as a signal or cue in the blue crab.

There are several possible roles for claw coloration in Callinectes. First, claw color may play a role in species recognition. There are as many as 16 species of Callinectes, 10 of which have overlapping habitats with C. sapidus in the Caribbean Sea (Robles et al., 2007). Claw color and pattern vary with species (Fig. 7) and may potentially be used to identify conspecific mates. It is possible that certain claw colors tested here were not recognized as belonging to potential mates and thus did not receive male preference.

Measurements of crab claw coloration and their estimated appearance to the blue crab eye indicate that claw color may also function in sex identification. Color differences between males’ blue claws and females’ red claws should be apparent to the blue crab’s dichromatic visual system (Fig. 4). While we have not conducted tests between photographs of blue- and red-clawed crabs, in previous tests between photographs of red- and white-clawed crabs, males often addressed the white-clawed crab photographs with agonistic behavior (Baldwin and Johnsen, 2009). As the exterior face of male claws is largely white, this may indicate that male test subjects viewed our white-clawed crab photographs as male competitors. Also, previous behavioral tests of color vision in the blue crab showed that male and female blue crabs had significantly different reaction times to blue-, yellow- and red-colored approaching objects (Bursey, 1984).

Claw color may also act as a cue of sexual maturity. Spectral reflectance measurements show that claw color changes with sexual maturity in male and female crabs (Figs 1 and 4). Also, claw colors of reproductively ready female crabs (prepubertal and sexually mature females) fall within the boundaries of observed male preference of both relative luminance and opponency (Fig. 5B). Together, these data provide evidence that males may use claw color to identify sexually mature female blue crabs. Examples of color cues indicating sexual maturity are found in many species and are most often described in males (Kodric-Brown, 1985; Frischknecht, 1993; Bakker and Mundwiler, 1994), but are being increasingly documented in females (McLennan, 1995; McLennan, 2000; Amundsen and Forsgren, 2001). Additionally, our color measurements show similarities between immature males and immature females (Figs 1 and 4). In species where juveniles and adults have different coloration, juvenile coloration may be a non-threatening cue to adult conspecifics, indicating a lack of competition for territory, food or mates (Neal, 1993; Mahon, 1994).

Fig. 6.

(A) Results of the binary choice experiments testing male preference between red and orange female claws. (B) Previous results of binary choice tests showing male preference for females with red claws over those with white claws and dark gray claws that were isoluminant to the red claws (Baldwin and Johnsen, 2009). Asterisks denote significance (*P≤0.05, **P≤0.01 and ***P≤0.001).

Fig. 6.

(A) Results of the binary choice experiments testing male preference between red and orange female claws. (B) Previous results of binary choice tests showing male preference for females with red claws over those with white claws and dark gray claws that were isoluminant to the red claws (Baldwin and Johnsen, 2009). Asterisks denote significance (*P≤0.05, **P≤0.01 and ***P≤0.001).

Finally, individual variation in claw color also invites speculation regarding claw color and individual quality. In the blue crab, both the blue and red colors are due to carotenoid-based pigments located in the hypodermis (Smith and Chang, 2007). As carotenoids cannot be synthesized de novo in animals, they must be ingested and may reflect an individual’s foraging ability (Bagnara and Hadley, 1973; Brush and Power, 1976). In environments where carotenoids are limited, carotenoid-based pigmentation may serve as an indicator of individual quality (Endler, 1980; Hill and Montgomerie, 1994). In a number of species, carotenoid-based coloration may reflect an individual’s parasite load, immunological health and overall condition (Borgia and Collis, 1989; McGraw and Hill, 2004; Clotfelter, 2007). Connections between coloration and mate attractiveness have been documented in several species of fish (Frischknecht, 1993; Bakker and Mundwiler, 1994; Evans and Norris, 1996) and birds (Hill and Montgomerie, 1994; Saks et al., 2003). Such results imply that both male and female blue crab claw coloration could be indicative of individual quality. While a relationship between claw color and individual quality has not yet been established for either male or female blue crabs, the possibility that claw color could advertise individual quality has intriguing applications for future studies of mate choice and intraspecific competition.

Fig. 7.

Coloration of species in the genus Callinectes. Of the 16 species of Callinectes, up to 10 species may overlap in coastal regions of the Caribbean Sea. Eight of the 10 Caribbean species are pictured here (C. maracaiboensis and C. affinis are not shown). Photographs courtesy of the Southeastern Regional Taxonomic Center (SERTC).

Fig. 7.

Coloration of species in the genus Callinectes. Of the 16 species of Callinectes, up to 10 species may overlap in coastal regions of the Caribbean Sea. Eight of the 10 Caribbean species are pictured here (C. maracaiboensis and C. affinis are not shown). Photographs courtesy of the Southeastern Regional Taxonomic Center (SERTC).

We thank Mark Hooper for assistance in capturing and classifying crabs and Dr Almut Kelber for commenting on earlier drafts of the manuscript.

FUNDING

J.B. was supported in part by an Anne T. and Robert M. Bass Fellowship for Undergraduate Instruction. S.J. was supported in part by grants from the National Science Foundation [OCE-0852138] and from the Office of Naval Research [N00014-09-1-1053].

Amundsen
T.
,
Forsgren
E.
(
2001
).
Male mate choice selects for female coloration in a fish
.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
98
,
13155
13160
.
Bagnara
J. T.
,
Hadley
M. E.
(
1973
).
Chromatophores and Color Change: the Comparative Physiology of Animal Pigmentation
.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ
:
Prentice-Hall
.
Bakker
T. C. M.
,
Mundwiler
B.
(
1994
).
Female mate choice and male red coloration in a natural three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) population
.
Behav. Ecol.
5
,
74
80
.
Baldwin
J.
,
Johnsen
S.
(
2009
).
The importance of color in mate choice of the blue crab Callinectes sapidus
.
J. Exp. Biol.
212
,
3762
3768
.
Benjamini
Y.
,
Hochberg
Y.
(
1995
).
Controlling the false discovery rate – a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing
.
J. R. Stat. Soc. B.
57
,
289
300
.
Borgia
G.
,
Collis
K.
(
1989
).
Female choice for parasite-free male satin bowerbirds and the evolution of bright male plumage
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
25
,
445
453
.
Briscoe
A. D.
,
Chittka
L.
(
2001
).
The evolution of color vision in insects
.
Annu. Rev. Entomol.
46
,
471
510
.
Brush
A. H.
,
Power
D. M.
(
1976
).
House finch pigmentation: carotenoid metabolism and the effect of diet
.
Auk
4
,
725
739
.
Bursey
C. R.
(
1984
).
Color recognition by the blue crab, Callinectes Sapidus Rathbun (Decapoda, Brachyura)
.
Crustaceana
47
,
278
284
.
Butcher
G. S.
,
Rohwer
S.
(
1989
).
The Evolution of Conspicuous and Disctinctive Coloration for Communication in Birds
.
New York
:
Plenum Press
.
Christy
J. H.
(
1987
).
Competitive mating, mate choice and mating associations of brachyuran crabs
.
Bull. Mar. Sci.
41
,
177
191
.
Clotfelter
E. D.
,
Ardia
D. R.
,
McGraw
K. J.
(
2007
).
Red fish, blue fish: trade-offs between pigmentation and immunity in Betta splendens
.
Behav. Ecol.
18
,
1139
1145
.
Cronin
T. W.
,
Forward
R. B.
(
1988
).
The visual pigments of crabs
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
162
,
463
478
.
Detto
T.
(
2007
).
The fiddler crab Uca mjoebergi uses colour vision in mate choice
.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
274
,
2785
2790
.
Detto
T.
,
Zeil
J.
,
Magrath
R. D.
,
Hunt
S.
(
2004
).
Sex, size and colour in a semi-terrestrial crab, Heloecius cordiformis (H. Milne Edwards, 1837)
.
J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol.
302
,
1
15
.
Detto
T.
,
Backwell
P. R. Y.
,
Hemmi
J. M.
,
Zeil
J.
(
2006
).
Visually mediated species and neighbour recognition in fiddler crabs (Uca mjoebergi and Uca capricornis)
.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
273
,
1661
1666
.
Endler
J. A.
(
1980
).
Natural selection on color patterns in Poecilia reticulata
.
Evolution
1
,
76
91
.
Evans
M. R.
,
Norris
K.
(
1996
).
The importance of carotenoids in signaling during aggressive interactions between male firemouth cichlids (Cichlasoma meeki)
.
Behav. Ecol.
7
,
1
6
.
Frank
T. M.
,
Widder
E. A.
(
1999
).
Comparative study of the spectral sensitivities of mesopelagic crustaceans
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
185
,
255
265
.
Frischknecht
M.
(
1993
).
The breeding colouration of male three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) as an indicator of energy investment in vigour
.
Evol. Ecol.
7
,
439
450
.
Giurfa
M.
,
Vorobyev
M.
(
1997
).
The detection and recognition of color stimuli by honeybees: performance and mechanisms
.
Isr. J. Plant. Sci.
45
,
129
140
.
Giurfa
M.
,
Vorobyev
M.
(
1998
).
The angular range of achromatic target detection by honey bees
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
183
,
101
110
.
Giurfa
M.
,
Vorobyev
M.
,
Brandt
R.
,
Posner
B.
,
Menzel
R.
(
1997
).
Discrimination of coloured stimuli by honeybees: alternative use of achromatic and chromatic signals
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A.
180
,
235
243
Hill
G. E.
,
Montgomerie
R.
(
1994
).
Plumage colour signals nutritional condition in the house finch
.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
258
,
47
52
.
Jivoff
P.
,
Hines
A. H.
,
Quackenbush
S.
(
2007
).
Reproductive Biology and Embryonic Development
.
College Park, MD
:
Maryland Sea Grant College, University of Maryland
.
Johnsen
S.
(
2005
).
The red and the black: bioluminescence and the color of animals in the deep sea
.
Integr. Comp. Biol.
45
,
234
246
.
Kelber
A.
(
2005
).
Alternative use of chromatic and achromatic cues in a hawkmoth
.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
272
,
2143
2147
.
Kelber
A.
,
Osorio
D.
(
2010
).
From spectral information to animal colour vision: experiments and concepts
.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
277
,
1617
1625
.
Kelber
A.
,
Vorobyev
M.
,
Osorio
D.
(
2003
).
Animal colour vision – behavioural tests and physiological concepts
.
Biol. Rev.
78
,
81
118
.
Kennedy
V. S.
,
Cronin
L. E.
(
2007
).
The Blue Crab: Callinectes sapidus
.
College Park, MD
:
Maryland Sea Grant College, University of Maryland
.
Kodric-Brown
A.
(
1985
).
Female preference and sexual selection for male coloration in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata)
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
3
,
199
205
.
Losos
J. B.
(
1985
).
An experimental demonstration of the species-recognition role of Anolis dewlap color
.
Copeia
4
,
905
910
.
Lythgoe
J. N.
(
1979
).
The Ecology of Vision.
New York
:
Clarendon Press
.
Mahon
J. L.
(
1994
).
Advantage of flexible juvenile coloration in two species of Labroides (Pisces: Labridae)
.
Copeia
2
,
520
524
.
Marquez
R.
,
Verrell
P.
(
1991
).
The courtship and mating of the Iberian midwife Toadalytes cisternasii (Amphibia: Anura: Discoglossidae)
.
J. Zool.
225
,
125
139
.
Marshall
N. J.
,
Jones
J. P.
,
Cronin
T. W.
(
1996
).
Behavioral evidence for colour vision in stomatopod crustaceans
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
179
,
473
481
.
Marshall
N. J.
,
Vorobyev
M.
,
Siebeck
U. E.
(
2006
).
What does a reef fish see when it sees a reef fish? Eating ‘Nemo’
. In
Communication in Fishes
(ed.
Ladich
F.
,
Collin
S. P.
,
Moller
P.
,
Kapoor
B. G.
), pp.
393
422
.
Enfield, NH
:
Science Publishers
.
Martin
F. G.
,
Mote
M. I.
(
1982
).
Color receptors in marine crustaceans: a second spectral class of retinular cell in the compound eyes of Callinectes and Carcinus
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
145
,
549
554
.
McGraw
K. J.
,
Hill
G. E.
(
2004
).
Plumage color as a dynamic trait: carotenoid pigmentation of male house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus) fades during the breeding season
.
Can. J. Zool.
82
,
734
738
.
McLennan
D. A.
(
1995
).
Male mate choice based upon female nuptial coloration in the brook stickleback, Culaea inconstans (Kirtland)
.
Anim. Behav.
50
,
213
221
.
McLennan
D. A.
(
2000
).
The macroevolutionary diversification of female and male components of the stickleback breeding system
.
Behaviour
137
,
1029
1045
.
McLennan
D. A.
,
McPhail
J. D.
(
1990
).
Experimental investigations of the evolutionary significance of sexually dimorphic nuptial coloration in Gasterosteus aculeatus (L): the relationship between male color and female behavior
.
Can. J. Zool.
68
,
482
492
.
Milikin
M. R.
,
Williams
A. B.
(
1984
).
Synopsis of biological data on the blue crab, Callinectes sapidus Rathbun
.
NOAA Technical Report NMFS
1
,
NOAA
.
Neal
T. J.
(
1993
).
A test of the function of juvenile color patterns in the pomacentrid fish Hypsypops rubicundus (Teleostei: Pomacentridae)
.
Pac. Sci.
47
,
240
.
Newcombe
C. L.
,
Sandoz
M. D.
,
Rogerstalbert
R.
(
1949
).
Differential growth and moulting characteristics of the blue crab, Callinectes sapidus Rathbun
.
J. Exp. Zool.
Osorio
D.
,
Vorobyev
M.
(
2008
).
A review of the evolution of animal colour vision and visual communication signals
.
Vision Res.
48
,
2042
2051
.
Palmer
J. M.
(
1995
).
The measurement of transmission, absorption, emission, and reflection
. In
Handbook of Optics
, Vol.
2
(ed.
Bass
M.
,
Van Strylan
E. W.
,
Williams
D. R.
,
Wolfe
W. L.
), pp.
251
255
.
New York
:
McGraw-Hill Inc.
Rajkumar
P.
,
Rollmann
S. M.
,
Cook
T. A.
,
Layne
J. E.
(
2010
).
Molecular evidence for color discrimination in the Atlantic sand fiddler crab, U. pugilator
.
J. Exp. Biol.
213
,
4240
4248
.
Robles
R.
,
Schubart
C. D.
,
Conde
J. E.
,
Carmona-Suárez
C.
,
Alvarez
F.
,
Villalobos
J. L.
,
Felder
D. L.
(
2007
).
Molecular phylogeny of the American Callinectes Stimpson, 1860 (Brachyura: Portunidae), based on two partial mitochondrial genes
.
Mar. Biol.
150
,
1265
1274
.
Rohwer
S.
(
1975
).
The social significance of avian winter plumage variability
.
Evolution
4
,
593
610
.
Rowland
W. J.
(
1979
).
The use of color in intraspecific communication
. In
The Behavioral Significance of Color
(ed.
Burtt
E.
), pp.
380
421
.
New York
:
Garland Press
.
Saks
L.
,
Ots
I.
,
Hõrak
P.
(
2003
).
Carotenoid-based plumage coloration of male greenfinches reflects health and immunocompetence
.
Oecologia
134
,
301
307
.
Schöne
H.
(
1968
).
Agonistic and sexual display in aquatic and semi-terrestrial brachyuran crabs
.
Am. Zool.
3
,
641
654
.
Sköld
H. N.
,
Amundsen
T.
,
Svensson
P. A.
,
Mayer
I.
,
Bjelvenmark
J.
,
Forsgren
E.
(
2008
).
Hormonal regulation of female nuptial coloration in a fish
.
Horm. Behav.
54
,
549
556
.
Smith
S. G.
,
Chang
E. S.
(
2007
).
Molting and growth
. In
The Blue Crab, Callinectes sapidus
(ed.
Kennedy
V. S.
,
Cronin
L. E.
), pp.
197
245
.
College Park, MD
:
Maryland Sea Grant College, University of Maryland
.
Sumner
P.
,
Mollon
J. D.
(
2000
).
Catarrhine photopigments are optimized for detecting targets against a foliage background
.
J. Exp. Biol.
203
,
1963
1986
.
Vorobyev
M.
,
Osorio
D.
(
1998
).
Receptor noise as a determinant of colour thresholds
.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
265
,
351
358
.
Watt
D.
(
1986
).
Relationship of plumage variability, size and sex to social dominance in Harris’ sparrows
.
Anim. Behav.
34
,
16
27
.

Supplementary information