SUMMARY
An unequal resource distribution is commonly seen in dominance hierarchies, in which the individual with the higher status is more successful in obtaining the resource. One possible resource is preferred temperature. When situations allow, ectotherms regulate their body temperature by behaviourally selecting different environmental conditions, achieving, when possible, a preferred temperature. Using a shuttlebox, the preferred temperature for Procambarus clarkii was determined to be 23.9°C with upper and lower voluntary escape temperatures of 25.9 and 21.8°C, respectively. If this preferred temperature zone (21.8–25.9°C) was valued as a resource, given the choice between a preferred temperature and a non-preferred temperature, crayfish should compete over the preferred temperature, with the dominant individual of dyadic pairs achieving the preferred temperature more often than the subordinate. Using a dual-choice experimental tank, competition over a binary temperature choice between rank-established paired crayfish was determined under both warm and cold challenge conditions (warm vs preferred temperature and cold vs preferred temperature, respectively). In naive pairings, similar levels of competition over the preferred temperature occurred in both warm and cold challenge trials, as predicted by game theory. In established pairings, however, dominant crayfish gained significantly greater access to preferred temperature in both warm and cold challenge conditions. These results demonstrate that crayfish engage in a cost–benefit assessment during their initial agonistic contests over temperature, but as hierarchies mature, these thermal games are decided by the dominant animal gaining primary access to the temperature resource.
INTRODUCTION
Aggression and conflict occur ubiquitously throughout the animal kingdom (van der Dennen and Falger, 1990). Aggressive behavior often leads to the formation of stable dominance hierarchies, which form as a result of competition over a resource and can provide a peaceful resolution whereby the resource is often unequally divided (Barnard and Burk, 1979; Chase et al., 2002; Pusey and Packer, 1997). Generally, social animals fight when resources, such as food, shelter and space, are limited. Dominance hierarchies define an individual’s status and access to certain resources, including food and shelter. The higher the social status, the more advantaged the individual. This reinforces the fitness of the dominant individual, which wins the majority of the encounters, over the subordinate individual, which loses the majority of the encounters and retreats. Winning an agonistic encounter increases the likelihood of winning the next encounter, and the opposite is also true (Chase et al., 1994; Dugatkin, 1997; Hsu and Wolf, 1999; McDonald et al., 1968). These are known as the winner and loser effects, which help to both separate and stabilise dominance hierarchies.
Aggressive behavior has been well studied in crayfish, which readily fight over shelters (Martin and Moore, 2008; Ranta and Lindstrom, 1992) or space when no shelters are provided (Bovbjerg, 1956). Under such conditions, aggressive encounters include several well-characterised behaviours, such as approaching, pushing, grasping, striking with the claws and the eventual retreat of one opponent (Bruski and Dunham, 1987; Herberholz et al., 2003; Issa et al., 1999; Tierney et al., 2000). Dyadic interactions between crayfish ultimately distinguish dominant and subordinate members of the pair, and interactions within larger groups result in linear dominance hierarchies (Bovbjerg, 1956; Copp, 1986; Issa et al., 1999) (Martin and Moore, 2008; Ranta and Lindstrom, 1992). Size difference is an important factor in determining the outcome of such encounters, with larger crayfish generally having an advantage, but other factors such as claw size, previous fighting experience and prior residence also affect the outcome (Bergman et al., 2003; Daws et al., 2002; Issa et al., 1999; Peeke et al., 1995; Ranta and Lindstrom, 1992; Rutherford et al., 1995). The frequency and intensity of agonistic encounters increase sharply within the first few minutes and subsequently decrease, rapidly during the first hour, and slowly over days to weeks (Huber et al., 2000; Issa et al., 1999). In at least some cases, stable dominance hierarchies can take hours or days to be established (Fero et al., 2007; Huber et al., 2000) and can reverse following the introduction of a large intruder (Graham and Herberholz, 2009). There is evidence that dominant crayfish have preferred access to shelter, food and mates (Herberholz et al., 2007; Herberholz et al., 2003; Ranta and Lindstrom, 1992; Villanelli and Gherardi, 1998).
Agonistic behaviours, however, use physiological resources that could be available for other behaviours (Briffa and Sneddon, 2007), and are considered costly (Castro et al., 2006; Hack, 1997). As a result, they occur at frequencies inversely related to their cost (Hack, 1997), although game theory can be used to assess whether the benefits of aggression outweigh the costs of contest escalation (Sneddon et al., 2000), and would predict that traits that enhance an animal’s resource holding potential are the result of selection (Angilletta, 2009; Briffa and Sneddon, 2007). Whole-organism physiological performance predicts success in agonistic encounters (Mowles et al., 2010) and may underlie the dominance hierarchies (Briffa and Sneddon, 2007; Seebacher and Wilson, 2006; Seebacher and Wilson, 2007). Acclimation to different temperatures will also affect the outcome of agonistic encounters in crayfish, because of changes in intrinsic biochemical properties of the muscles rather than chelae force generation (Seebacher and Wilson, 2006). Indeed, Wilson and colleagues (Wilson et al., 2007; Wilson et al., 2009) have shown that in adult crayfish, although chela size is a dishonest signal of strength, it is a reliable predictor of resource holding potential and contest outcome. As swimming speed is inversely related to chela size, chela size may represent a trade-off (via the handicap principle), signaling greater resource holding potential based not on force production, but on other parameters that cannot be readily detected, such as thermoregulation (Darnell and Munguia, 2011). Because of the risks of injury, however, accurate signaling between conspecifics may still be important in minimising the escalation of contests.
The costs of interspecific or intraspecific interactions as they offset the benefits of resource acquisition have been explored using game theory (Angilletta, 2009; Briffa and Sneddon, 2007; Crowley, 2001; Dugatkin, 1997; Dugatkin et al., 2000; Mitchell and Angilletta, 2009). The general premise is that there are evolutionarily stable strategies (the Nash equilibria) that describe the behavioural phenotypes reflecting the best strategy for a ‘rational’ individual (i.e. one seeking to maximise its payoff) to ‘play’ against another individual (Crowley, 2001). These strategies are argued to be ‘resistant to invasion by other behavioural strategies’, and thus maintained by natural selection (Cronin, 1992). Although detractors of game theory’s broader applicability exist (see Siegfried, 2006), evolutionary biologists argue for the utility of this approach in predicting optimal behavioural strategies (Dugatkin et al., 2000). This approach has recently demonstrated that predator habitat selection is based on prey thermoregulatory behaviour (Angilletta, 2009), with the surprising prediction that predators choose temperatures for which they derive no thermal benefit, whereas prey distribute more evenly among available thermal patches (Mitchell and Angilletta, 2009).
To the best of our knowledge, game theory has not been applied in the context of a preferred temperature serving as a resource over which animals compete, particularly in animals that form dominance hierarchies. This question is important when considering ectothermic species, many of which regulate body temperature (Tb) behaviourally (Myhre and Hammel, 1969). In groups of bluegill fish (Lepomis macrochirus), dominant members set up territories within their preferred temperature (PT), and subordinates are displaced to areas of lower temperature (Beitinger and Magnuson, 1975; Medvick et al., 1981). The notion that crayfish have a specific PT is not firmly established. Crawshaw (Crawshaw, 1974) observed that crayfish (Orconectes immunis) avoid extreme temperatures while moving freely in a wide temperature zone, in what is thought to best represent the optimal thermal range for physiological processes (Fraenkel and Gunn, 1961). Peck (Peck, 1985), however, reported that Orconectes virilis select nearly the same temperature when tested and re-tested in a thermal gradient. Interestingly, Peck (Peck, 1985) also observed that although dominant and subordinate crayfish select the same temperature when placed individually in a thermal gradient, dominants and subordinates segregated when placed in the gradient together, with dominants obtaining the PT (in 40% of trials) and subordinates obtaining lower temperatures (Peck, 1985). These observations strongly suggest that crayfish can exhibit a specific PT and that they will compete for it within a thermal gradient.
One disadvantage with using thermal gradients to measure PT is that the animal may simply be exploring the chamber and experiencing a wide range of temperatures, but not specifically selecting those temperatures. Mean thermal preference and avoided temperatures are more accurately determined using a ‘shuttlebox’, with two chambers whose temperatures are controlled independently. Movement into one chamber causes temperature to increase, and movement into the other chamber causes temperature to decrease (Cadena and Tattersall, 2009b). The animal shuttles from one chamber to the other; this approach estimates the PT as that chosen behaviourally through operant conditioning, as well as defining the upper and lower avoided temperatures (Barber and Crawford, 1979; Dreisig, 1984; Garrick, 1979).
The aims of the present work were to demonstrate that juvenile crayfish Procambarus clarkii (Girard 1852) have a specific PT and to determine whether dominant and subordinate members of crayfish pairs compete for the PT. Our approach was to estimate PT and upper and lower voluntary escape temperatures precisely by placing crayfish individually in a dual chamber shuttlebox and then to allow crayfish to compete for this PT after placing them together in a dual chamber box, with one side held at the PT and the other side either 4°C warmer or 4°C cooler. This approach allowed us to determine whether subordinate crayfish prefer cooler temperatures in the presence of a dominant crayfish, or if they are simply excluded from accessing the PT entirely. We performed these experiments on naive pairs of animals and on pairs whose dominance ranks had been established over a 3-day period. The results indicate that dominant members of established pairs select the PT and displace subordinates to either warmer or cooler ‘non-preferred’ temperatures (NPTs), but that such differences are not apparent within the first 4 h of pairing.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Animals and husbandry
Juvenile crayfish (P. clarkii) 2.5 to 5.0 cm in length were purchased from Atchafalaya Biological Supply Co. (Raceland, LA, USA). Except for the naive pairs, which were held in solitary containers for at least 2 weeks prior to experimentation, all crayfish were kept in a communal tank. The water was held at 22–23°C, a 12 h:12 h light:dark cycle was maintained, and several plastic (PVC/ABS) pipes (10 to 15 cm in length) were given as shelters. To maintain the water in the tank, an aquarium filter was used in addition to weekly water replacement. Crayfish were fed artificial crabmeat (Imitation crab meat, Aquamore Inc., Rancho Cucaomonga, CA, USA) three times per week and were allowed to acclimate for 2 weeks prior to beginning any experiments. No crayfish was used in more than one experiment.
Experimental apparatus
The experimental apparatus used throughout allowed for a dual-choice behavioural design for solitary or paired animals (Table 1). The experimental tank was constructed of Plexiglas® (26.8×10.7×9.0 cm) lined with white contact paper with a height-adjustable central divider to partition the tank into two approximately rectangular chambers (Fig. 1A). To allow access of the crayfish from one chamber to the other, the centre contained a 4.9×4.7 cm opening with a slightly inclined ramp to help commit the crayfish to a given side and to minimise idling between the chambers. At either end of the tank, a smaller compartment (6.4×3.7×14.6 cm) connected via numerous perforations to the larger compartments was used to aerate the water (Fig. 1A). Air was vigorously bubbled into the smaller chambers, to act as a mixing compartment, allowing the water in the larger chambers to be constantly aerated without bubble agitation interfering with the behaviour or visual detection of the crayfish.
To control the temperature of the chambers independently, a temperature probe was attached at the bottom outer corner of both chambers, opposite from the inflow of water (Fig. 1A). These probes relayed information to a computer. Using custom-built software (ICFish v.2.0, Brock University Electronics Shop, St Catharines, ON, Canada), the set-point temperature for both chambers was changed independently and precisely (±0.1°C). The dual chamber temperature box regulated the water temperature continuously to its set point, which depended on the experimental condition. Constant flow of water was achieved through an inflow of water towards the surface and an outflow of water towards the base of each chamber. Water was pumped from each chamber using a magnetic impeller pump (Eheim model 1046A) through a cooling element consisting of copper coils placed in a water bath containing antifreeze (Lauda Brinkmann RE 106), allowing the water in the system to be cooled. The water then passed through separate 600 W heating elements (modified Black and Decker 5-cup coffee makers), rheostatically powered by the dual chamber temperature box, thereby initiating precise heating when needed. Before returning to the tank, the water passed through flow valves allowing the flow rates to be set accordingly.
Crayfish pairings and agonistic assessments
Prior to assessing competition between two crayfish over temperature, crayfish had to be paired to determine dominance status. Two juvenile crayfish were selected randomly from the communal tank and placed into separate Tupperware™ containers. Body length and mass were measured, after which the crayfish were given unique identifiers using white nail polish. They were placed back into their respective Tupperware™ containers and set aside for 30 min without any distractions. The crayfish then were transferred together into another container where they were allowed to interact while being recorded to video for 30 min. In the case of the naive pairings, this assessment occurred within the dual-choice chamber, immediately preceding the temperature-choice trials (see Series IIIa, below), whereas in the established pairings, the behavioural assessments were followed by 3 days of continuous pairing under laboratory conditions, with one plastic pipe placed in the Tupperware™ container to act as a shelter.
To identify the winner and loser in each pairing, the 30-min video was scored for withdrawals and avoidances, as described previously (May and Mercier, 2006; May and Mercier, 2007). A withdrawal was defined as one crayfish retreating after an encounter involving physical contact, whereas an avoidance was defined as a direction change after a head-on approach without making contact. The crayfish with the lowest number of withdrawals and retreats was termed the winner whereas the other crayfish was termed the loser; a dominance index was calculated as in May and Mercier (May and Mercier, 2006) as the proportion of encounters won divided by the sum of all withdrawals and avoidances. Within the established pairs, to verify that the winner could be equated with dominant status, the crayfish were viewed once daily after being paired to determine which crayfish occupied the shelter. These daily viewings lasted a minimum of 5 min, and in all instances, the winner had gained exclusive access to the shelter, suggesting no rank reversals. Shelter occupancy has been a useful indicator of dominance status in crayfish (Capelli and Hamilton, 1984; Fero and Moore, 2008; Fero et al., 2007; Herberholz et al., 2007; Loring and Hill, 1976; Martin and Moore, 2008; May and Mercier, 2007; Peeke et al., 1995; Ranta and Lindstrom, 1992). In the present study, it was only used as further evidence that we correctly understood the hierarchy. The paired crayfish remained together in the Tupperware™ container for 3 days prior to conducting any experiments on them, and no reversals in dominance status were observed, which is consistent with previous research (May and Mercier, 2006; May and Mercier, 2007).
Experimental procedures
Series I: habituation and side bias trials
To establish the duration of time required for crayfish to become accustomed to the experimental chamber and reduce its exploratory activity to a consistent level (Cadena and Tattersall, 2009a; Cadena and Tattersall, 2009b), a ‘habituation’ experiment was performed (N=10 individuals). This experiment was also used to determine whether a side preference or bias existed in the test tank. Both sides of the experimental tank were set at a constant temperature of 23°C, after which a crayfish was randomly selected from the communal tank and placed into the experimental tank. The initial side placement of the crayfish was counter-balanced, with five of the trials having the crayfish placed in the left side initially and five of the trials having the crayfish placed in the right side initially. Each crayfish was allowed to explore freely both sides of the tank for a total of 4 h.
Series II: PT assessment
To determine the PT for P. clarkii, a set of thermoregulation experiments was performed (N=10 individuals). The resulting temperature was used as the ‘preferred temperature’ in the pairing trials (Series III). To determine the PT, the experimental tank was converted into a ramping temperature, dual-choice shuttlebox (see Cadena and Tattersall, 2009b) as follows: first, the crayfish location was detected continuously using custom-built software (Fig. 1B); second, this positional information was translated by the software to allow continuous determination of which side the crayfish occupied; and third, this location information was utilised to initiate the appropriate temperature change in both chambers (positive or negative). When the crayfish occupied the left (arbitrary) side of the tank, the entire system would cool, whereas when the crayfish occupied the right (arbitrary) side, the system would warm. The two sides were always held 4°C apart from one another, thereby continually providing the crayfish the opportunity to select its PT behaviourally. To ensure that lethal temperatures would not be reached, upper and lower temperature maxima/minima were set at 35 and 12°C, respectively. The body temperature was assumed to be the instantaneous temperature of the chamber that the crayfish occupied because of the small size of the crayfish, the high heat capacity of water, and the low temperature ramp speed used (0.5°C min–1).
The initial condition for the PT experiments consisted of a constant temperature of 23°C for both the right and left chambers. A juvenile crayfish was placed randomly into either the left or right side of the choice tank. During an initial 1 h period, the crayfish was allowed to habituate to the tank, moving freely from side to side, after which a 4°C temperature difference was initiated with a temperature ramp speed of 0.5°C min–1 for a subsequent 4 h (experimental duration). Each time the crayfish moved from one side to the other, a shuttle was deemed to have occurred, and the temperature ramping slope was modified. By shuttling from one side to the other, the crayfish was always able to select a temperature that differed by ±4°C from the one currently occupied and/or choose to remain in one chamber while the water temperature slowly rose or fell to the voluntary escape temperatures.
Series IIIa: naive crayfish competition over PT
To evaluate whether dominance status influences competition over temperature, warm and cold temperature challenge experiments were conducted, where the experimental tank was organised with a simple binary choice: PT versus warm NPT (warm challenge) or PT versus cold NPT (cold challenge). Initially, this was conducted using naive pairs of crayfish, whose winner/loser status had only just been determined. Crayfish were placed randomly into a particular side of the experimental tank at a constant temperature of 23.9°C (the PT determined in Series II). A complete divider separated the crayfish. After a 30-min period to adjust to the initial period of agonistic assessment, a temperature difference was initiated (15 min), such that one side of the tank was held at the PT whereas the other side was held 4°C higher (warm challenge=27.9°C; N=10 pairs) or 4°C lower (cold challenge=19.9°C; N=10 pairs). Following this 15-min equilibrium period, the divider was removed and the crayfish were allowed to interact for a subsequent 4 h period. They were videotaped at constant temperature for only the first 30 min of this time, for subsequent scoring of winner/loser status, but their presence in either side of the tank was recorded throughout. These experiments were deemed thermal ‘challenge’ experiments, as the binary choice in temperature was PT versus an alternative temperature that was above or below the voluntary escape temperatures.
Series IIIb: established pairs and competition over PT
Crayfish that were assessed for dominance status and subsequently paired for 3 days to reinforce the hierarchy were examined in an identical matter as in Series IIIa, with the following exceptions. First, interactions between crayfish were videotaped 3 days prior to placement in the temperature-choice tank and were not videotaped thereafter. Second, in addition to similar warm (N=12 pairs) and cold challenge (N=12 pairs) experiments, a constant temperature control experiment was added to assess the interactions between dyadic crayfish behaving at their PT. This control experiment (N=8 pairs) consisted of both sides of the choice chamber being held at the PT (23.9°C). In all cases, crayfish were placed into the experimental tank, randomly with respect to side, prior to assessing the time spent in each side of the choice chamber for a similar, 4 h period of interaction.
Image capture and crayfish detection
For experiments involving solitary crayfish (Series I and II), custom-designed motion tracking software (ICFish v.2.0) was used to follow the crayfish (1 frame s–1; Fig. 1B). The white adhesive tape lining the tank allowed for a contrast-based software recognition algorithm to detect the crayfish. The location of the crayfish in Series II was used by the temperature controller to modify the direction of thermal ramping (positive vs negative). For experiments involving crayfish pairs, focal individual motion tracking was not feasible; therefore, time-lapsed webcam-based image capture (Flix v.2.0, http://www.nimisis.com/projects/flix.php) was used throughout the 4 h duration. Images were analysed manually by determining, for each time point, the position of the dominant and subordinate crayfish within their respectively occupied chambers.
Determining crayfish location for every second of the experimental duration allowed us to assess events such as when crayfish began to share a compartment and when crayfish departed from a compartment. As a result, we were able to characterise interactions between crayfish based on which animal initiated sharing chambers and which animal called off this interaction (i.e. retreated); we referred to these as induced retreats. D↘S retreats were interactions where the dominant induced the subordinate to retreat to the opposing chamber, and S↘D retreats were interactions when the subordinate induced the dominant to retreat to the opposing chamber. The mean number and duration of these induced retreats were assessed for all agonistic trials.
Data analysis
Data (crayfish location, temperature of both chambers) from all experiments were binned at 1-s intervals, allowing the timing that each animal initiated a shuttle from one side of the choice chamber to the other, as well as the proportion of time spent on each side, to be assessed. To determine whether there was a side bias in Series I, a paired t-test was performed comparing the total time the crayfish spent in the left versus the right chamber. To determine thermoregulatory parameters (Series II), the mean selected temperature was used, obtained from the instantaneous temperature of the occupied chamber. Knowledge of every shuttle event (defined as the departure from one chamber to the adjacent chamber) also allowed for the upper and lower extreme temperatures (UETs and LETs) to be determined. The PT was determined using the mean selected temperatures over the final 3 h, to account for habituation to the available temperature choices. In the Series III experiments, images from every second were observed to determine the side that the dominant and subordinate crayfish occupied throughout the experiment.
Game theory assessment
We utilised a modified hawk–dove game to describe the equilibrium game (Crowley, 2001), where the cost of fighting is incurred when sharing chambers, and the benefits of temperature are realised based on which chamber is occupied (see Table 2 for parameters). For the purposes of this study, it was not necessary to derive empirical values for all of the parameters; however, the values used for costs of fighting and benefits of temperature, as well as probability of winning contests, are based on previously used values (Crowley, 2001). Two Nash equilibria result from this type of situation (each with one animal achieving PT and the other achieving NPT; Fig. 2). Because there are two equilibria, a mixed strategy outcome can be assessed from this kind of analysis, given the relative costs of fighting and benefits of temperature. The probability that a focal crayfish will choose PT can be calculated from the payoff matrix by optimising the payoff for one individual given the other individual’s behaviour (Siegfried, 2006); when there is no temperature difference, the probability of choosing left or right is 0.5, whereas when the temperature difference is 4°C, the probability is at least 0.6 (note: this probability will change, depending on what benefits are used).
RESULTS
Habituation, side bias and overall shuttling behaviour
Animals exhibited no preference for either side of the shuttlebox (Series I); there was no significant difference in the mean time spent on the left side (55.2±20.1%) compared with the right side (t9=0.86; P=0.4125). There was an overall effect of time (F3,195=56.1, P<10–5) and treatment (F6,195=4.93, P=0.0001) on the number of shuttles, but no significant interaction effect (F18,195=1.48, P=0.1) for the Series I experiments. Upon initial placement in the experimental tank, crayfish exhibit a higher activity in the first hour, followed by an overall decline (Table 3, Fig. 3). Under all experimental series, the number of shuttles was significantly greater during the first hour than in the second, third or fourth hours (Table 3). Significantly more shuttling occurred in the thermoregulation trials than in the constant temperature pairing trials (P<0.001). In addition, the cold challenge in the established pairings also exhibited significantly more shuttling (P<0.001) than the constant temperature pairing experiments (Table 3).
Preferred temperatures
After habituating to the shuttle box at a constant temperature, and following the initiation of the ramping temperature protocol, variability in selected temperature was initially elevated for 1 h (Fig. 3), although PT itself was not significantly influenced by time (F3,27=0.831, P=0.49). There was also a significant effect of time on instantaneous velocity (F3,27=11.8, P<0.001; Fig. 3), with the first hour demonstrating a higher rate of movement. To account for the habituation required to react to the ramping shuttle box, thermal preference was expressed as the average temperature over the final 3 h period (Fig. 4). The mean PT for over this final 3 h period was 23.9±1.0°C (range 22.2–25.8°C). The mean UET and LET over this same time course were 25.9±0.9 and 21.8±1.0°C, respectively.
Agonistic interactions
Establishment of winning and dominance status as determined by the initial 30-min video analysis, yielded dominance indices (pooled across all agonistic trials) of the winning crayfish of 0.935±0.084 and the losing crayfish of 0.065±0.084, which were significantly different (paired t-test, t51=37.5, P<0.001). Winner status was not influenced by body mass, as the winning crayfish had body masses similar to those of the losing crayfish (pooled across all agonist trials: 3.34±1.22 g vs 3.27±1.28 g; F1,51=1.226, P=0.276). In the established hierarchies, none of these dominance associations reversed (based on which crayfish was observed occupying a shelter in the home cage) during the 3-day socialisation period.
The duration of agonistic behaviours was assessed by examining the duration of bouts when crayfish occupied the same chambers in the agonistic trials (Series III). These contests were classified based on which crayfish entered and which crayfish subsequently exited a particular chamber (Table 4). In the case of the established control trials, these contests all lasted for a similar length of time (21.8 to 28.7 s; Friedman RM ANOVA, χ23=0.384, P=0.943). During thermal agonistic challenges, there were distinct differences in the duration of these bouts (interaction between experimental group and direction of retreat, F3,123=16.49, P<0.001; Table 4). Bouts involving contests over PT were significantly longer if the dominant induced retreat of the subordinate (D↘S), whereas disputes where the subordinate induced retreat of the dominant (S↘D) were shorter in duration, and virtually non-existent at the NPT (although dominants did induce retreats from NPT). The actual number of induced retreats was not dependent on social status in the control, constant temperature trials (Fig. 5), whereas dominance status was a significant factor in the thermal agonism experiments (Fig. 6), with the dominant inducing far more retreats than the subordinate (interaction between experimental group and direction of retreat, F1,40=36.2, P<0.001). There was no influence of experimental treatment (naive vs established, cold or warm challenge) on the overall number of induced retreats.
Over the 4 h of examination, crayfish pairs altered their time spent together and apart during their contests over temperature (Fig. 6). In all cases, the percent time spent at NPT was either low (<20%) throughout, or declined throughout the 4 h experiment (to as low as 2%), whereas the percent time spent together at PT was 21–35%. Naive dominant crayfish were unable to gain solitary access to PT, whereas established dominant crayfish achieved almost 50% of their time at PT on their own.
Consequences of thermal choice in the context of agonistic pairings
Providing crayfish with a binary choice in temperature (PT vs warm and PT vs cold) in the context of competition still resulted in an evident preference for 23.9°C: in trials where a thermal choice existed, crayfish spent a majority of time at the PT (Fig. 7), although this was influenced by rank in established pairs. In the established hierarchies, the percentage time spent at the PT for the dominant crayfish was significantly different from that of the subordinate (established warm, Wilcoxon T0.05(2),12=6, P<0.01; established cold, Wilcoxon T0.05(2),12=3, P<0.005), but was only significantly different from the game theory expectation of 60% (see Materials and methods) for the established warm crayfish (t11=2.95, P=0.013 and t11=1.37, P=0.19). In the control, constant temperature trials, the mean percentage of time the dominant crayfish spent on the left (arbitrary) side was not significantly different from the subordinate (established control: Wilcoxon T0.05(2),8=9, P>0.2; Fig. 5), nor from game theory predictions (t7=0.25, P=0.81). In the naive pairs, however, there was no clear effect of winner/loser status on the percentage time spent at the PT; both winner and loser spent 60–65% of their time at PT (Figs 6, 7). In none of the naive trials was there a difference in the total time spent at PT between winner and loser crayfish (naive warm, Wilcoxon T0.05(2),12=21, P=0.2; naive cold, Wilcoxon T0.05(2),12=15, P=0.08), nor from a game theory expectation of 60% (t9=0.50, P=0.81 and t9=0.094, P=0.98).
The effective selected temperature (Tsel) within the pairing trials resulted in different thermal outcomes depending on social status (two-way ANOVA interaction effect, F3,40=3.49, P<0.024; Fig. 7). Although treatment had an overall significant effect as well (F3,40=64.6, P<0.001), this was driven by how the design of the warm versus cold challenges constrained available temperatures. In terms of social status, however, winner crayfish from the naive pairs did not exhibit statistically distinguishable differences in the resultant temperatures from loser crayfish. Dominant crayfish from the established pairs exhibited Tsel values that were significantly different than those of subordinate crayfish under both warm and cold challenges (Fig. 7).
DISCUSSION
In species where aggressive and/or social interactions occur, the benefits of a higher social rank are clearly seen through agonistic interactions; dominant crayfish have more access to resources such as food and shelter (Herberholz et al., 2007). In the present study, temperature was determined to be a resource over which crayfish will compete, which has not been explicitly demonstrated previously. Under both warm and cold challenge conditions, where crayfish have a choice between a preferred and a non-preferred temperature, the subordinate crayfish occupied the non-preferred temperature for the majority of the time. Interestingly, however, such an outcome of thermal agonism depends on prior establishment of dominance ranks, suggesting that competition for thermal resources depends on the social context. Naively paired crayfish continue to compete for temperature over the 4 h time course of these experiments. Hierarchies established over a 3-day period, however, resolved thermal competition within a 4 h time period, and the dominant crayfish obtained predominant access to the resource.
Because winners and losers are clearly distinguishable as early as 30 min after adult crayfish are paired (May and Mercier, 2006; May and Mercier, 2007), we might have predicted that naive juvenile crayfish pairs would resolve conflicts over temperature and dissociate between the preferred and non-preferred temperatures within the subsequent 4h period. Our data, however, indicate that even in the fourth hour of pairing, losing crayfish from naive pairs do not withdraw to the chamber with the non-preferred temperature, whereas losing crayfish of established pairs do (Fig. 6). Thus, behaviours of losers must change between the first and third days of continuous pairing. Winners also occupy the preferred temperature for longer times following 3 days of pairing (Fig. 6), which implies that their behaviour may change as well. Thus, our results indicate that behaviours of juvenile crayfish continue to change after ‘winner’ and ‘loser’ status has been determined. This is not surprising, as dominance status is associated with successful resource acquisition (Crowley, 2001). Furthermore, after dominance ranks are established in crayfish, the frequency of agonistic encounters declines rapidly for a few hours but continues to decline slowly for a few days (Huber et al., 2000; Issa et al., 1999). Finally, responses to a reflective environment are remarkably similar between winners and losers after 30 min of pairing, with both members approaching the reflection (a dominant-like behaviour), whereas responses of the losers change over 3 days of continuous pairing to indicate an increase in avoidances rather than approaches (May and Mercier, 2007). Because memory plays an important role in conspecific recognition (Crowley, 2001), the differences between naive and established pairs may be based on slowly developing neurophysiological differences (Fujimoto et al., 2011; Sneddon et al., 2000).
Thermoregulation in crayfish
In the wild, crayfish are primarily nocturnal but have been found to be active both at night and during the day (Crawshaw, 1974). Nevertheless, during the day, P. clarkii move into deeper, cooler water, with mean temperatures of 24.0°C, whereas at night they select water temperatures of up to 26.7°C (Bückle Ramirez et al., 1994). By selecting cooler waters during the day, crayfish lower their metabolic energy expenditure during their normally inactive periods, which presumably are associated with increased predator avoidance in deeper waters (Crawshaw, 1974). In contrast, during active periods, crayfish select warmer temperatures, increasing metabolic energy expenditure, with a trade-off being an increased predation risk as the crayfish that have burrowed congregate with other crayfish in shallower waters. Thus, it appears that the detection and selection of temperature is an important eco-physiological trait in P. clarkii and, presumably, other crayfish (Lagerspetz and Vainio, 2006).
In the present study, we demonstrated that P. clarkii learn to thermoregulate behaviourally within a dual-choice shuttlebox. The resulting PT of 23.9°C is very similar to previously published values [23.4°C (Espina et al., 1993)], whereas the symmetric upper and lower escape temperatures (i.e. ±2°C from the PT) support the notion that crayfish actively select and defend a temperature (Bückle Ramirez et al., 1994; Casterlin and Reynolds, 1977; Casterlin and Reynolds, 1980; Crawshaw, 1974; Payette and McGaw, 2003; Taylor, 1984; Taylor, 1990). Furthermore, crayfish required to thermoregulate are far more active than those that are held at a constant temperature, indicating that temperature selection is a strong motivating factor (Cadena and Tattersall, 2009a; Cadena and Tattersall, 2009b). Indeed, previous research has demonstrated that the crayfish Orconectes immunis will also raise their thermal preference upon infection, by as little as 1°C (Casterlin and Reynolds, 1977). This alteration in thermal preference is driven by humoral substrates (prostaglandin E1), because upon injection with the antipyretic, paracetamol, the behavioural rise in thermal preference disappears (Casterlin and Reynolds, 1980). Thus, even subtle changes in temperature are readily detected and carefully regulated in crayfish.
Behavioural response to experimental conditions and binary choices
Based on the rapid decline in the number of shuttles, an initial 1 h period was required to habituate crayfish to changes in the experimental tank, after which they behaved consistently, as Payette and McGaw (Payette and McGaw, 2003) have observed. Because the activity level of the crayfish continually decreased from the first to the fourth hour, but did not cease entirely, the last hour was most representative of conflict resolution in the agonistic trials. In the case of the naive hierarchies, the level of shuttling was similar to that observed in the established hierarchies (Table 3), suggesting that overall movement does not drive the agonistic outcome. During all experiments, the crayfish were initially very active in the experimental tank, and while they explored, they initiated encounters upon coming close to each other. The subsequent reduction in activity may have indicated some conflict resolution, but this is unlikely, because solitary crayfish reduced shuttling to a steady-state level similar to that seen in the agonistic trials. Indeed, only when both crayfish were provided with no conflicting resource (PT in both chambers) did shuttling rate decline to very low levels (Table 3). When faced with a thermal choice, it may be more efficient for the subordinate to tolerate the NPT instead of competing with the dominant crayfish and risking injury.
Interestingly, in the absence of a temperature difference, established crayfish spent little time together, presumably shuttling past one another on those few occasions (Table 4, Fig. 5). Some salient results occurred when a temperature choice was provided to crayfish pairs. For example, there are more interactive bouts resolved by dominant-induced retreat of the subordinate throughout all thermal agonism trials (Fig. 6), suggesting that the dominant drives the social dynamics. One consequence of this is that the dominant will still leave the PT to pursue the subordinate within the opposite chamber (see Table 4), suggesting that territoriality is an important factor in their assessment of temperature as a resource. Because these NPT interactions were relatively brief and similar in duration to those observed in the constant temperature trials, they may simply represent non-aggressive searching behaviour. Interestingly, game theory would predict that two Nash equilibria exist for these types of interactions (where individuals achieve the highest fitness by segregating between the two available choices); the outcome from the constant temperature agonistic trials confirmed this expectation, with nearly complete segregation occurring (Fig. 5). When temperature is a limited resource over which to compete, there is still evidence that segregation occurs, but a substantial (∼25–30%) percentage of time with both crayfish occupying the PT chamber occurs. In the naive trials, this outcome was consistent with the game theory predictions, where we estimated that the overall probability of occupying PT would be 0.6. However, established crayfish in the warm and cold challenge trials spent less time together, with the dominant predominantly occupying the PT. On the whole, this suggests that the crayfish simply alternate the chambers they occupy, driven by the dominant’s activity. In the cold challenge conditions, however, both crayfish still spent some time together at the NPT (Fig. 6), which is in sharp contrast to the warm challenge condition where they spend only 2% of time together in the warm, NPT. This result supports the contention of Vickers et al. (Vickers et al., 2011) that the higher fitness costs of warmer temperatures will drive more precise thermoregulatory behaviour, although only the dominant is able to realise this potential.
The naive crayfish, in contrast, appear to show no particular resolution over temperature. We surmise that immediately following our initial scoring of the dominance index, the introduction of new benefits and costs (by adding a temperature choice) alters the context of competition, which may alter the rate of conflict resolution and increase the tendency for dominance reversal over the course the thermal agonism experiments. Given the close size and novel establishment of the hierarchy in the naive pairs, it is reasonable to expect that a change in resource may alter the outcome. The fact that the 4 h time course responses in the established group follows a resolution-based pattern (Fig. 6), whereas the time course within the naive crayfish demonstrates fluctuations over time, highlights the very different nature of hierarchy establishment. Indeed, the payoff of each behavioural strategy (dominant vs subordinate) must change within the 3 days of hierarchy formation. For example, the dominant crayfish would be more assured of winning agonistic encounters, whereas in the naive pairs, there would still be little physiological difference between the winner and loser crayfish (Fujimoto et al., 2011; May and Mercier, 2007). According to game theory, increasing the probability (or cost) of winning a fight would increase the overall probability of the focal animal choosing the PT, which is consistent with how the established dominant crayfish performs. Overall, this suggests that the payoff of the dominant choosing the PT is higher than that for the subordinate, consistent with the evidence that contests over PT last longer, and that dominant crayfish induced subordinate crayfish to retreat more often. It is unlikely that other components (i.e. V, T and L in Table 2) of the payoff matrix (Fig. 2) change within the 3 days of hierarchy reinforcement, although we need to confirm this with empirical data.
Temperature selection during agonistic interactions
The method of assessing thermal preference in ectotherms can affect the conclusions drawn regarding thermoregulation and behavior (Anderson et al., 2007; Cadena and Tattersall, 2009b). Peck (Peck, 1985) reported that when two crayfish from an established hierarchy were placed in a temperature gradient, the dominant crayfish displaced the subordinate, and rarely did the two crayfish occupy the same section of the gradient. The average temperature difference between the dominant and the subordinate crayfish was up to 6.6°C, showing that the dominant significantly displaced the subordinate (Peck, 1985). There was, however, no constant temperature control trial to verify that space was not driving the behavioural displacement. Whereas the mean temperature for both the dominant and subordinate crayfish when they were alone was approximately 24°C, when placed together, the subordinate was usually displaced to lower temperatures with a mode of approximately 16°C (Peck, 1985). If the PT was the only driving force for agonistic behaviour, then it is expected that the subordinate crayfish would be displaced equally to higher and lower temperatures. Instead, the subordinate was displaced to colder temperatures, which suggests that the subordinate crayfish was competing to select cooler temperatures rather than warmer temperatures, but not necessarily competing over its PT. It also indicates, however, that the dominant crayfish was competing/defending the higher temperature conditions. In addition, only 41% of the dominant crayfish retained the same temperature preference in pairs compared with when they were tested alone, and in a few trials, the dominant crayfish actually selected higher temperatures (Peck, 1985). Thus, the results from thermal gradient experiments suggest that thermal preferences have a strong social context, and that the presence of conspecifics can profoundly alter attempts to select optimal temperatures behaviourally, even in the absence of overt agonistic behaviours (see Khan et al., 2010).
In the present study, a choice chamber was used containing two compartments, one with the PT and one with an NPT. The aggressive interactions resulted in much simpler spatial segregation. Nevertheless, with binary choices in temperature, dominant crayfish of established pairs are able to achieve effective selected temperatures (24.6 and 22.9°C in the warm and cold challenge trials, respectively) much closer to the PT value (23.9°C) than those achieved by subordinates (26.3 and 21.5°C, respectively). Interestingly, these resultant temperatures demonstrate that subordinate crayfish are relegated to selecting temperatures that are both above and below their UET (25.9°C) and LET (21.8°C), respectively. This suggests that the displacement of the subordinate crayfish is effectively to just beyond the mean voluntary escape temperature, whereas the dominant crayfish remains within the refractory zone for behavioural thermoregulation (Bückle Ramirez et al., 1994; Crawshaw, 1974; Fraenkel and Gunn, 1961). In short, both crayfish are able to regulate temperature, but the subordinate is forced to inhabit NPTs for longer periods of time.
These results have important implications. For many biological processes including enzyme function, metabolic rate and organism development, there is an optimal temperature range (Angilletta, 2009); temperatures above this can have detrimental effects (Taylor, 1990), particularly if metabolic demand outstrips the respiratory capacity, or if animals approach the upper lethal temperatures that are associated with cellular thermal stress. By comparing the thermal optimum for Procambarus spiculifer to the optimal growth rate, Taylor (Taylor, 1990) found that in the spring and fall, when temperatures were below the thermal optimum, the growth rate was the highest, whereas in the summer, when the environmental temperatures were above the thermal optimum, the growth rate was at a minimum. Therefore, at higher temperatures, an increased metabolic rate and a higher rate of catabolism results in less energy being available for growth and development. Even though the summer temperatures exceeded the thermal optimum of 23.4°C by only 6°C, the resulting growth rate was minimal, showing that higher temperatures may be more detrimental than lower temperatures.
It would be beneficial to test the present paradigm of PT versus NPT, but at increasingly further differences in temperature. Game theory would predict increased levels of conflict between the two crayfish as the benefit of achieving PT exceeds that of the non-optimal temperature. Although the voluntary UET and LET were within the range of the temperatures of the chambers, by using temperatures that are closer to the upper and lower limits of P. clarkii (Payette and McGaw, 2003), it would be possible to explore questions of whether warm temperatures, by virtue of being closer to lethal values, are less ‘desirable’ than cold temperatures (see Vickers et al., 2011).
Context of thermal agonism
Social compromises related to or constrained by temperature have been studied in numerous other contexts in the past. From trade-offs between oxygen and temperature in amphibians (Noland and Ultsch, 1981; Ultsch et al., 1999), thermoregulation and social aggregation or predator avoidance in reptiles (Khan et al., 2010; Lelievre et al., 2011; Vickers et al., 2011), food and temperature in daphnia (Lampert, 2005; Lampert et al., 2003), to social trade-offs with habitat selection (Hughes and Grand, 2000; Hugie and Dill, 1994; Nocera and Forbes, 2010), animals must make decisions based on minimising the costs of conflict and maximising access to the desired resource. Indeed, Hugie and Dill (Hugie and Dill, 1994) argue that game theory can explain why prey will weigh the risk of predation when making foraging choices, demonstrating that these trade-offs can be used to predict behaviours within the context of conflicting choices.
As a result of the high growth rates, dense populations and the agonistic nature of P. clarkii (Barbaresi and Gherardi, 2000), competition for resources in their natural environment is quite fierce. Furthermore, because of the temperature dependency of growth, metabolism and locomotory processes (Croll and Watts, 2004), and the fact that crayfish prefer certain temperatures, competition over specific thermal environments is expected. Indeed, thermal niches in the crayfish’s environment might be exploited in nature, or may simply augment agonistic interactions over mutually occurring resources because of the thermal resource’s attractiveness. In their natural environment, crayfish compete over resources such as food and shelter, but the relative importance of these resources may change depending on the situation. For example, when shelters are placed within a thermal gradient at temperatures above and below the favourable thermal zone (for the species Orconectes causeyi, 14–29°C), the crayfish will not occupy the shelter (Loring and Hill, 1976). However, when the shelter is placed within this zone, the mean selected temperature of the crayfish tracks the location of the shelter. This shows that to a certain extent, crayfish will exhibit a shift in their body temperature in order to obtain the benefits of obtaining a shelter. However, above and below a certain point, the importance of maintaining a favorable temperature is greater than occupying a shelter. Therefore, depending on habitat conditions, certain resources will be favoured, which will help in reinforcing the status of the dominant crayfish.
Conclusions and perspectives
Our results indicate that temperature is a resource over which crayfish will compete for occupancy when exposed to temperatures ≥4°C from the preferred condition. In other words, competition for a thermal resource involves holding and securing the resource, much like inhabiting a shelter or holding a food item. It is important to consider temperature as a possible resource (in addition to food and shelter), particularly because of the temperature dependency of ectothermic physiology. Nevertheless, agonistic pair history also determines how temperature is valued as a resource, as naive pairs continue to compete over temperature for 4 h whereas established hierarchies exhibit conflict resolution. Our experimental design is well suited for theoretical modeling, and we anticipate that more accurate mathematical models of agonistic interactions can be placed into the context of trade-offs with relevant abiotic constraints for subsequent empirical testing. To help understand the driving forces that influence the competitive context of crayfish in the wild, future research may consider whether other naturally varying environmental parameters, such as dissolved oxygen, carbon dioxide (see Bierbower and Cooper, 2010) or water flow, are also resources over which crayfish will compete.
Acknowledgements
We would like to acknowledge Viviana Cadena, Jacob Berman and Qian Long for assistance with experiments, Brock University’s Technical Services team for building the electronic shuttle box, Miriam Richards for guidance with the behavioural assessment, and Mat Vickers for consultation with parts of the analysis.
FOOTNOTES
FUNDING
This research was funded by Discovery Grants from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) to G.J.T. and A.J.M.