SUMMARY
Flight control in insects depends on self-induced image motion (optic flow), which the visual system must process to generate appropriate corrective steering maneuvers. Classic experiments in tethered insects applied rigorous system identification techniques for the analysis of turning reactions in the presence of rotating pattern stimuli delivered in open-loop. However, the functional relevance of these measurements for visual free-flight control remains equivocal due to the largely unknown effects of the highly constrained experimental conditions. To perform a systems analysis of the visual flight speed response under free-flight conditions, we implemented a `one-parameter open-loop' paradigm using `TrackFly' in a wind tunnel equipped with real-time tracking and virtual reality display technology. Upwind flying flies were stimulated with sine gratings of varying temporal and spatial frequencies, and the resulting speed responses were measured from the resulting flight speed reactions. To control flight speed, the visual system of the fruit fly extracts linear pattern velocity robustly over a broad range of spatio–temporal frequencies. The speed signal is used for a proportional control of flight speed within locomotor limits. The extraction of pattern velocity over a broad spatio–temporal frequency range may require more sophisticated motion processing mechanisms than those identified in flies so far. In Drosophila, the neuromotor pathways underlying flight speed control may be suitably explored by applying advanced genetic techniques, for which our data can serve as a baseline. Finally, the high-level control principles identified in the fly can be meaningfully transferred into a robotic context, such as for the robust and efficient control of autonomous flying micro air vehicles.
INTRODUCTION
Flying insects exhibit impressive flight control capabilities that far outperform human technology. The challenge of controlling stable flapping flight in these small animals is met with highly specialized reflexive sensorimotor pathways that need to operate at short time spans. Self-induced image motion, or optic flow (Gibson,1958), provides insects with an external frame of reference[allocentric cues (Wehner,1992)], which plays a particularly important role in the control of various aspects of flight, such as altitude and flight speed (for a review,see Collett et al., 1993). Therefore, the mechanisms underlying the processing of optic flow by the visual system and the means by which it can generate appropriate motor control signals for velocity and course control have been met with strong research interest over the past several decades (for reviews, see Egelhaaf and Kern, 2002; Srinivasan and Zhang, 2004)[for a recent paper on visual free-flight control in Drosophila, see Mronz and Lehmann (Mronz and Lehmann,2008)].
Rotational control
Classic studies of visual motion processing(Hassenstein and Reichardt,1956; Kunze, 1961; Fermi and Reichardt, 1963; Götz, 1964; Eckert, 1973) applied a black-box approach in which the visual processing mechanisms were inferred from the turning responses measured from tethered walking or flying insects in the presence of rotating gratings of varying temporal and spatial frequencies(Fig. 1A). Because tethering breaks the reafferent visual coupling normally experienced during free movements, this preparation allows arbitrary visual stimuli to be presented to an insect without the animal's behavioral response affecting the sensory input in any way [open-loop condition (e.g. Taylor et al., 2008)].
A characteristic feature of optomotor turning responses to visual gratings is their tuning to a particular angular spatial frequency [sf (the number of cycles per unit visual arc)] and a particular temporal frequency[tf (the number of cycles passing a given point on the retina per unit time)] (reviewed by Buchner,1984; Srinivasan et al.,1999). This observation, first made by Hassenstein and Reichardt in walking Chlorophanus beetles(Hassenstein and Reichardt,1956), has been repeatedly confirmed in tethered preparations using walking [e.g. Drosophila(Buchner, 1976)] and flying[Apis (Kunze, 1961); Drosophila (Götz,1964)] insects. The spatio–temporal response properties are consistent with the predictions of the influential `correlation-type' motion detector model (Fig. 1B),proposed by Reichardt (Reichardt,1961) almost 50 years ago. According to this model, motion signals are computed locally by elementary motion detectors (EMDs) from a spatio–temporal correlation of intensity signals sensed in neighboring visual channels (for reviews, see Buchner,1984; Borst and Egelhaaf,1989; Egelhaaf and Borst,1993; Hausen,1993; Borst and Haag,2002). This view is consistent with a broad body of electrophysiological evidence showing that inputs of correlation-type EMDs are integrated by the well-characterized lobula plate tangential cells (LPTC),whose outputs are believed to represent control signals for corrective steering maneuvers (for reviews, see Egelhaaf and Borst, 1993; Borst and Haag, 2002).
Translational flight control
Visual behaviors have also been explored extensively in freely flying insects (Collett et al., 1993; Srinivasan and Zhang, 2004). Most of the available data have been obtained from honeybees, which can be easily trained to fly through experimental setups under well-controlled conditions. In various experiments and behavioral contexts, honeybees have been shown to maintain a consistent flight speed with respect to a perceived pattern, irrespective of its spatial frequency and contrast(Srinivasan et al., 1991; Srinivasan et al., 1996; Baird et al., 2005).
This phenomenon, often referred to as `pattern invariance' or `velocity dependence', is not limited to honeybees. Similar evidence has emerged from free-flight studies performed in other insects, including fruit flies. David(David, 1982), for example,induced Drosophila virilis to hover stationary in a wind tunnel by manually adjusting the speed of a surrounding `barber's pole' pattern. He showed that the flies hovered at a particular `preferred' pattern speed,irrespective of whether the pattern consisted of broad (72 deg.) or narrow (40 deg.) stripes [see fig. 8 in David (David,1982)]. Furthermore, he showed that the preferred flight speed was invariant to substantial changes in headwind [see fig. 3 in David(David, 1982)], as is the case in other insect species [e.g. Aedes(Kennedy, 1939); Apis(Barron and Srinivasan, 2006)]. The strict dependence on optic flow makes flight speed control a powerful model behavior to characterize optic flow processing in the context of free flight.
Does a common visual pathway underlie rotational and translational responses?
The observed pattern-invariant flight responses of flies stand in apparent contradiction to the spatio–temporal tuning properties of optomotor turning responses and the motion-processing pathways of flies [e.g. the LPTCs in Drosophila (Joesch et al.,2008)]. Various suggestions have been put forth to explain pattern-invariant behaviors based on the known mechanisms of motion computation, including hypothetical correlation-based mechanisms(Zanker et al., 1999), or higher order motion processing circuits that extract a more accurate velocity estimate from arrays of differently tuned correlators(Srinivasan et al., 1999). More recently, the sensory and behavioral ecology of visual flight control have received increased attention. This more ethological approach suggests that the known properties of motion-sensitive visual interneurons will make more functional sense if the spatio–temporal properties of real visual scenes are taken into account, including an insect's actual body motion during flight maneuvers (O'Carroll et al.,1996; Egelhaaf et al.,1998; Egelhaaf et al.,2002; Lindemann et al.,2005; Straw et al.,2008). The relevance of experiments performed using highly controlled but partially unrealistic laboratory conditions deserves explicit attention.
The apparent discrepancy in the motion processing mechanisms observed from turning and flight speed responses may relate to the disparate conditions under which the experiments were performed. First, turning responses were measured in the presence of panoramic patterns of constant angular wavelength whereas flight speed responses were measured using planar patterns (compare Fig. 1A with Fig. 1C; also see Fig. 2B). From the vantage point of the fly, the planar pattern is geometrically distorted such that the angular wavelength (λ) of the pattern is maximal at lateral positions and decreases toward frontal and caudal positions(Fig. 2B). Second, turning responses were measured under tethered conditions, in which various sensory feedback loops relevant for flight control are broken(Taylor et al., 2008). The interruption of feedback loops, in particular haltere feedback, is the likely cause for significant behavioral artifacts observed in tethered flying fruit flies (Fry et al., 2005). Third, turning responses have been characterized from their open-loop transfer properties (see above) whereas speed responses have been characterized from the stable-state flight conditions reached under different experimental conditions. While the latter approach can provide insights into the visual mechanisms at play when the insect flies at its preferred flight speed, e.g. from the observed invariance to pattern spatial frequency and contrast, it is unsuited to explore how flight speed is actually controlled from varying optic flow conditions. For this, transient responses need to be measured in the presence of experimentally defined optic flow stimuli that vary over a large spatio–temporal parameter range.
Experimental analysis of optomotor turning and visual flight speed responses. (A) Optomotor turning response paradigm. An insect is tethered within a patterned rotating drum and its steady-state turning responses are measured. Because the body is fixed, the behavioral reactions have no effect on the perceived stimulus (open-loop condition). (B) Idealized spatio–temporal tuning of optomotor turning reactions. The response surface represents the steady-state output of a basic correlation-type motion detector scheme, calculated using the equations applicable for sine grating stimuli provided by Borst and Bahde (Borst and Bahde, 1986). Normalized response strength (green and dark red correspond to values between 0 and 1, respectively) is plotted as a function of the temporal frequency (tf, in s–1) and angular spatial frequency (sf, in deg.–1) of sine grating patterns. Optomotor turning responses are maximally elicited by a particular optimal temporal frequency (tfopt) and optimal spatial frequency (sfopt). (C) Standard flight speed paradigm. An insect is induced to fly along a patterned channel and its flight speed is measured under steady-state conditions. The insect adjusts its flight speed according to the perceived visual feedback from the pattern (closed-loop condition). (D) Flight speed measurements in the presence of sine grating patterns with varying linear spatial frequency (SF, in m–1). Insects adjust their flight speed as to hold constant the ratio of temporal and linear spatial frequency, such that measurements(red circles) fall on a diagonal line in the TF–SF parameter space, which corresponds to the insect's preferred velocity(Vpref=TF/SF) in m s–1. (E)One-parameter open-loop paradigm, which allows arbitrary patterns to be defined with respect to the fly's body coordinates, irrespective of its flight speed. Open-loop stimulation requires measuring the fly's position (symbolized by the two cameras) and controlling the pattern in real-time (black arrow).(F) Open-loop stimulation is required to characterize the transfer properties of the visual speed response in the two-dimensional TF–SFparameter space.
Experimental analysis of optomotor turning and visual flight speed responses. (A) Optomotor turning response paradigm. An insect is tethered within a patterned rotating drum and its steady-state turning responses are measured. Because the body is fixed, the behavioral reactions have no effect on the perceived stimulus (open-loop condition). (B) Idealized spatio–temporal tuning of optomotor turning reactions. The response surface represents the steady-state output of a basic correlation-type motion detector scheme, calculated using the equations applicable for sine grating stimuli provided by Borst and Bahde (Borst and Bahde, 1986). Normalized response strength (green and dark red correspond to values between 0 and 1, respectively) is plotted as a function of the temporal frequency (tf, in s–1) and angular spatial frequency (sf, in deg.–1) of sine grating patterns. Optomotor turning responses are maximally elicited by a particular optimal temporal frequency (tfopt) and optimal spatial frequency (sfopt). (C) Standard flight speed paradigm. An insect is induced to fly along a patterned channel and its flight speed is measured under steady-state conditions. The insect adjusts its flight speed according to the perceived visual feedback from the pattern (closed-loop condition). (D) Flight speed measurements in the presence of sine grating patterns with varying linear spatial frequency (SF, in m–1). Insects adjust their flight speed as to hold constant the ratio of temporal and linear spatial frequency, such that measurements(red circles) fall on a diagonal line in the TF–SF parameter space, which corresponds to the insect's preferred velocity(Vpref=TF/SF) in m s–1. (E)One-parameter open-loop paradigm, which allows arbitrary patterns to be defined with respect to the fly's body coordinates, irrespective of its flight speed. Open-loop stimulation requires measuring the fly's position (symbolized by the two cameras) and controlling the pattern in real-time (black arrow).(F) Open-loop stimulation is required to characterize the transfer properties of the visual speed response in the two-dimensional TF–SFparameter space.
Experimental setup and measurement procedure. (A) TrackFly. The free-flight experimental setup consisted of a wind tunnel (only the working section is shown) equipped with a real-time 3-D position tracking system (Trackit 3D)(Fry et al., 2000; Fry et al., 2004) and custom-programmed graphical rendering software (based on the VisionEgg)(Straw and O'Carroll, 2003; Straw et al., 2006). Flies were induced to fly upwind (red dotted arrow shows the flight direction of fly; blue arrows indicate wind direction), while their position was tracked in real time (green arrows pointing from cameras). Visual stimuli were projected onto the sidewalls via pairs of mirrors (yellow arrows show the light path from the projector to the screen for one side of the wind tunnel). The virtual reality features of TrackFly were used to implement a one-parameter visual open-loop paradigm [see text and Fry et al.(Fry et al., 2008) for further details]. (B) Definition of linear and angular coordinate systems. A plan view of the screens (sine gratings are represented by stripes for clarity) is shown to scale. The linear spatial frequency (SF, in m–1)of the pattern displayed on the screen corresponds to the inverse of the linear spatial pattern period (λ, in m). In the example, SF=10 m–1 and thus Λ=0.1 m. The angular period (λ,unit: deg.) of the displayed patterns depends on the azimuth, with λdecreasing toward frontal and caudal positions (note red arrows). Angular spatial frequency (sf, in deg.–1), the inverse ofλ, therefore increases toward frontal and caudal positions. The linear wavelength (λ1, λ2) and linear spatial frequency SFremain constant. (C) Sample acceleration responses and parameter extraction. The time course of body position along the wind tunnel (red traces) is shown for 11 measurements performed under identical stimulus conditions. Flies were first held near the middle of the wind tunnel by controlling the pattern speed(t<0). At t≥0, the flies were stimulated in open-loop(TF=4 s–1; SF=12.5 m–1;stimulus condition is marked with an asterisk in Fig. 4A). Flies reacted to the back-to-front image motion by accelerating forward, as indicated by the exponential increase of the position function (red traces, right part of the plot). Mean acceleration of each sample was measured from the fitting parameters of a parabola (t>0.1 s, black traces). These values were then averaged over the trials to obtain the response strength for a single TF–SF combination (each marked with a dot in Fig. 3 and Fig. 4). Figure modified from Fry et al. (Fry et al.,2008).
Experimental setup and measurement procedure. (A) TrackFly. The free-flight experimental setup consisted of a wind tunnel (only the working section is shown) equipped with a real-time 3-D position tracking system (Trackit 3D)(Fry et al., 2000; Fry et al., 2004) and custom-programmed graphical rendering software (based on the VisionEgg)(Straw and O'Carroll, 2003; Straw et al., 2006). Flies were induced to fly upwind (red dotted arrow shows the flight direction of fly; blue arrows indicate wind direction), while their position was tracked in real time (green arrows pointing from cameras). Visual stimuli were projected onto the sidewalls via pairs of mirrors (yellow arrows show the light path from the projector to the screen for one side of the wind tunnel). The virtual reality features of TrackFly were used to implement a one-parameter visual open-loop paradigm [see text and Fry et al.(Fry et al., 2008) for further details]. (B) Definition of linear and angular coordinate systems. A plan view of the screens (sine gratings are represented by stripes for clarity) is shown to scale. The linear spatial frequency (SF, in m–1)of the pattern displayed on the screen corresponds to the inverse of the linear spatial pattern period (λ, in m). In the example, SF=10 m–1 and thus Λ=0.1 m. The angular period (λ,unit: deg.) of the displayed patterns depends on the azimuth, with λdecreasing toward frontal and caudal positions (note red arrows). Angular spatial frequency (sf, in deg.–1), the inverse ofλ, therefore increases toward frontal and caudal positions. The linear wavelength (λ1, λ2) and linear spatial frequency SFremain constant. (C) Sample acceleration responses and parameter extraction. The time course of body position along the wind tunnel (red traces) is shown for 11 measurements performed under identical stimulus conditions. Flies were first held near the middle of the wind tunnel by controlling the pattern speed(t<0). At t≥0, the flies were stimulated in open-loop(TF=4 s–1; SF=12.5 m–1;stimulus condition is marked with an asterisk in Fig. 4A). Flies reacted to the back-to-front image motion by accelerating forward, as indicated by the exponential increase of the position function (red traces, right part of the plot). Mean acceleration of each sample was measured from the fitting parameters of a parabola (t>0.1 s, black traces). These values were then averaged over the trials to obtain the response strength for a single TF–SF combination (each marked with a dot in Fig. 3 and Fig. 4). Figure modified from Fry et al. (Fry et al.,2008).
`One-parameter open-loop' free-flight paradigm
To this end, we implemented a novel behavioral paradigm that allowed us to measure visual speed responses under free-flight conditions, while controlling pattern motion in open-loop (Fig. 1E) using `TrackFly' (Fig. 2) (Fry et al.,2008). To do so, we equipped a wind tunnel with a real-time 3-D path tracking system, i.e. Trackit 3D (Biobserve GmbH, Bonn, Germany) (see also Fry et al., 2004) and an image rendering system, i.e. the VisionEgg (v. 1.0; www.visonegg.org)(Straw et al., 2006). By experimentally de-coupling the retinal slip from the fly's flight speed, we were able to measure the open-loop transfer function over a broad range of spatio–temporal frequencies (Fig. 1F). Automation from software control allowed us to collect a large data set of visual responses (n=12,711 trials) to a broad range of visual stimulus parameters.
Our results show that visual control of flight speed in fruit flies depends on linear pattern velocity (V=TF/SF) over a broad range of spatio–temporal frequencies (for definitions of V, TFand SF, see Coordinate system in Materials and methods). Furthermore,visual responses are largely invariant to spatial frequency composition, image contrast and wind speed. Our results suggest the presence of a sophisticated motion-processing pathway that is able to robustly extract V as a control signal for flight speed control.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Animals
Fruit flies (Drosophila melanogaster Meigen) were obtained from a stock descended from a wild-caught population of 200 mated females. A standard breeding procedure was adopted, in which flies were bred from 25 females and 10 males and reared on standard nutritive medium on a 12 h:12 h light:dark cycle. 2–4-day-old male and female flies were isolated and deprived of food, but not water, for 12–16 h preceding an experiment. Experiments were performed during the first 6 h of the subjective day.
TrackFly experimental setup
Experiments were performed using TrackFly; a wind tunnel equipped with virtual reality display technology, described in more detail elsewhere(Fry et al., 2008). In the present study, we provide an overview of the system components that are principally relevant for the understanding of the concepts and methodology applied in the present study.
Wind tunnel
The behavioral tests were performed in a commercial, open-circuit,closed-throat wind tunnel (Engineering Laboratory Design, Inc., Lake City, MN,USA). The wind tunnel provided a laminar airflow in a working section made of clear acrylic, 1.55 m in length and ≤0.305 m in width and height. Standard tests were performed using a wind speed of 0.29 m s–1. To motivate the flies to fly upwind, we vaporized an attractant odor (`Kressi'herb vinegar, diluted to 5% water solution) using an ultrasonic humidifier(Boneco, Plaston AG, Widnau, Switzerland) at a rate of ∼7.2 mg s–1 from four nozzles positioned in front of the air intake end of the wind tunnel. This procedure provided a homogenous dispersal of the odor, thus preventing a plume structure or a concentration gradient that the flies could have used as additional cues.
Real-time position tracking
Flies were tracked from above using Trackit 3D(Fry et al., 2004), equipped with two infrared-sensitive video cameras. Homogeneous back lighting was provided from a custom-built lamp emitting in the long wavelength spectrum(>700 nm). Fruit flies are comparatively insensitive to long wavelengths(Heisenberg and Buchner, 1977; Stark and Johnson, 1980) and,consistently, we did not notice any effects resulting from the light shining from below. The three-dimensional (3-D) position of single flies was transferred with short delay to a client computer at a rate of 50 Hz using a TCP/IP (Transmission Control Protocol/Internet Protocol) network interface.
Image rendering and display
On the client computer, visual stimuli were rendered using custom programmed software based on the VisionEgg open-source image rendering library(Straw and O'Carroll, 2003; Straw et al., 2006). The images were displayed at a refresh rate of 60 Hz on a flicker-free LCD (liquid crystal display) projector (Sony, VPL-ES1, Tokyo, Japan). The image was split and projected via mirrors onto tracing paper screens (1.0 m×0.3 m) attached to the sidewalls of the wind tunnel(Fig. 2A). Various calibrations and control measurements were performed to ensure that the patterns defined in the software were displayed faithfully onto the wind tunnel screens. The average latency of TrackFly between measuring the fly's position and displaying the position-dependent stimulus was 0.038 s.
Coordinate system
Throughout the text, capitalized and non-capitalized symbols are used to denote linear and angular metrics, respectively. We define the spatial frequency [SF (unit: m–1)] of the displayed sine grating, with reference to the sidewalls of the wind tunnel, as the number of cycles per unit length of the visual display(Fig. 2). Accordingly, linear pattern wavelength (λ)is given by Λ=1/SF (unit: m).+X denotes upwind direction. The temporal frequency (TF) of the pattern is defined as the number of cycles passing a given point of the display per unit time (unit: s–1) positive upwind. In the present experiments, TF and SF were varied systematically.
Due to perspective distortion of the pattern as viewed by the fly, the angular spatial frequency (sf) and angular velocity (v) are not constant across the visual field but instead vary with azimuth. Alternatively, we could have stimulated the flies with a single sf by rendering a cylindrical projection of the pattern centered on the fly(Fry et al., 2008). Such experiments are indeed useful to test specific hypotheses about optic flow processing (N.R. and S.N.F., in preparation); however, this was not the aim of the present study. To instead explore how the fly controls its flight speed with respect to realistic optic flow conditions, we displayed planar patterns;these appear largest and fastest in the lateral field of view, just like any object a fly passes by under natural free-flight conditions (e.g. David, 1982; Srinivasan et al., 1996).
Measurement procedure
Process automation
The detailed measurement of a response surface over a broad TF–SF parameter space required a large number of trials. We automated the measurement procedure by implementing the `optomotor clamp' procedure inspired by David's(David, 1982) earlier approach. For this, we manipulated the perceived visual flight speed of the flies by varying the horizontal speed of a sine grating pattern(SF=6.66 m–1), depending on the fly's position in the wind tunnel. If the fly was too far upwind, we simulated fast, forward flight by increasing the downwind pattern speed, in response to which the fly reduced flight speed and drifted downwind. Likewise, if the fly was too far downwind, we simulated slow, forward flight by decreasing the downwind pattern speed, in response to which the fly increased flight speed and moved upwind. In consequence, the fly was kept hovering near the middle of the wind tunnel(X=0), where the pattern was moving at the flies' preferred speed. A test was performed as soon as a fly was measured to hover stably near the center of the wind tunnel.
`One-parameter open-loop' testing paradigm
Individual flies were stimulated for 1 s with a moving sine grating,defined by TF and SF. To hold TF constant with respect to the fly (open-loop condition), the pattern phase was continually adjusted according to the fly's current position along the wind tunnel. Thus,only one parameter, the TF of the horizontal optic flow stimulus, was controlled in open-loop, i.e. decoupled from the fly's resulting behavioral response. All other sensory feedback, such as mechanosensory feedback from the halteres or antennae, remained under natural closed-loop conditions. This measurement paradigm is accurately termed `one-parameter open-loop' condition but for simplicity we will use the term `open-loop condition' throughout the paper. The fly's 3-D positions were logged, together with the stimulus conditions, to a data file for later analysis.
The testing protocol consisted of four test conditions and a control condition (TF=–2 Hz, SF=10 m–1),which were repeated sequentially until sufficient data were acquired for each pattern condition. We used the control condition to detect any significant differences in response strength that might have resulted from inter-individual differences or uncontrolled experimental conditions; however,this never occurred and the variability of the responses was consistently low(Fig. 4C). We therefore pooled the data and treated them as independent. Our approach was further justified by the large number of trials performed (e.g. n=12,711 for Fig. 3). Although the exact number of flies tested is unknown, we estimated it to be in the order of N=1000, based on our observation that a single fly contributed about 10 trials on average.
Spatio–temporal tuning of speed responses. Body acceleration is shown color coded as a function of the open-loop temporal frequency (TF)and linear spatial frequency (SF) of presented sine grating stimuli. Contour lines show iso-response curves at intervals of 0.5 ms–2. The response surface was obtained by linearly interpolating over 435 parameter conditions, each indicated by a dot. These were calculated from a total of 12,711 trials.
Spatio–temporal tuning of speed responses. Body acceleration is shown color coded as a function of the open-loop temporal frequency (TF)and linear spatial frequency (SF) of presented sine grating stimuli. Contour lines show iso-response curves at intervals of 0.5 ms–2. The response surface was obtained by linearly interpolating over 435 parameter conditions, each indicated by a dot. These were calculated from a total of 12,711 trials.
Tests with a photographic image
We also tested the flies' responses to a moving photographic image containing a naturalistic mixture of spatial frequencies. The image is one of a set in which each image evoked similar responses of LPTCs in the hoverfly[image D in Straw et al. (Straw et al.,2008); see Movie 1 in supplementary material]. In this case, the pattern velocity was controlled in open-loop as to maintain a constant V = TF/SF with respect to the fly.
Data analysis
The flies responded to open-loop regressive (back-to-front) motion stimuli with a constant forward acceleration (Fig. 2C) that depended on stimulus conditions. A constant acceleration is consistent with a constant net forward thrust, suggesting that the aerodynamic effects of varying air speed on the wings and the body are compensated. This view is consistent with the previously mentioned invariance of flies to wind speed (see Introduction).
Response surface in TF–SF parameter space
Because the spatio–temporal properties of a moving sine grating are uniquely described by its TF and SF, the measured responses are appropriately represented in a two-dimensional parameter space using TF and SF as Cartesian coordinates(Fig. 3). We interpolated the accelerations measured from 435 different pattern conditions(n=12,711 trials) using Matlab's (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA,USA) griddata function (linear method). The accelerations are represented in color code, with `hot' and `cold' colors representing up- and downwind accelerations, respectively (Fig. 3; see Fig. 2B for sign conventions).
RESULTS
Open-loop response transfer function
Applying an automated one-parameter open-loop paradigm, we characterized the flight speed response from its behavioral open-loop transfer function,measured from 435 different pattern combinations and a total of 12,711 individual trials. Maximal upwind accelerations (upper left of Fig. 3) are observed for TF around 8 s–1 and SF around 5 m–1. At SF≈25 m–1, we observe a response inversion, which is consistent with spatial aliasing at the spatial resolution limit of the fly's eye. The response structure of downwind accelerations (lower left of Fig. 3) is similar, albeit less clear and shifted toward lower SFs.
Superficially, the measured speed response tuning is reminiscent of that computed from a basic correlation-type motion detector (compare upper half of Fig. 3 with Fig. 1B) for rotating patterns. First, the measured speed responses show a response plateau in a limited range of TF and SF similar to the response maximum predicted by the correlator model due to its spatio–temporal band-pass filter properties (Fig. 1B). Second,the speed responses diminish above SF≈25 m–1 and reverse direction (note blue color code indicating backward accelerations for SF around 30–55 m–1), as predicted by the correlation model.
Velocity dependence and locomotor limits. (A) Response surface relevant to forward acceleration responses. The surface and iso-response curves are re-plotted from Fig. 3 (subset of 166 parameter conditions and 6245 trials); the zero iso-response line shown in bold and indicated with a black arrow. Labeled diagonal lines represent pattern iso-velocity lines (pattern velocity, V=TF/SF). Note the close correspondence between the iso-response curves and the pattern iso-velocity lines for pattern speeds up to 0.5 m s–1. Colored arcs follow the gradient of pattern velocity for varying radii in the spatio–temporal parameter space. (B) Pattern-invariant response properties. The response surface was evaluated along the four colored arcs shown in A to obtain response strength as a function of pattern slip speed,which is plotted in the respective colors together with the standard deviation. The responses depend on pattern velocity over a large range of the TF–SF parameter space. (C) Velocity-dependent responses to sine gratings and a naturalistic image. The mean responses to sine gratings moving at a particular velocity were obtained by averaging the response surface along the respective pattern iso-velocity line (within the range of the red and blue arcs). These are plotted against pattern velocity in black together with the standard deviation (gray shaded area). The red line shows a linear regression for pattern velocities up to V<0.6 m s–1(R2=0.985; see text for further details). Mean responses to a photographic image moving at various velocities are plotted in green together with the standard deviation (shaded). (D) Air speed saturation. Median and 25 percentile values of maximal forward accelerations induced with strong motion stimuli (V⩾0.6 m s–1; SF<25 m–1) in presence of low (left: –0.29 m s–1; N=941) and high (right: –0.73 m s–1; N=45) wind speeds. The measured accelerations reveal a significant dependence on the wind speed, suggesting an air-speed-dependent response saturation (Wilcoxon test, P<0.001).
Velocity dependence and locomotor limits. (A) Response surface relevant to forward acceleration responses. The surface and iso-response curves are re-plotted from Fig. 3 (subset of 166 parameter conditions and 6245 trials); the zero iso-response line shown in bold and indicated with a black arrow. Labeled diagonal lines represent pattern iso-velocity lines (pattern velocity, V=TF/SF). Note the close correspondence between the iso-response curves and the pattern iso-velocity lines for pattern speeds up to 0.5 m s–1. Colored arcs follow the gradient of pattern velocity for varying radii in the spatio–temporal parameter space. (B) Pattern-invariant response properties. The response surface was evaluated along the four colored arcs shown in A to obtain response strength as a function of pattern slip speed,which is plotted in the respective colors together with the standard deviation. The responses depend on pattern velocity over a large range of the TF–SF parameter space. (C) Velocity-dependent responses to sine gratings and a naturalistic image. The mean responses to sine gratings moving at a particular velocity were obtained by averaging the response surface along the respective pattern iso-velocity line (within the range of the red and blue arcs). These are plotted against pattern velocity in black together with the standard deviation (gray shaded area). The red line shows a linear regression for pattern velocities up to V<0.6 m s–1(R2=0.985; see text for further details). Mean responses to a photographic image moving at various velocities are plotted in green together with the standard deviation (shaded). (D) Air speed saturation. Median and 25 percentile values of maximal forward accelerations induced with strong motion stimuli (V⩾0.6 m s–1; SF<25 m–1) in presence of low (left: –0.29 m s–1; N=941) and high (right: –0.73 m s–1; N=45) wind speeds. The measured accelerations reveal a significant dependence on the wind speed, suggesting an air-speed-dependent response saturation (Wilcoxon test, P<0.001).
In conclusion, the conspicuous similarities between the measured response surface and the tuning properties of a basic correlation detector circuit reflect physiological constraints of the visuomotor system rather than a common motion processing mechanism.
A further constraint explains the previously mentioned differences in upwind- and downwind-directed speed responses (positive and negative accelerations in Fig. 3,respectively). When accelerating upwind, the flies kept their body orientation closely aligned with the wind tunnel long axis, as confirmed with high-speed recordings made from the side through a slit in one of the display screens(see Movie 1 in supplementary material). There must be limits, however, to the degree with which the flies compensate increasing tailwinds with deceleration,as this would eventually lead to the somewhat paradoxical situation of flies flying backward with respect to the surrounding air. Indeed, the flies tolerated only little front-to-back retinal slip before they reversed their body orientation, as revealed by high-speed video analysis (data not shown). Likewise, Kennedy (Kennedy,1939) identified a threshold for front-to-back pattern motion,above which mosquitoes reversed their flight direction and flew downwind. Because the retinal stimulation changed along with the fly's turning responses, the data are difficult to interpret and were therefore excluded from further analysis.
Visual encoding properties of the speed response
To gain insight into the strategy of visual control of flight speed, we focused our analysis on forward-directed acceleration responses in a reduced parameter range that is most likely to be relevant for flight speed control(Fig. 4A). Because previous research has shown that preferred flight speed depends on pattern velocity(see Introduction), it is meaningful to evaluate the response surface in view of identical pattern velocities represented by the different combinations of TF and SF. The linear velocity V of a sine grating corresponds to the ratio of its TF and SF(V=TF/SF). Therefore, all sine gratings that move at a particular V fall onto a diagonal line through the origin with slope V=TF/SF. For example, the 0.5 m s–1 pattern iso-velocity line connects all TF and SF coordinates for which V=TF/SF=0.5 m s–1, such as (6,12) and (2,4).
Pattern iso-velocity lines for –0.1≤V≤1 m s–1 are shown as labeled white lines superimposed over the response surface shown in Fig. 4A. Whereas the Cartesian TF–SF coordinate system describes the patterns from their temporal and spatial frequency composition, the superimposed polar coordinate system describes the patterns in terms of their velocity, with the angular coordinateα=tan(V). The radial coordinate
Set-point transfer properties
We first evaluate the measured response surface in view of the previous finding that the `preferred' flight speed of free-flying insects is invariant to the spatial pattern properties (see Introduction). In control terms, the`preferred' flight speed represents the controller's set point, which defines the stable equilibrium under normal closed-loop conditions. In an open-loop input–output function, the set point is identified from a zero crossing of a response function, with a positive slope indicating a stable equilibrium point (Strogatz, 1994). If consistent with previous free-flight data, we would expect our response surface to reveal a zero response contour line (the zero-crossing) that is flanked by a positive response gradient (required for closed-loop stability)and follows a pattern iso-velocity line (required for pattern invariance).
As shown in the lower part of Fig. 4A, the zero response contour line (shown bold and marked with a black arrow) indeed runs roughly diagonally, corresponding to a pattern velocity of approximately –0.15 m s–1 (i.e. front-to-back). This preferred pattern velocity is similar to the preferred flight speed of D. melanogaster measured in free flight(Tammero and Dickinson, 2002). Our data show that the preferred pattern velocity of D. melanogasterspans a roughly 8-fold range of SF (between about 2–16 m–1), consistent with previous findings in honeybees(Baird et al., 2005).
The findings based on the zero response iso-line measured under open-loop conditions are consistent with previous data measured under closed-loop flight conditions, validating our experimental approach for a functional analysis of free-flight control.
Pattern velocity as a control signal for corrective speed maneuvers
The particular shape of the response surface reveals the spatio–temporal pattern properties relevant for speed control and how corrective speed maneuvers depend on them. Over a broad range of the response surface shown in Fig. 4A, the response iso-lines are roughly aligned with the diagonally oriented pattern iso-velocity lines, suggesting a close correspondence between the response strength and pattern velocity, rather than a response tuning to a particular combination of TF and SF.
To quantify the dependence of response strength on the open-loop pattern velocity, or retinal slip speed (Vslip), we evaluated the response surface along four radial paths in the two-dimensional TF–SF parameter space (red, green, dark blue and light blue arcs in Fig. 4A, with r values of 4, 8, 12 and 16, respectively). The responses, sampled over a broad TF–SF parameter space, show a similar dependence on Vslip and are statistically indistinguishable from one another. The responses increase with Vslip and saturate at around 3 m s–2 for Vslip>0.6 m s–1(Fig. 4B). The variance of the mean responses (likewise linearly interpolated) is shown with error bars for the 48 measurement conditions. The standard deviation is roughly constant at about ±0.5 m s–2 and increases to about twice this value at high retinal slip speeds.
Averaging the responses measured for radii between 8 and 16 (i.e. along the pattern iso-velocity lines between the red and light blue arcs in Fig. 4A) reveals a linear-dependence (R2=0.985) of response strength for Vslip<0.6 m s–1, above which the responses saturate at around 3 m s–2 (black line in Fig. 4C). Within the linear range, therefore, the flies' accelerations depend directly on the linear pattern velocity V=TF/SF as the relevant visual control parameter for flight speed.
Linearity in response provides constant reaction time
T relates only to the period of constant acceleration and does not include a constant response time lag of approximately 0.10 s (see Fig. 2C, analysis not shown). We therefore estimate the total time required by the fly to correct a visual perturbation to lie slightly above 0.37 s. A more detailed analysis of the control properties will be presented elsewhere (N.R. and S.N.F., in preparation).
Speed responses are robust for SF composition and image contrast
Previous research has explored the possibility that pattern invariance could arise in a correlation-based motion-processing pathway from the processing of images with a broad spatial frequency spectrum, as is found in natural visual environments of flies (see Introduction). Indeed, stimulation with a broad SF spectrum is expected to result in more meaningful responses if non-linearities in the visual system operated on various spatial frequency components. To test this possibility, we measured flight speed responses in the presence of a moving photographic image that was used in a previous electrophysiological study to reflect the broad spatial frequency spectrum of a fly's natural habitat (Straw et al., 2008). We controlled the image velocity in open-loop and measured the resulting acceleration responses as before. Whereas the previous experiments used a single TF–SF combination for individual trials, the image presentation experiments tested the responses to a simultaneous presentation of TF–SF combinations,corresponding to numerous points along the respective iso-velocity line in the TF–SF parameter space.
The responses to the photographic image were indistinguishable from the averaged responses measured using sine gratings (compare green and black traces in Fig. 4C). We therefore conclude that the computation of pattern velocity by the fly's visual system is largely invariant for spatial frequency content of the planar patterns. Single SFs and a broadband photographic image both lead to robust flight velocity control.
Additionally, although image contrast differed considerably between the sine grating (Michelson contrast: C=0.5) and the photographic image [see image C in Straw et al. (Straw et al.,2008); see also for a discussion of contrast metrics for natural images], this did not lead to a significant difference in response strength,suggesting that speed responses are largely contrast invariant, consistent with recent findings in honeybees in a similar behavioral context(Baird et al., 2005). A biologically plausible explanation for contrast invariance could lie in contrast saturation in the early visual system(Dror et al., 2001), although other mechanisms are known to be responsible for contrast insensitivity observed in the tangential cells of other fly species(Straw et al., 2008).
Response saturation
As shown above, accelerations reached a plateau of about 3 m s–2 at pattern speeds above 0.6 m s–1. The underlying cause of the response saturation is relevant to the understanding of the underlying control mechanisms. We consider two likely explanations for the saturation. First, saturation could occur in the visual pathways, limiting the encoding of pattern speeds to a particular value. Second, the saturation could result from locomotor limits, either due to an upper limit in acceleration of 3 ms–2 or in the maximum air speed attainable by the fly due to increasing drag acting on the wings and body [for drag effects on the wing, see also Hesselberg and Lehmann(Hesselberg and Lehmann,2007)].
To distinguish between these two possibilities, we compared the accelerations reached under standard wind conditions (–0.290 m s–1) with those measured in the presence of an increased headwind (–0.73 m s–1)(Fig. 4D). Increasing the headwind caused a significant reduction of the acceleration from 2.67 to 1.32 m s–2 (Fig. 4D), indicating that the response saturation did not result from a limit of pattern velocities extracted by the visual system but instead depended on the air speed. Apparently, the flies reached an upper limit of air speeds at which they were able or willing to fly. When flying below their maximum air speed, the flies' acceleration was invariant to the wind speeds tested (data not shown), in accordance with results published for various insect species [e.g. Aedes (Kennedy,1939); Aphis (Kennedy and Thomas, 1974); D. virilis(David, 1982); Apis(Barron and Srinivasan,2006)].
DISCUSSION
Open-loop stimulation and response tuning
To gain a functional understanding of optic flow processing for flight speed control, we applied a one-parameter open-loop paradigm in free-flying fruit flies. A single fly at a time was induced to hover near the center of the wind tunnel, where we briefly stimulated it with a moving sine grating. During the 1 s stimulation, we compensated for changes in the fly's position by adjusting the phase of the displayed pattern in real time, such that the temporal frequency of the pattern was held constant with respect to the fly. This experimentally induced open-loop condition allowed us to characterize the fly's visual flight speed response using retinal temporal frequency and spatial frequency as independent control parameters(Fig. 3; Fig. 4A).
While our method allowed us to characterize an important free-flight reflex in a two-dimensional spatio–temporal parameter space, the one-parameter open-loop condition was experimentally induced, raising the question of whether our measurements were subject to artefacts, resulting from the highly artificial experimental conditions.
A first question relates to other sensory modalities, such as mechanosensory feedback from the halteres and antennae and olfactory cues,which remained in closed-loop and might have provided conflicting cues. The experimentally induced disparity between the visual and other sensory inputs is in fact by no means unnatural and mimics a control scenario faced by a fly flying upwind quite closely. Whether flying against a constant wind or in still air, a fly adjusts its air speed so as to maintain a constant`preferred' retinal pattern slip speed [see fig. 3 in David(David, 1982)] (see also Introduction). This situation is similar to our pre-test condition, in which the fly was induced to hover near the middle of the wind tunnel, where the visual pattern motion matched the fly's preferred retinal slip speed. In a natural environment, a gust of wind from the front could easily cause the fly to momentarily slow down or even be carried backward, in which case the fly would perceive regressive (back-to-front) retinal slip. At this moment, the retinal slip depends largely on the strength of the wind gust and not the fly's behavior; a situation closely corresponding to the open-loop condition we implemented using TrackFly.
While mechanosensory input is likely to provide information about the fly's air speed [e.g. from the antennae(Gewecke, 1967; Taylor and Krapp, 2008)],there is no mechanism besides vision known to provide the fly with a reference for its ground speed. In this context, it is interesting to consider the role of the halteres, which sense the Coriolis forces associated with angular body velocity but probably not the much weaker forces resulting from linear accelerations (Pringle, 1948; Nalbach, 1993; Nalbach and Hengstenberg,1994). The halteres are therefore part of an inner control loop mediating changes in body pitch, as required for flight speed control(David, 1978; Dickson et al., 2008), but are unlikely to affect the outer control loop, which appears to be driven by pattern velocity alone (Fig. 5). Our one-parameter open-loop paradigm therefore selectively provided experimental access to the outer, visually mediated control loop,while other sensory modalities remained under realistic closed-loop conditions, as required for realistic free-flight control.
A second question relates to the de-coupling of retinal slip speed from the fly's behavior, which to be perfect would require controlling the stimulus with zero time delay. Instead, the total system latency of TrackFly wasτ=38 ms (see Materials and methods), which is in the order of the fastest visual behaviors documented in free-flying flies [e.g. 30 ms in chasing Fannia (Land and Collett,1974)]. The difference between the intended and the actual stimulation was previously calculated for the present testing paradigm,assuming a constant τ=38 ms and that the flies responded with a constant acceleration (Fig. 2B). After 20 ms, the actual slip speed reached a constant level that was reduced by 7.6%compared with the desired slip speed [see fig. 6 in Fry et al.(Fry et al., 2008)]. Correcting our results for the measured inaccuracy of the stimulation, the GOL is reduced from 3.73 to 3.45 s–1 and the duration of the acceleration response is correspondingly increased from 270 to 290 ms. Otherwise our conclusions should not be affected by this systematic error.
Flight speed control scheme. Optic flow was delivered in open-loop using TrackFly, such as to deliver a constant pattern slip speed during the short trials. The visual system (`V') extracts a speed signal, which is converted to a flight speed command. The actual measured flight speed is subject to air speed saturation. A quantitative control model and experimental verification under open- and closed-loop conditions is to be presented elsewhere (N.R. and S.N.F., in preparation).
Flight speed control scheme. Optic flow was delivered in open-loop using TrackFly, such as to deliver a constant pattern slip speed during the short trials. The visual system (`V') extracts a speed signal, which is converted to a flight speed command. The actual measured flight speed is subject to air speed saturation. A quantitative control model and experimental verification under open- and closed-loop conditions is to be presented elsewhere (N.R. and S.N.F., in preparation).
Together with a rough estimate of 100 ms response delay from Fig. 2C, the total time it takes for a fly to correct for a perturbation of flight speed is in the order of 400 ms, which is surprisingly long compared with the much faster responses of Fannia. This discrepancy is likely to reflect varying response dynamics of reflexes adapted to different behavioral tasks, as well as species- or scale-dependent differences in flight mechanics.
Control of corrective speed maneuvers
A proportional control law governs flight speed control
The zero acceleration iso-line in Fig. 4A identifies pattern properties that do not cause corrective speed responses and therefore correspond to the stable-state condition of the speed controller. We found that the zero-response iso-line corresponded to a particular pattern velocity (V=TF/SF), consistent with the previous finding that many insects maintain a `preferred' retinal slip speed at steady state under normal closed-loop conditions (see Introduction; Fig. 1D).
The ability to test flies in open-loop allowed us to furthermore explore how flies responded to visual perturbations of this steady-state flight speed,which under natural flight conditions would likely arise from wind gusts or air turbulence. We found that the speed responses depended linearly on the difference between the preferred pattern speed Vpref and the actual retinal pattern slip speed Vslip over a broad range of spatio–temporal frequencies.
Based on our open-loop response characterization, the fly's visual responses can be functionally interpreted to result from a feedback controller, whose process variable is Vslip and whose control output is flight speed (Fig. 5). By experimentally de-coupling the retinal slip from the fly's flight speed (symbolized by the open switch in the feedback loop in Fig. 5), we were able to measure the open-loop transfer function over a broad range of spatio–temporal frequencies, represented by the data shown in Fig. 3 and Fig. 4A. Under closed-loop conditions (closed feedback loop in Fig. 5), the fly would quickly reach a flight velocity at which the optic flow is perceived at the preferred slip speed.
Whereas free-flight experiments performed under steady-state closed-loop conditions can reveal what is being controlled, the measured open-loop transfer properties provide functional insight into how this control is achieved. A quantitative model and simulations of open- and closed-loop conditions will be presented elsewhere (N.R. and S.N.F., in preparation). Recently, a PID (proportional-integral-derivative) control scheme was implied to underlie visual control of flight altitude in honeybees(Franceschini et al., 2007)(see also Taylor et al.,2008). Commonly used in engineering applications, PID controllers also use a control signal's derivative and integral to control the output. We found no evidence that this is the case for speed control, which depends directly on the retinal slip speed, thus representing a simpler proportional controller.
Simple control rules are based on sophisticated sensorimotor control pathways
Our control scheme is represented without reference to the physiology and biophysics of flight, which in a control model are typically represented as the plant dynamics. The physiological mechanisms underlying visuomotor flight speed control are presumably highly complex and remain little understood. First, the extraction of pattern velocity over a broad range of TFand SF requires sophisticated neural processing that is not easily explained with our current understanding of the fly's visual system. Second,the control of flight speed depends on subtle motor control and additional internal control loops. To modulate the flight speed, a fly needs to adjust its pitch angle (David, 1978)from exceedingly subtle changes in wing kinematics(Fry et al., 2003; Fry et al., 2005). Pitch control itself depends on proprioceptive mechanosensory feedback from the halteres (Pringle, 1948; Nalbach, 1993; Nalbach and Hengstenberg,1994). An additional control loop to compensate for varying air speeds may also exist, as acceleration responses below locomotor limits are invariant to air speed (data not shown) [see also fig. 3 in David(David, 1982)].
Due to the immense complexity of the underlying control mechanisms, any attempt to isolate specific components of the controller would seem highly speculative and misleading. We have therefore restricted our analysis to the high-level control strategy without assumptions or inferences about the underlying sensorimotor control mechanisms.
Despite the high complexity of the involved sensorimotor mechanisms, visual control of flight speed nevertheless obeys a surprisingly simple control rule. An embedding of complex non-linear mechanisms into a comparatively simple high-level control strategy may not be uncommon in biological systems(Gewecke and Heinzel, 1980; Sherman and Dickinson, 2004)(see also Taylor et al.,2008). Whereas the adaptive advantage of such simple control strategies remains unknown, they evidently benefit insects, whose evolutionary success is largely due to their superior flight capabilities(Dudley, 2000).
Visual processing of optic flow
There is current debate over the degree to which rotational and translational flight control is mediated by a common neural substrate (see Introduction). Our open-loop speed response measurements provide a baseline against which current and future models of motion-processing pathways can be evaluated. Our results show that the motion-processing pathway pertaining to speed control computes the linear velocity of planar patterns of arbitrary spatial frequency content. Importantly, we have shown that a behavioral characterization of the speed response can be meaningfully performed using sine grating stimuli, which allows a response characterization in the two-dimensional TF–SF parameter space. It would therefore be informative to compare our results with electrophysiological data and modeling results that are obtained with planar sine grating stimuli that are systematically varied in TF–SF parameter space. Such comparisons may be possible due to recent advances in electrophysiological techniques, which have allowed recording from LPTC in Drosophila (Joesch et al.,2008).
Furthermore, the measurement of the transient response properties to a step change in sensory input will allow a more detailed evaluation of the underlying motion computations. It is essential to assess whether transient or steady-state response properties of the motion-processing pathways dominate at the behaviorally relevant time scales(Borst and Bahde, 1986). Recent electrophysiological studies in flies have explored transient response properties of motion pathways in playback experiments that used realistic optic flow stimuli reconstructed from free-flight behavior (e.g. Kern et al., 2005; Karmeier et al., 2006). The finding that the steady-state physiology of a visual interneuron does not predict its response to more dynamic stimuli emphasizes the importance of re-examining behavioral responses under transient conditions, as performed in our present study.
Outlook
Future studies combining advanced genetic techniques with detailed behavioral analyses promise advances in our understanding of the neural substrate for motion processing in different behavioral contexts. For example,identified motion processing neurons can be genetically targeted(Joesch et al., 2008), which will allow them to be manipulated to explore their involvement and possible role from the behavioral effects (Duffy,2002). In a recent study, a forward genetic approach was applied in walking Drosophila and selective effects of large field optic flow were identified on walking speed and turning responses. Based on these results, it has been proposed that visual pathways subserving the control of translation and rotation may separate at the earliest stage of visual processing (Katsov and Clandinin,2008). It will be highly informative to combine advanced genetic techniques with detailed flight control analyses in a meaningful functional context to explore the physiological basis of speed control. In particular,genetic modification of the known visual pathways may provide evidence for the involvement and role of the known pathways in the context of visual control of flight speed.
Finally, our results are of direct relevance for biomimetic robotic implementations, such as autonomous flying micro-robots. We showed that visual speed control is based on a remarkably simple control strategy, however,requires a sophisticated vision sensor to robustly signal the linear pattern speed. Our work suggests that the fly's speed control strategy could be meaningfully transferred into the context of autonomous micro air vehicles,while substituting the complex biological control mechanisms with functionally equivalent engineering solutions.
LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS
- Acc
acceleration
- EMD
elementary motion detector
- GOL
open-loop gain
- LPTC
lobula plate tangential cells
- r
radius in TF–SF parameter space
- SF
linear spatial frequency
- sf
angular spatial frequency
- sfopt
optimal angular spatial frequency for correlator
- T
reaction time
- TF, tf
temporal frequency
- tfopt
optimal temporal frequency for correlator
- V
linear pattern velocity
- v
angular pattern velocity
- Vpref
preferred pattern velocity
- Vslip
retinal pattern slip speed
- Vset
set point pattern velocity
- X,
pX fly's upwind position in the wind tunnel
- α
angular coordinate in TF-SF parameter space
- λ
angular pattern wavelength
- Λ
linear pattern wavelength
- ΔΦ
inter-ommatidial angle
- τ
time delay of tracking system
FOOTNOTES
We wish to thank Martin Bichsel for technical support with Trackit 3D,David O'Carroll for providing a naturalistic image used in previous studies,Jérôme Frei, Marie-Christine Fluet, Nils Perret and Martin Ehrensperger for preliminary experiments using TrackFly. We thank Richard Hahnloser, Martin Zápotocky and Petr Marsalek for valuable comments and suggestions, as well as two anonymous reviewers for their useful comments. Financial support was provided by the Human Frontiers Science Program, the University of Zürich, the Swiss Federal Institute of Technology (TH-11/05-3), the Volkswagen Foundation, the National Science Foundation (FIBR 0623527)) and the Air Force Office of Scientific Research (FA9550-06-1-0079 to M.H.D.).