ABSTRACT
The subalar–tegminal resonance/auditory feedback hypothesis attempts to explain how crickets control the carrier frequency (fC), the loudness and the spectral purity of their calls. This model contrasts with the ‘clockwork cricket’ or escapement model by proposing that fC is not controlled by the resonance of the cricket’s radiators (the harps) but is instead controlled neurally. It suggests that crickets are capable of driving their harps to vibrate at any frequency and that they use a tunable Helmholtz-like resonator consisting of the tegmina and the air within the subalar space to amplify and filter the fC. This model predicts that fC is variable, that call loudness is related to tegminal position (and subalar volume) and that low-density gases should cause fC to increase.
In Anurogryllus arboreus, fC is not constant and varied by as much as 0.8 % between pulses. Within each sound pulse, the average fC typically decreased from the first to the last third of a sound pulse by 9 %. When crickets called in a mixture of heliox and air, fC increased 1.07- to 1.14-fold above the value in air. However, if the subalar space were part of a Helmholtz-like resonator, then its resonant frequency should have increased by 40–50 %. Moreover, similar increases occurred in species that lack a subalar space (oecanthines). Experimental reduction of the subalar volume of singing crickets resulted neither in a change in fC nor in a change in loudness. Nor did crickets attempt to restore the subalar volume to its original value. These results disprove the presence of a subalar–tegminal resonator.
The free resonance of freshly excised Gryllus rubens tegmina shifted by 1.09-fold when moved between air and a mixture of helium and air. Auditory feedback cannot be the cause of this shift, which is similar to the fC shifts in intact individuals of other species.
Calculations show that the harp is 3.9–1.8 times more massive than the air that moves en masse with the vibrating harps. Replacing air with heliox-air lowers the mass of the vibrating system sufficiently to account for the fC shifts. These results re-affirm the ‘clockwork cricket’ (escapement) hypothesis. However, as realized by others, the harps should be viewed as narrow-band variable-frequency oscillators whose tuning may be controlled by factors that vary the effective mass.
Introduction
Two competing hypotheses have been advanced to explain the control of the carrier frequency (fC) or pitch of a cricket’s call. The escapement or ‘clockwork cricket’ model (Elliott and Koch, 1985) proposes that the fC is determined principally by the mechanical resonance of the wing cells that radiate sound. More recently, Stephen and Hartley (1995b) proposed an alternative that we here term the subalar–tegminal resonance/auditory feedback model. It claims that fC is controlled neurally, not mechanically. We here refute this model.
To produce advertisement calls, male grylloid crickets spread and elevate their tegmina (Fig. 1). This behavior creates a semi-enclosed subalar airspace between the tegmina and body (Fig. 1) which Stephen and Hartley (1995b) claimed has important acoustic properties (see below). A pulse of sound is produced when a cricket closes its tegmina through a small arc (typically several millimeters) while engaging stridulatory structures. These structures, known as the files and plectra, are found on both tegmina. The files are Cu2 veins that differ from typical wing veins in that their lower surfaces are supplied with knobs or teeth. The plectra are heavily sclerotized areas on the anterior medial margin of each tegmen; they are attached to the anterior medial end of each file (see Bennet-Clark, 1989). During typical stridulation, the plectrum of the bottom wing is alternately captured and released by the file teeth on the lower side of the top wing at a rate referred to as the plectrum-to-file impact rate (fPFI). With each capture, the Cu2 vein of each wing is set into vibration – directly in the case of the top wing and via the plectrum for the lower wing. Since each wing’s Cu2 vein forms one border of a triangular wing cell known as the harp, vibrations of Cu2 excite the harps to vibrate and radiate sound at the fPFI. Thus, the fPFI equals the fC of a cricket’s call. Pulses of sound alternate with silent periods when the insect reopens its tegmina (see Fig. 6) at what is referred to as the pulse rate or wing stroke rate (fWS) (Walker, 1962).
Nocke’s (1971) work was utilized by Elliott and Koch (1985), Koch et al. (1988) and Bennet-Clark (1989) to explain how field crickets (Gryllus species and their close relatives) manage to maintain a relatively constant fC regardless of temperature changes. Although the fC of field crickets changes only slightly with temperature, the fWS shows strong temperature-dependence. From a knowledge of the stridulatory apparatus, one would expect that a change in fWS would alter the fPFI and fC by a similar proportion.
The ‘clockwork cricket’ (escapement) model explains this paradox by invoking parallels between escapement clocks and stridulating crickets. Escapements are devices that regulate energy input and motion. In mechanical clocks, they alternately lock and release the mechanism that moves the clock’s hands. The rhythm of lock and release is, in turn, controlled by a regulating oscillator such as a pendulum or balance spring. Escapements also provide a means to provide the regulator with discrete amounts of in-phase energy in order to maintain its oscillations (Landes, 1983). In the clockwork cricket analogy, the escapement is composed of the stridulatory structures with the harps acting as the regulating oscillator. (If one wishes to complete the analogy, radiated sound can be viewed as the clock’s hands.) Each time a tooth captures the plectrum, they lock together and motion briefly stops. Energy is transferred to the harps in phase with their motion, just as in an escapement clock. A moment later, when the direction of the harp’s vibration reverses, the plectrum is released and it skips to the next tooth. The results are that the resonance of the harp determines the fPFI and that the energy to produce sound is provided without disrupting the harp’s vibrations. Moreover, closing speed and fC remain roughly constant (being governed by the harp’s resonance) while fWS can change, primarily by altering the silent tegminal opening time.
Stephen and Hartley’s (1995b) rejection of the escapement model grew out of spectral measurements of the calls of Gryllus bimaculatus. Their data showed that crickets do not keep perfect time and that the harp’s oscillations had a number of imperfections that, if found in a watch, would eventually render it useless as a timepiece. For instance, they reported that the sound pulses had two spectral peaks. The most energetic (the fC) had a mean value of 4.86 kHz, but it varied by up to 3 % from pulse to pulse. They also reported a minor peak (12 dB SPL below the fC) whose value (5.5 kHz, the tegminal free resonant frequency) was constant from pulse to pulse regardless of experimental conditions. They argued that clocks cannot run at variable frequencies, let alone at two frequencies at once – especially when the minor frequency corresponds to the harp’s resonance. They concluded that the fC was not controlled via an escapement mechanism regulated by the harps.
After some additional experiments, Stephen and Hartley (1995b) constructed their alternative subalar–tegminal resonance/auditory feedback model of cricket sound production. It can be summarized as follows. (i) The fPFI (and therefore the fC) is controlled neurally and can have any value. They believed that information from the auditory system was used to control central neural mechanisms (‘auditory feedback’) that in turn determined the fPFI. They cited the dual spectral peaks mentioned above in partial justification of this supposition. (ii) They realized that, if the tegmina were driven at fPFI values greatly different from their resonance, the radiated sounds would be weak and would probably contain energy at a number of additional frequencies. To amplify and filter the tegminal signal, they proposed a tunable resonator consisting of the subalar space and tegmina. As with the fPFI, the tuning of this ‘subalar–tegminal resonator’ would be controlled by neural mechanisms. This resonator will be described in more detail below. To summarize their scheme briefly, they believed that a cricket listens to its song and then adjusts its fPFI and subalar–tegminal resonance to produce a loud, pure-frequency signal.
Let us consider the proposed subalar space–tegminal resonator in more detail. Bailey and Broughton (1970) speculated that the subalar space (Fig. 1) might act as part of a resonator in bush crickets (Tettigoniidae). Following their lead, Stephen and Hartley (1995b) proposed that a Helmholtz-like resonator operated in gryllids. This resonator would be able to amplify any fPFI, would filter out unwanted frequencies and would tend to entrain the fPFI to its resonant frequency. In many ways, what they proposed resembled the abdominal resonator found in many species of cicada (Young, 1990; Bennet-Clark and Young, 1992).
For these reasons, the present study set out to test experimentally the major features of Stephen and Hartley’s (1995b) model. First, we repeated their investigations of pulse-to-pulse variation in fC and of fC shifts in heliox-air mixtures. Next, we compared frequency shifts in the free resonance of excised tegmina with the fC shifts of intact individuals. We predicted that, if auditory feedback caused the fC shifts, then the shifts should not occur in isolated wings. To determine whether a subalar–tegminal resonator was present, we experimentally reduced the subalar volume of calling A. arboreus and looked for decreases in song amplitude, compensatory increases in fC and/or outward movement of the tegmina to restore the original subalar volume.
Materials and methods
Crickets
Most experiments used Anurogryllus arboreus T. Walker, a member of the same subfamily (Gryllinae) of crickets (Gryllidae) as Gryllus bimaculatus (Otte, 1994; Walker, 1998). Sound production in A. arboreus shares a number of features in common with members of the genus Gryllus. A. arboreus possesses a similar tegminal morphology, including a pre-costal fold (Fig. 1), and when stridulating it holds its tegmina in a similar position, thus producing a subalar space. We also examined two species that lack a subalar space: the tree crickets Oecanthus celerinictus T. Walker and O. quadripunctatus Beutenmuller. Although members of the Gryllidae (Otte, 1994), the tegmina of oecanthines (which belong to a different subfamily, Oecanthinae) are flatter and are held at a nearly vertical orientation during stridulation. Their stridulatory structures are similar to those of other gryllids, but their fC has a greater temperature dependence and may be controlled by a different mechanism than in the genera Gryllus and Anurogryllus (Walker, 1963; Sismondo, 1979; Bennet-Clark, 1989). For our measurements of the effect of air load on tegminal free resonance, we used tegmina isolated from G. rubens Scudder, a congener to G. bimaculatus.
The calling chamber and gas composition
We recorded calls in a chamber (Fig. 2) similar in design to that used by Stephen and Hartley (1995a,b). The base (0.22 m diameter) consisted of styrofoam covered with acoustical foam. Cut into the base was a port through which to introduce the cricket (filled with foam and sealed when the chamber was in use) and openings for an Electret microphone (Radio Shack 33-3013 or equivalent), a Tygon tube through which heliox or air could be pumped, and a thermocouple to measure temperature. A polyethylene bag was taped to the circumference of the base. When the chamber was filled, the height of the bag reached approximately 0.28 m.
During an experiment, we introduced the cricket (which was free to roam about the chamber) and then slowly pumped in air or heliox (20 % O2:80 % He), both saturated with water vapor. Final gas concentrations were calculated on the basis of the fraction of the gas that was CO2 using an Anarad (model AR 50) CO2 analyzer (500 p.p.m. full scale) and were brought to either 78 % He or <2 % He. Temperature was maintained at 25.2±0.3 °C with A. arboreus and 24.3±0.5 °C (means ± S.D.) for the two Oecanthus species. In no case were the slight deviations in temperature sufficient to affect stridulation sufficiently to explain the fC shifts we observed (Prestwich and Walker, 1981). Moreover, there was no consistent pattern to these small variations in temperature. Temperatures might be slightly higher or lower in heliox than in air in different individuals or in replicates of the same individual.
The apparatus leaked, and after 30 min the concentration of He was typically reduced to near 70 %. Thus, there was always some air in the ‘heliox’ chambers and we will use the term ‘heliox-air’ to emphasize that these were mixed atmospheres whose composition varied over time. Crickets usually spontaneously began calling continuously 10–15 min after gas had been pumped into the chamber; if sufficient data were not obtained within 30 min, the chamber was refilled with the appropriate gas. We randomized the order of exposure of individuals to different gases.
We were concerned that reflected sound might badly distort our recordings, especially with the heliox-air mixture because of the large difference in the index of refraction of the gas in the chamber compared with that in the room air. To check for this, we filled chambers with either pure helium (therefore producing an even larger difference in the indices) or with air and then played a 5 kHz tone through a 2.5 cm diameter cone speaker located in the middle of the chamber. Simultaneously, we recorded this sound using the chamber’s microphone. Fig. 3 shows a typical result, and statistical analysis showed that that there was no difference in the frequency measured in chambers filled with air or helium-air (P=0.83, N=4, paired t-test).
Sound pressure level
To determine whether the sound pressure level (SPL) changed in heliox-air versus air, we placed a Larson-Davis (model 812) SPL meter outside the calling chamber. Its microphone was located 0.5 m from the cricket along a line that originated near the center of the cricket rising towards the anterior at an angle of 45 °. It was not possible to place the microphone in the chamber because of near-field effects.
Measurements made outside the chamber will suffer some excess attenuation, and it was not possible to achieve precise positioning of the microphone relative to the cricket. Nevertheless, experiments using a speaker in the chamber showed that this arrangement could reliably detect changes of 1–2 dB SPL re 20 μPa.
Tegminal free resonance
To measure the free resonances of isolated wings in air and helium-air, we used apparatus and procedures similar to those of Nocke (1971) and Bennet-Clark (1987). We clipped the basal stalk of freshly isolated tegmina from male Gryllus rubens to a flexible wire, put it in an inverted position (the wing is curved), dusted it lightly with charred cork dust and positioned it over an exponential horn loudspeaker. The output of an audio signal generator (Leader Electronics, model LAG 120B) was amplified by a Realistic SA-155 audio amplifier that in turn drove the horn loudspeaker. The harp could be observed with a low-power microscope. We placed microphones for monitoring SPL and frequency near the wing, and then enclosed the entire apparatus except for the signal generator, amplifier and SPL meter in a plastic bag that was sealed with tape. The microscope objective entered the bag via a taped opening.
We inflated the bag with either compressed air or helium (which became ‘heli-air’ mixture since the bags leaked) until it was nearly turgid. We calculated the degree to which the helium was diluted using the method described previously for the recording chambers. At the time of the measurements, typical ‘heli-air’ mixtures were 75 % helium:25 % air. The experimental sequence was air, heli-air and then air to establish that there were no changes to the harps during the experiment. Experiments consisted of varying the frequency of applied sound while looking for harp resonance which could be identified by movement of the cork dust on the vibrating harp. We maintained constant SPL levels (within ± a few tenths of a decibel) regardless of driving frequency. We found the resonant frequency by recording (and analyzing as described below) a sample of the sound that produced the visually determined maximum displacement of the cork dust.
Collection and digitization of sounds
We collected sounds using either the calling chamber’s Electret microphone (Fig. 2) or, in the case of recordings in the anechoic chamber (see below), an Azden ECZ-660 zoom microphone. In all cases, the microphones were attached to a Marantz PMD 201 cassette recorder using type II high-bias tape. We digitized the sounds using 16 bit, 44.1 or 48 kHz A/D converters. Nearly all the energy in our recordings was at frequencies below 6 kHz. The signals lacked any significant energy near the Nyquist limit (22 or 24 kHz) so aliasing was not a problem.
Analysis of sounds
We analyzed sounds using Canary 1.2 sound-analysis software (Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology, Ithaca, New York, USA) and a Macintosh computer. Spectra had a frequency resolution of approximately 22 Hz.
Average carrier frequencies were determined on the basis of 1–2 s of constant calling (40–160 sound pulses, depending on the species and time). We also determined the fC of individual sound pulses by isolating and placing them in a sound window preceded and followed by 10 ms of silence. Inclusion of the silent periods avoided ‘end effects’ (frequency artifacts in the spectrum when the beginning or end of the signal coincides with the onset or cessation of sound). To check for intra-pulse frequency modulation, we isolated calls as described above and used the software’s spectrograph feature. Since we identified a decrease in frequency with time, we divided single calls into three equal parts, isolated each third as described above, determined its average frequency and then tested the results for statistically significant differences.
We verified our ability to detect frequency changes of the same magnitude as those we observed in our experiments by creating synthetic sounds with known spectral characteristics that resembled those measured from cricket songs. We played these through a speaker, and recorded, digitized and analyzed them using identical methods to those used for the experimental data. These calibrations showed that we could distinguish between frequencies that varied by as little as 22 Hz. This result was in agreement with the theoretical frequency discrimination limit for our software as we employed it. Moreover, the changes that we observed when crickets called in different atmospheres or the within-pulse modulation of cricket calls (when measured between different thirds or a call) was at least 10 times greater than our discrimination limit. Thus, we were confident in our ability to accurately discern frequency shifts in our experimental subjects.
Reduction of the subalar volume
For these experiments, A. arboreus were released in an anechoic room illuminated by low levels of red light. Shortly after a cricket began to sing, we started recording. As we continued recording, we reduced the subalar volume (Fig. 1) of the actively calling cricket by placing wooden dowels, dowels covered with acoustic foam or even fingers into the subalar space. In control experiments, the same objects were placed just outside and anterior or lateral to the subalar space. Unless the crickets were inadvertently touched during these manipulations, they usually remained in one position and continued calling. During all these experiments, the Azden recording microphone was situated at a constant position relative to the cricket in the far field at a distance between 0.25 and 0.5 m.
Estimation of the quality factor (Q) for the advertisement call of Anurogryllus arboreus
Statistical analyses
In all cases, the threshold for rejecting the null hypothesis was set at P,s;0.05. Whenever possible, we used a paired t-test to establish statistical significance, although in some cases we were forced to use a less powerful t-test which assumes unequal variances or a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). To discuss variation, we use the coefficient of variation (CV), defined as 100 times the standard deviation divided by the mean.
Results are presented as means ± S.D
Results
Frequency variations among whole sound pulses in Anurogryllus arboreus
For any individual, the coefficient of variation (CV) of the mean fC for whole sound pulses was less than 0.8 %, and in four of the six individuals used, the CV was less than 0.04 % (N=6 individuals, 10 pulses occurring within 2 s analyzed per individual). The mean fC was approximately 5.60 kHz with an average range encompassing approximately ±40 Hz (maximum of 100 Hz). Similar results were obtained in heliox-air with the exception that the mean frequency was shifted upwards (see below). Calls recorded from the same individuals several days apart at the same temperature had mean whole sound pulse carrier frequencies that were not significantly different for any individual (P=0.62, paired t-test), thus showing very little variation over time.
Within-pulse fC and amplitude changes
In A. arboreus calling in air, there was a highly significant decrease of approximately 9 % (480 Hz) in the mean fC from the first to last third of the call (P<0.0001, paired t-test, Table 1). Moreover, there were significant decreases in frequency from each third of the sound pulse to the next (P<0.0001, paired t-test, Table 1). The pattern was the same in heliox-air (Table 1). Although Stephen and Hartley (1995b) reported that within-pulse fC remained constant in G. bimaculatus, other published spectrograms for Gryllus species show the fC decreases during a pulse (Alexander, 1957; Leroy, 1966; Koch et al., 1988).
Stephen and Hartley (1995b) proposed that the slow decay in the amplitude of a sound pulse in crickets (see Fig. 6), which often begins well before the end of the pulse, is caused by an increasing mismatch between the excitation frequency and f0 of the proposed subalar–tegminal resonator. Thus, if the tooth strike rate (driving frequency) either remains constant (as they reported) or decreases (present study; Alexander, 1957; Leroy, 1966; Koch et al., 1988) then, as the tegmina close, f0 of the proposed subalar–tegminal resonator would increase since the subalar volume decreases (equations 4 and 5). Our examination of thousands of sound pulses from 14 individuals showed that, in most cases, the amplitude reached a maximum early in the call and then decayed slowly for an extended period followed by a rapid fade-out when the animal reversed its wings. However, it was not uncommon to see changes in this pattern within an individual, even over a short period. Most significantly, the calls of O. celerinictus and O. quadripunctatus showed the same patterns even though these species lack a subalar space.
Carrier frequency and SPL in air and heliox
Table 2 gives the mean fC and fWS for all three species in air and heliox-air at 25 °C. The values for air agree well with those previously published for the same species (Walker, 1963, 1973; Prestwich and Walker, 1981). Placing A. arboreus in heliox-air resulted in a statistically significant average 1.14-fold increase in fC with no statistically significant differences in fWS. When we determined mean SPL values in air and in heliox-air for each of five individuals (based on 2–4 separate measurements per individual per atmosphere), a paired t-test showed no statistically significant differences (P=0.24). The mean SPLs in both atmospheres for each individual were always within ±2 dB of each other; in two cases, the mean SPL in heliox-air was slightly higher than the mean SPL in air, while in the remaining three cases the SPL in heliox-air was slightly lower than in air. Sound pressure level measurements were difficult for technical reasons (see Materials and methods). Some uncertainty is therefore inherent in these measurements, but it is not so great as to undermine the conclusions drawn from these data.
Fig. 4 gives the frequency spectra for representative single sound pulses in air and heliox-air for an individual A. arboreus. The principal difference is an upward shift in frequency in heliox-air. The spectra from seven individuals showed minor frequency peaks in addition to the fC, but these were not consistent from individual to individual. Furthermore, spectra for sound pulses taken from the same individual at different times showed variation in the positions of these frequency peaks. We found no evidence of a consistent frequency peak near the fC that might be due to a separate resonance (cf. Stephen and Hartley, 1995b).
In the oecanthines, heliox-air induced an increase in fC similar to that in A. arboreus (Table 2). Surprisingly, the sound pulse rate also decreased, although this was only statistically significant in O. quadripunctatus. This was not due to temperature: in the singing chamber, the mean temperature (24.3±0.5 °C) never changed by more than 0.6 °C. Usually the change was less than 0.3 °C, and the changes that did occur were not in a consistent direction. A reduction in fWS should result in a reduction in fC (Walker, 1963). We are unable to explain the reduction in fWS, although we suspect that the paradoxical increase in fC is the result of changes in the air load acting on the tegmina (see Discussion) leading to excitation of different tegminal resonators (Sismondo, 1979; Bennet-Clark, 1989). Sound pressure level measurements for each individual in each atmosphere were within ±3 dB of each other. As with A. arboreus, there was no consistency as to the atmosphere in which the higher SPL occurred.
Alteration of the subalar volume in Anurogryllus arboreus
Table 3 presents the results of a direct test of the subalar–tegminal resonance model of fC control in A. arboreus. When variously sized objects were placed in the subalar spaces of calling individuals such that between 50 and over 90 % of the volume was filled, there were no statistically significant changes in fC. Upon reducing the subalar volume, fC might either decrease or increase but in no case was the change greater than 0.8 % of the control fC. Note that the experiments reported in Table 3 were carried out on generally smaller crickets than those used in other experiments (mean for this experiment 0.279±0.063 g; mean mass for chamber recording experiments 0.4434±0.025 g). Smaller A. arboreus have higher fC than larger individuals (K. N. Prestwich, unpublished data) and this accounts for most of the 18 % difference in control values of fC (compare Tables 2 and 3). In addition, a small part of the increase in fC can be accounted for by a higher mean temperature of 30.4±0.4 °C (Prestwich and Walker, 1981). The important point is that the experimental manipulations resulted in no significant changes in fC compared with control values. Moreover, in two additional experiments at 26 °C, reduction of the subalar volume was accompanied by a slight decrease in fC in one case and an increase in another (both changes were less than 1 %).
Subalar volume reduction also did not affect the SPL, as can be seen from the constant sound pulse amplitudes in Figs 5, 6. Furthermore, the crickets made no observable adjustments in tegminal position when the subalar volume was reduced (the experimenter was within 0.3 m of the subject and could clearly see the tegmina). The experimental volume reductions were so large that any postural compensations or SPL reduction should have been evident.
Tegminal free resonances in Gryllus rubens
At 23 °C, the mean free resonant frequency of harps isolated from G. rubens was 5.21±0.41 kHz (N=4) in air and 5.69±0.29 kHz (N=4) in a helium-air mixture containing approximately 75 % helium:25 % air (‘heli-air’). This difference was statistically significant (P<0.01, one-tailed paired t-test). The mean ratio of the resonant frequency in heli-air to air was 1.09±0.04 (N=4), similar to that found in intact crickets (Table 2). This indicated that auditory feedback did not cause the frequency shifts in singing animals. Free resonant frequencies in air were higher than the fC of natural G. rubens calls, which we determined to have a mean value of 4.79±0.23 kHz (N=4) at 23 °C.
Measurement of Q in Anurogryllus arboreus
The mean Q of A. arboreus calls in air was 18±5 (N=3), based on measurements of 10 pulses for each individual; the CV for each individual was approximately 25 %. This value of Q is similar to that reported by Bennet-Clark (1971), but lower than that reported by Nocke (1971) for isolated Gryllus campestris tegmina (Q≈28).
Discussion
Stephen and Hartley (1995b) regarded the 5–10 % change in fC that occurred when crickets called in different atmospheres as central evidence for their model of fC control. Their model predicts that, for crickets with a subalar space, such as A. arboreus, calling in heliox-air would cause an increase in the fPFI and therefore an increase in fC. Presumably, the cricket would attempt to call initially at its normal fC and would then adjust its call using auditory feedback. Their model also predicts that the subalar volume would need to decrease so as to retune the subalar–tegminal resonator to match and amplify the new fPFI. Failing this adjustment, the amplitude of the new higher-frequency call would decrease.
We found fC shifts in crickets calling in heliox-air (7–14 % increases; Table 2) similar to those reported by Stephen and Hartley (1995b). However, these occurred regardless of whether the species possessed a subalar space and were accompanied neither by decreases in SPL nor by observable alterations in tegminal position. These results cast doubt on a causal role for the subalar space. But could the fC shifts have been due to auditory distortion as Stephen and Hartley (1995b) suggest? We did not observe a period of frequency adjustment when a cricket first began to call in a new atmosphere. The fC of the initial calls in the new atmosphere was already shifted to the same extent as subsequent calls. However, we did record shifts in the free resonance frequency of isolated tegmina from G. rubens that were similar to the fC shifts of intact crickets, even though auditory feedback was impossible. The auditory feedback element of the model is thus disproved.
Our results also show conclusively that the subalar space and tegmina do not form a Helmholtz-like resonator. Direct experimental manipulation of the subalar volume did not affect the call amplitude nor did it result in changes in the fC or in the position of the tegmina. Moreover, there is no reason to believe, a priori, that the subalar space could function as the compliant element of a Helmholtz-like resonator. Fig. 1 shows that the rear of the subalar volume, its largest aperture, is fully open to the environment; the lateral regions are also open. These gaping openings present virtually no impedance to bulk airflow. Any tendency to compress air between the wings and the body is negated by flow out through the sides and the rear aperture. Instead of resembling a Helmholtz resonator, the tegmina and subalar space are analogous to a flask with its bottom broken off. Such flasks do not resonate. We conclude that a subalar–tegminal resonator also does not exist in crickets that stridulate and produce songs similar to those of Anurogryllus arboreus and Gryllus bimaculatus.
What then caused the fC shifts in heliox-air? Shifts in the free resonance frequency of excised tegmina were very similar to the fC shifts observed in living crickets, suggesting that the structures affected by the change in gas composition were the tegminal harps. According to equation 1, f0 will increase if either the system’s compliance C and/or its inertance L decrease. It is possible that compliance would decrease if the tegmina dried when they were exposed to dry gas, such as heliox from a compressed air cylinder. However, humidification of both air and heliox-air had no effect on the fC or free resonance shifts, thereby ruling out compliance changes as the cause.
These air loads were larger than expected from other calculations (Bennet-Clark, 1989). So, at the urging of an anonymous referee, we compared the estimate with a calculation of the air mass (inertance) using published values for G. campestris. In G. campestris, the area of the harp is approximately 15 μm2 (Nocke, 1971); if we approximate its shape to a disk (for ease of calculation), it would have a radius of 2.2 mm2. In this case, the radiator is small compared with the sound’s wavelength, and so bulk movement of air occurs to a distance approximating the radiator’s radius (Fletcher, 1992). Thus, the volume of the air load on the two sides of the harp is approximately 66 μl, and the total air mass is approximately 78 μg at 20 °C. Nocke (1971) calculated the effective mass of the harp (the inertance due to the mass of the vibrating portion of the harp plus the air load) of G. campestris to be 163 μg. For comparison, he reported that the same harp weighed 190 μg. Thus, in G. campestris, the air mass makes a substantial contribution to the total system mass, perhaps approaching as much as half of the effective mass (78/163=0.48 μg). This agrees well with our calculated values from the present study, suggesting that the explanation for the fC shifts does indeed lie in changes in the gas load portion of the total inertance of the vibrating system.
The ‘clockwork cricket’ hypothesis
When proposing the clockwork cricket model, Koch et al. (1988) clearly realized that crickets do not operate as perfect clocks. For example, their data show that the fC tended to decrease during a sound pulse, suggesting that the regulator of the fC was a variable-frequency oscillator. Nevertheless, harps have often been discussed as if they were fixed-frequency devices (probably for the sake of simplicity). This is not a problem unless one reifies this clockwork analogy into an actual clock. This was perhaps the fundamental error made by Stephen and Hartley (1995b).
For instance, they pointed to multiple spectral peaks and stated that no clock could run at two frequencies simultaneously. We also observed minor peaks (Fig. 4) although, unlike those reported by Stephen and Hartley (1995b), these peaks were not in constant locations relative to the principal peak. Accessory peaks are probably due to additional tegminal resonances (Bennet-Clark, 1989) whose values could change with subtle differences in wing position or stridulatory movement. Or, they could be caused by reflections within the recording chamber. Regardless, in both studies, the accessory spectral peaks were of very low amplitude compared with the fC peak. Any slight effect they may have had on the timing of the movement of the stridulatory structures would only be noticeable over long periods. Crickets do not need to maintain their fC accurately for long periods: the duration of a sound pulse is generally less than 50 cycles (10 ms).
Stephen and Hartley (1995b) reported up to ±3 % pulse-to-pulse variation in the fC of G. bimaculatus; we found up to ±1 % in A. arboreus. In both cases, the variation is far greater than would be found in a useful clock (Stephen and Hartley, 1995b). However, these findings do not disprove the existence of an escapement type mechanism in crickets. Instead, they point to differences in the operation of the controlling oscillators of clocks in comparison with those of crickets. These differences relate to the relative values of Q and to the fact that the cricket’s oscillator is a variable-frequency device.
Q is a measure of the range of frequencies over which a system can be driven successfully. Systems with higher values of Q have greater amplitudes at resonance, but this response drops off rapidly as the difference between f0 and the driving frequency increases (Fletcher, 1992). If accurate timekeeping is the objective, the oscillator needs a high Q, which makes it difficult to operate at any frequency but f0. Oscillators of mechanical clocks typically have Q values in the range of several hundred; quartz oscillators have far greater Q values and are far more accurate (Landes, 1983; Fletcher, 1992). In contrast, cricket harps must both control the fC and act as an effective radiator. To be a good radiator, the harp must be damped by high radiation resistance (Bennet-Clark, 1975, 1989). This reduces Q since it is proportional to the ratio of system mass to the radiation (damping) resistance (equation 7). The low Q (Q=18 in A. arboreus) makes it possible to drive the harp over a wider range of frequencies than if it were less damped.
A low Q permits frequency variation, but what mechanism is responsible for producing such variation? Not only does fC variation occur between pulses but also, in most species, there is pronounced within-pulse frequency modulation. We found a 9 % intra-pulse decrease in fC in A. arboreus (Table 1) similar to other values for gryllids (Alexander, 1957; Leroy, 1966; Koch et al., 1988). Recently, Simmons and Ritchie (1996) showed that the degree of intra-pulse frequency modulation in G. campestris depends on the relative dimensions and therefore the masses of the left and right harps. In experiments in which they ablated one or the other harp, their results suggest that the resonance of the left (usually smaller) harp is largely responsible for setting the fC during the initial third of the pulse. The right harp, which is usually larger, with a lower f0, then becomes dominant for the remainder of the pulse. These surprising results have recently been replicated in A. arboreus (K. N. Prestwich, unpublished data).
Bennet-Clark (1999) believes that these data show that the resonating system’s mass increases during the tegminal closure. He argues that the fC is typically lower than the tegminal free resonance (see, for example, our data for G. rubens) and concludes that, when the two harps are coupled together via the stridulatory apparatus, mass is added to the system. He cautions that little is known of the exact mechanism, but speculates that the cause is a shift in effective mass caused by changes in the point from which the right harp is excited during a wing stroke. Although the left (lower) harp is driven throughout the tegminal closure from its medial apex via the plectrum, the driving point for the right (more massive) harp moves laterally from its medial apex as the contact point with the plectrum skips along the file. If the vibrating mass changes as a function of the relative sizes of the left and right harps and the position of the plectrum on the file, then it is easy to imagine that pulse-to-pulse differences in fC could be due to differences in the point where the plectrum first contacts the file.
So, it is clear that crickets are not as accurate as fine Swiss watches. However, both watches and crickets are controlled by escapement mechanisms. The workings of a watch are optimized to keep time accurately for human uses. Crickets use an analogous escapement/regulator mechanism, but optimize its operation to make brief loud signals at a frequency as near as possible to the peak auditory sensitivity of conspecific females. Calling males seek to attract mates and make time, not keep time. We might add that watches are also used to attract mates, but the mechanism in this case is different.
List of symbols
- BW−3dB,SPL
bandwidth at 3 dB SPL re 20 μPa below the peak
- c
speed of sound
- C
compliance
- f0,air
resonant frequency in air
- fC
carrier frequency
- f0
resonant frequency
- fPFI
plectrum-to-file impact rate
- f0,heliox-air
resonant frequency in heliox-air
- fWS
wing stroke or sound pulse rate inertance
- Lair
inertance of air moving en masse together with the vibrating harp
- Lgas
inertance of any gas moving en masse together with the vibrating harp
- Lharp
inertance of the harp
- Lheliox-air
inertance of heliox-air moving en masse together with the vibrating harp
- l′
effective length
- loge(decrement)
the natural logarithm of the rate of decrease of the amplitude of an undriven oscillation mass
- P0
pressure in the absence of sound
- Q
quality factor
- R
damping (specific acoustic) resistance
- S
cross-sectional area
- SPL
sound pressure level, re 20 μPa
- V
volume
- γ
dimensionless ratio of specific heats
- λ
wavelength
- ρ
density
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We wish to thank Dr Thomas J. Walker and the Department of Entomology and Nematology at the University of Florida for generously providing K.N.P. with research space and for numerous useful and stimulating discussions. Professor Henry Bennet-Clark of Oxford University provided us with a number of excellent ideas relating to certain of our experiments and also posed stimulating questions and invaluable insights during the preparation of this manuscript, and we are very grateful to him. Similar thanks goes to an anonymous reviewer of this manuscript, especially for her/his suggestions dealing with the role of radiation mass and for stylistic comments. Special gratitude goes to Dr Catherine Langtimm of the Biological Resources Division of the US Geological Survey and Holy Cross College for her help with certain experiments and analysis and for her suggestions relating to the manuscript. We offer additional thanks for helpful discussions, advice and materials to our Holy Cross colleagues Dr George Hoffmann of the Biology Department and Drs Janine Shertzer, De-Ping Yang and Robert Garvey of the Physics Department. We also wish to thank Dr Alice Deckert of the Chemistry Department at Allegheny College for a number of useful discussions. Finally, thanks to Professor R. D. Alexander of the University of Michigan for kindly giving us permission to use his line drawing of a stridulating Gryllus and to members of the Editorial Office of JEB for their assistance and many helpful comments. This study was partially funded by a grant from the Holy Cross College Research and Publications fund.