ABSTRACT
Using a moveable loudspeaker and an implanted microphone, we studied the sound pressure transformation of the external ears of 47 species of bats from 13 families. We compared pinna gain, directionality of hearing and interaural intensity differences (IID) in echolocating and non-echolocating bats, in species using different echolocation strategies and in species that depend upon prey-generated sounds to locate their targets. In the Pteropodidae, two echolocating species had slightly higher directionality than a non-echolocating species. The ears of phyllostomid and vespertilionid species showed moderate directionality. In the Mormoopidae, the ear directionality of Pteronotus parnellii clearly matched the dominant spectral component of its echolocation calls, unlike the situation in three other species. Species in the Emballonuridae, Molossidae, Rhinopomatidae and two vespertilionids that use narrow-band search-phase echolocation calls showed increasingly sharp tuning of the pinna to the main frequency of their signals. Similar tuning was most evident in Hipposideridae and Rhinolophidae, species specialized for flutter detection via Doppler-shifted echoes of high-duty-cycle narrow-band signals. The large pinnae of bats that use prey-generated sounds to find their targets supply high sound pressure gain at lower frequencies. Increasing domination of a narrow spectral band in echolocation is reflected in the passive acoustic properties of the external ears (sharper directionality). The importance of IIDs for lateralization and horizontal localization is discussed by comparing the behavioural directional performance of bats with their bioacoustical features.
INTRODUCTION
Next to wings, the external ears or pinnae are among the most conspicuous features of many species of bats, the members of the mammalian order Chiroptera. There are at least three reasons why the Chiroptera offer excellent possibilities for examining the role of the external ear in acoustic behaviour. First, there are echolocating and non-echolocating bats. Although it is tempting to associate the echolocation abilities of bats with the features of their ears, echolocation is well-developed and widespread in the Microchiroptera, but not in the Megachiroptera. Second, echolocation is a trait of the Microchiroptera but not all species use the same approach to echolocation and some do not depend upon echolocation to find their prey (Neuweiler and Fenton, 1988). Third, among the Microchiroptera some species have large and conspicuous pinnae (e.g. Megadermatidae, Nycteridae, some Phyllostomidae and some Vespertilionidae) while others have smaller, less conspicuous ones (e.g. many Emballonuridae, Vespertilionidae and Molossidae).
In the Microchiroptera, we know of four basic approaches to echolocation (Neuweiler and Fenton, 1988). The first three involve the use of echolocation for detecting and tracking targets: (1) species producing relatively short, broad-band echolocation calls at low duty cycle (<20%) when searching for prey; (2) species with low duty-cycles (<20%) using longer calls of narrower bandwidth; and (3) the flutter detectors or constant-frequency (CF) bats producing long, narrow-band signals at high duty-cycle (>80%). The fourth approach involves species that may echolocate but usually use preygenerated cues to find their targets. These four approaches occur in several families of Microchiroptera, suggesting that they are polyphyletic.
The physical effects of sound propagation in air (Lawrence and Simmons, 1982) make echolocation a rather short-range orientation system (Kick, 1982). Bat’s acoustic receivers, including their pinnae, are one central component in maximizing the distance over which they can hear. Call features are another, but increasing call strength also makes the echolocator more conspicuous to potential prey (Roeder, 1970), to conspecifics (Balcombe and Fenton, 1988) and to predators (Fenton, 1980). Large external ears also contribute to low auditory thresholds in some microchiropteran bats (e.g. <0dB SPL in Megaderma lyra, Schmidt et al. 1984, and Rhinolophus ferrumequinum, Long and Schnitzler, 1975). In echolocation, sound production and reception should be matched to the physical properties of the acoustic waves. Guppy and Coles (1988a) showed a correlation between the wavelength of sonar signals and pinna dimensions in some rhinolophid and hipposiderid bats, high-duty-cycle species specialized for high, narrow-band frequencies; they found a similar trend in the Molossidae, low-duty-cycle bats. Pinnae are central to sound reception in general, and in some species large pinnae are thought to amplify the faint, low-frequency sounds emanating from moving prey (Guppy and Coles, 1988a; e.g. Megaderma lyra, Fiedler, 1979; Antrozous pallidus, Bell, 1982; Macrotus californicus, Bell, 1985). But large pinnae are not always associated with the use of passive sound in hunting. Several plecotine vespertilionids have very large pinnae, notably species in the genera Plecotus, Idionycteris, Corynorhinus and Euderma. Some of these bats show flexible hunting and echolocation behaviour (e.g. Idionycteris phyllotis, Simmons and O’Farrell, 1977; Plecotus auritus, Anderson and Racey, 1991), while others, such as Euderma maculatum, search for, detect and pursue airborne targets in a more conventional insectivorous bat fashion (Woodsworth et al. 1981; Fenton, 1990). Euderma maculatum, however, is distinctive for its use of sonar signals of low frequency (6–12kHz; Leonard and Fenton, 1984).
The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of pinnae on sound reception in bats. We made a threefold comparison: (1) echolocating with non-echolocating species; (2) echolocating species using different call designs; and (3) echolocating species that also rely on prey-generated sounds to locate their food. This comparison permitted us to test the hypothesis that the ear morphology and the passive properties of bat pinnae mirror the acoustic behaviour of bat species. We expected to find a close correspondence between the degree of frequency specialization of sonar signals and the acoustic properties of the external ears. Furthermore, since interaural pressure difference becomes a cue for azimuthal sound localization at higher frequencies (Gourevitch, 1987), we also tested the hypothesis that the performance of bats in pure-tone lateralization experiments (Witzke, 1989) is partly a function of the passive acoustic properties of their pinnae, particularly of the interaural intensity differences.
Over a range of frequencies, we calculated three main variables. First, signal amplification contributed by the pinnae as the pinna-induced pressure gain by comparing specimens with and without pinnae. Second, the hearing directionality associated with the pinnae by the −3 dB acceptance angle (Coles and Guppy, 1986). Third, the contributions of pinnae to lateralization of sounds, the binaural directionality or interaural intensity difference.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Data collection
We measured alcohol-preserved specimens (from the collection of the Department of Mammology, Royal Ontario Museum, Toronto) and a few frozen specimens (University of Munich) of 47 bat species (1–3 individuals per species) from 13 families (Table 1). In a few species, results obtained from specimens using both methods of preservation were compared. Pinna length (tip to base) and pinna width (largest width of pinna, Fig. 1C) were measured for the right ear to the nearest 0.5mm. Alcohol-preserved specimens were kept in deionized water for at least 1h before acoustic measurements to restore the water content and natural tension of the tissue. Measurements were performed in a small room (approximately 2 m3) lined with sound-absorbing material to decrease acoustic reflections. A calibration microphone (1/8inch Brüel and Kjaer, type 4138, protective grid removed) and a measuring amplifier (Brüel and Kjaer type 2610) were used to measure sound pressure levels. After opening the bulla from below, the microphone was fitted to a plastic cone (Schlegel, 1977a) whose tip was sealed with cyanoacrylate glue close to the position of the previously removed, right (also left in Cloeotis percivali and Lasiurus borealis) tympanic membrane (Jen and Chen, 1988, Coles et al. 1989). A sine-wave generator (Hewlett Packard model 3594A sweeping local oscillator), a power amplifier (Krohn-Hite) and a custom-built electrostatic speaker (2.5cm diameter) were used for sound production. The speaker had a flat frequency response (±3dB) between 15 and 125kHz, output was 15dB weaker at 5kHz and a gradual drop of 20dB occurred between 135 and 225kHz. Standing waves in the microphone–cone assembly created prominent fluctuations of the microphone sensitivity (±10dB) over the frequency range used, precluding comparisons of absolute sound pressure levels between frequencies. Mounted on a freely movable arm, the speaker could be remotely positioned on a virtual sphere of 15cm radius, centred at the position of the tympanic membrane (servo mechanism: Schlegel et al. 1988; Poussin and Schlegel, 1984). The frontal position of the bat (the assumed sonar direction; Fig. 1A) was aligned visually to 0° elevation and 0° azimuth. In Antrozous pallidus, Eptesicus fuscus, Megaderma lyra, Nycteris thebaica and Rhinolophus rouxi, flight photographs were used as a reference for the head orientation in the set-up. In species with nose leaves, these structures were set perpendicular to the horizontal plane, defined as the sonar horizon (see Results and Coles et al. 1989). A photograph was taken of every preparation for later comparison with collected data (e.g. position of acoustic axis, see below). The automated sampling scanned an area of −130° to +130° azimuth from the sagittal plane in steps of 2.00° (±0.°) and of −80° to +80° elevation from the sonar horizon in steps of 6.15° (±0.75°). The restriction of the scanning sphere to 260° azimuth times 160° elevation resulted from the mechanical arrangement. Continuous pure tones from 5 to 125kHz were used in 10kHz steps (5–120kHz in 5kHz steps in L. borealis) with sound pressure levels of 80–120dB SPL as test signals. An extended frequency range was tested with the 10kHz step width in three Hipposideridae (Asellia tridens: 5–155kHz, Cloeotis percivali: 5–225kHz, Hipposideros caffer: 5–205kHz). To prevent measurements of spurious harmonics or noise, the signal registered by the microphone and amplified by the measuring amplifier was bandpass-filtered with 3100Hz bandwidth, centred at the testing frequency (Hewlett Packard model 3590A wave analyzer). The sound pressure was measured as voltage re 20 μPa with a 12-bit A/D-converter board (Data Translation DT 2801A) installed in a microcomputer (Compaq Portable III). The same board also controlled the signal frequency (accurate to ±1%) and the servo mechanism, which executed the speaker movements. The automated measurement of 13 frequencies was accomplished in approximately 40min, the pinna of the measured ear was then removed and the same series of measurements was repeated. An adapted version of ASYST (ASYST Software Technologies) was used for data collection, analysis and display.
Data analysis
The sampled sound pressure values were stored in an array containing 26×130 data points. Further calculations were performed with this array. The position of the peak indicated the acoustic axis of the ear at a given frequency. By subtracting the intensity value measured at this position in the pinna-deprived preparation from the value sampled with the intact ear, we determined the pinna-induced pressure gain. As a measure of the directionality of the sound pressure transformation, the angular width (azimuth) and height (elevation) of a contour line 3dB below the peak were calculated; we have called this the −3dB acceptance angle (Coles and Guppy, 1986; see also Fig. 1).
We measured the characteristics of only one ear, assuming that both ears are symmetrical relative to the mid-sagittal plane. By adding the original data array to a copy of itself, mirrorred along the vertical central axis, we created a display of the total binaural directionality of hearing (Fig. 2A–D). Subtracting the mirrorred field from the original one creates a representation of the interaural intensity difference (IID; Fig. 2E–H), in some publications referred to as interaural level difference (ILD). The peak value in this derived array indicates the highest interaural intensity difference (HIID) for a given frequency. A horizontal cross section of this IID array at the sonar horizon was extracted for consecutive frequencies (open arrowheads in Fig. 3A–E) and laminated to a new array, termed the IID frequency display, now coding IID at the sonar horizon ( z-axis) as a function of azimuth (x-axis) and frequency (y-axis) (Fig. 3F, axonometric plot; Fig. 3G, contour line plot). The azimuthal inclination of the first IID data field (in degrees dB−1) records the intensity change occurring between the two ears for a given change in lateral position at any given elevation. We therefore calculated, in the derived IID frequency display, the inclination at the sagittal plane (0° azimuth; Fig. 3H), which in turn codes the frequency-dependent IID change available to the bat for lateralization of a pure-tone sound source at the frontal position. The IID frequency displays and the −3dB acceptance angles were of highest value for the comparison of the data with the animal’s behaviour.
RESULTS
General features concerning all species
A graphic visualization (Fig. 1A) of the sound pressure arriving at the tympanic membrane helps us to understand the animal’s perception of its acoustic surroundings and the three features we assessed. Sound pressure distribution (directionality) is illustrated in Fig. 1 as an axonometric pseudo-three-dimensional plot and as a contour plot (top and bottom, respectively, in Fig. 1B–G). A comparison of pinna-present (Fig. 1B–D) and pinna-absent (Fig. 1E–G) presentations demonstrates the pinna-induced increase of directionality and shows the effect of ear size (compare Fig. 1B and Fig. 1D). The pinnae work like magnifying glasses so moving them means changing the position of the acoustic axis, the ‘focus’ of the magnifying glass. In Fig. 1, directionality is indicated by the angular diameter (in degrees) of a slice through the ‘acoustic mountain’ 3dB under the peak (-3 dB acceptance angle; Fig. 1B–G, contour plots). The steepness of the mountain shows the ‘sharpness’ of the pinna as an acoustic magnifying glass.
The contributions of the pinnae to binaural directionality are shown for Lasiurus borealis in Fig. 2. Here, adding the inputs of both ears creates the symmetrical sensitivity pattern of the two pinnae (Fig. 2A–D). By subtracting the input to the left ear from the input to the right ear, we illustrate binaural directionality expressed as the sound pressure difference (IID; Fig. 2E–H; see also the data analysis for the calculation of the left ear input). The IID is the only intensity cue available to a bat for differentiating left from right. Steeper displays at the frontal vertical centre line show larger sound pressure differences between the two ears when a sound source moves laterally. A steep IID at the frontal position means potentially finer resolution of lateralization. Ear characteristics and IIDs change with frequency (Fig. 2A–D and E–H measured at 10–80kHz). To compare the frequency-dependent effect on the frontal IID inclination, we plotted this inclination (measured at 0° elevation, 0° azimuth) as a function of frequency (e.g. Figs 3, 6, 9, 10 and 14). These graphs predict the frequency-dependent lateralization performance for sound sources directly in front of the bat (see Fig. 14).
None of the measured variables alone accounted for the acoustic behaviour of the bats. In some species, pinna gain reached maximum values in behaviourally relevant frequency regions, while in others this was true of directionality or of the binaural effects (IID). We combined information for pinna gain, −3 dB acceptance angles and IID to investigate the relationships between pinnae and acoustic orientation. We found some general trends in all species analyzed.
At higher frequencies, the position of the acoustic axis moved towards higher elevations and the sagittal midline (Fig. 4), eventually reaching a relatively stable plateau above approximately 60kHz. Species with small pinnae (e.g. Myotis nigricans, Cloeotis percivali) did not show a clearly defined peak at low frequencies (<15kHz). In some Hipposideridae and Rhinolophidae, the elevation of the acoustic axis reached a plateau at higher frequencies and in molossids the vertical position changed less drastically than in other families. In most species emitting downward frequency-modulated calls (FM), the axis crossed 0° elevation in the spectral range of the echolocation calls, defining a sonar horizon (Coles et al. 1989). When we found a clear deviation from this pattern, the elevation of the horizontal cross sections for the calculation of the IID frequency field was adjusted to achieve an IID frequency display derived from elevations of comparable acoustical importance (the sonar horizon).
Pinnae showed increasing directionality with increasing frequency (Fig. 2A–C), affecting the extent of interaural intensity differences (Fig. 2D–E). The −3 dB acceptance angle decreased linearly with increasing wavelength (decreasing exponentially with increasing frequency, Fig. 5). The elevational height of the −3 dB acceptance angle reached about 40° above 60kHz and reached a minimum of 20° above 100 kHz. The angle tended to be smaller in elevation mainly because the pinna dimensions are asymmetric. In general a −3dB acceptance angle width of ⩽30° was reached above 80kHz. The removal of the pinna always reduced directionality (increased −3dB acceptance angle); this was usually accompanied by a decrease of the total dynamic range recorded over the sampled area (compare Fig. 1B–D with Fig. 1E–G). The direction of the acoustic axis shifted unpredictably after removal of the pinna.
Effects of preservation and sample size
The alcohol-preserved and frozen specimens of the frugivorous Phyllostomus discolor reached maximum IID (25–30dB) in the lower range of their echolocation calls (40kHz). The frozen specimen, however, showed a more regular rise of the IID with frequency and azimuth than the alcohol-preserved specimen. As in Phyllostomus discolor, the −3 dB acceptance angles were the same in a frozen and an alcohol-preserved specimen of Noctilio albiventris (see Fig. 11A,B). The −3 dB acceptance angles were the same for three different specimens of Lasiurus borealis.
Echolocating and non-echolocating bats
There were no striking differences between the pinnae of echolocating Rousettus aegyptiacus and R. amplexicaudatus and the non-echolocating Pteropus giganteus. Pteropus giganteus had the largest ears and showed the largest pinna gain compared with the two echolocators (see Table 1). The pinnae of all three species showed moderate directionality and binaural directionality was similar.
The binaural frequency-dependent directionality display (IID frequency display), however, was irregular in R. amplexicaudatus, while in P. giganteus it showed a relatively steady increase of IID with increasing azimuth and increasing frequency (Fig. 6A). In R. aegyptiacus, IID increased with frequency but decreased for azimuths above 60° (Fig. 6B). In P. giganteus at 0° azimuth, the IID frequency display showed a relatively stable slope of 3.3degreesdB−1 over the whole frequency range. This inclination reached 1.9degreesdB−1 above 25kHz in R. aegyptiacus (Fig. 6C).
Low-duty-cycle broad-band echolocating bats
In most bats using FM or multiharmonic broad-band search-phase signals, the correlation between ear dimensions and main echolocation call frequency, the frequency containing most energy in the power spectrum of the call (Fig. 7A,B), was not significant. Phyllostomids, for example, showed no consistently prominent feature in their ear characteristics, whether they fed on blood (Desmodus rotundus), nectar, pollen and insects (Glossophaga soricina), fruit (Phyllostomus elongatus) or insects (Micronycteris megalotis). Binaural directionality at the sonar horizon in D. rotundus (Fig. 8A) showed no prominent peaks and shallow inclinations for frontal directions (5–10degreesdB−1). There were some variations on this theme since one species (the frugivorous Carollia brevicauda) had a high pinna gain (maximum 18dB) and high IID (⩽36dB) in the spectral range of best hearing (40–80kHz) reported for a related species (Carollia perspicillata; Howell, 1974). In the frontal direction in C. brevicauda, the binaural directionality showed a constant inclination of 1.9degreesdB−1 between 35 and 105kHz, unlike the situation in D. rotundus where local minima in both −3 dB acceptance angles at 25–35kHz coincided with the highest pinna gain (30dB). Relatively large pinnae meant that the frugivorous Artibeus lituratus had a maximum IID of 33dB at 25kHz. From this frequency to 45kHz, the inclination of the IID frequency display at the frontal position showed a minimum of 1.9degreesdB−1, then rose to 3.3degreesdB−1 (55–65kHz) and dropped again to below 1.9degreesdB−1 above 85kHz.
In many Vespertilionidae, binaural directionality displays were obviously related to the frequencies dominating the echolocation calls, but the correlation between ear dimensions and main echolocation call frequency was not significant (Fig. 7). For example, in Eptesicus fuscus which uses relatively short broad-band FM calls, sweeping from approximately 40–20kHz, to search for airborne prey, the display becomes steeper (up to 1.9degreesdB−1) and the HIID (35dB) was located in this spectral area. Two other species using shorter, broad-band calls, Myotis nigricans and Miniopterus natalensis, have small ears which are fairly directional in the lower range of their echolocation calls (60 and 40kHz respectively). In this spectral range, both species had HIIDs of 25–30dB and the IID frequency display showed a steepness of 1.9degreesdB−1. This inclination increased to 0.6degreesdB−1 at 105kHz in M. nigricans, while it degraded above 75kHz in M. natalensis (Fig. 8B).
Most mormoopid bats use broad-band search-phase echolocation calls. The spectacular-looking ears of Mormoops megalophylla provided a pinna gain of 20dB at 15kHz, and the pinna characteristics corresponded to our knowledge of this species’ biosonar calls. The HIID of 30dB was reached between 45 and 55kHz and, after a drop of 10dB, the HIID rose again to 25dB at 85kHz. Both peaks and the peak and the inclination in the IID frequency display matched the two dominant harmonics in the biosonar signals (see Fig. 1A). The −3 dB acceptance angles measured did not seem to correspond to the echolocation behaviour of two species of Pteronotus. In P. personatus, the HIID (28–32dB) was located between 45 and 75kHz, falling within the range of the FM portion of the dominant harmonic. In this region, the inclination of the IID frequency display at the frontal position remained relatively low (3.5degreesdB−1; see Fig. 12C). In P. gymnotus, however (Fig. 12B), these displays showed a marked increase in steepness (down to 0.6degreesdB−1 at 50kHz), but the absolute peaks were reached at higher frequencies.
If these generalizations apply to other bats, Natalus micropus (Natalidae) should use echolocation calls with two harmonics in the range 40–80kHz. We base this prediction on three features of the pinnae: (1) the pinna gain amounts to 15dB at 25kHz and 11dB at 85kHz; (2) the IID peaked at 45kHz (26dB) and 85kHz (32dB) and (3) binaural directionality data suggest maximum frontal inclination of 1.9degreesdB−1 between 45 and 75kHz.
Low-duty-cycle narrow-band echolocating bats
Species with search-phase echolocation calls dominated by narrow bandwidths (some species in the Rhinopomatidae, Emballonuridae, Mormoopidae, Noctilionidae and Vespertilionidae) showed a closer relationship between pinna dimensions and echolocation call characteristics than did broad-band species. Species with search-phase echolocation calls dominated by higher frequencies tended to have smaller ears, particularly emballonurids and molossids. A close relationship between pinna features and echolocation calls was also evident in vespertilionids that depend upon narrower bandwidth calls to search for airborne prey. For example, in Lasiurus borealis and Lasiurus cinereus, the highest slope of the IID frequency display and the HIID appeared at the first and/or second harmonic (fundamental=first harmonic) of their echolocation calls (Fig. 9). Furthermore, in L. borealis, −3 dB acceptance angle dropped at 40kHz to a level not otherwise reached until above 80kHz.
Sometimes we found a clear relationship between echolocation call features and pinna features, and at other times we did not. The Noctilionidae provide a clear example. In Noctilio albiventris, there were clear IID peaks at the spectral range of their echolocation calls (Fig. 11C) and the IID frequency displays were also comparable, reaching a steep inclination (<1.9degreesdB−1) at the forward position above 55kHz. In Noctilio leporinus, however, there was no evident relationship between ear characteristics and acoustic features of search-phase echolocation calls. The IID gradually increased from 20dB at 25kHz to 30dB at 85kHz and neither −3 dB acceptance angle nor pinna gain was conspicuously related to the echolocation calls.
Among the emballonurids, external ear dimensions showed a non-significant trend to correlate with the dominant spectral component of the echolocation calls (Fig. 7C,D). Taphozous hildegardae showed an HIID of above 30dB between 15 and 55kHz and the IID frequency display was steep (1.9degreesdB−1) between 25 and 65kHz, becoming locally shallow again between 75 and 105kHz. Both variables matched with the biosonar calls (35–25kHz). In Saccopteryx bilineata, the IID showed a maximum of 27dB at 45–55kHz and the IID frequency display was steepest (4.5–1.9degreesdB−1) between 45 and 95kHz, the main call component being around 45–50kHz. The pinnae of Rhynchonycteris naso showed a local increase in directionality (decrease in −3 dB acceptance angle) at the lower harmonic of its echolocation call and the highest pinna gain (11dB) was located at the frequency of the FM part of the same harmonic (25–35kHz). The IID frequency display was relatively unspecific, increasing in absolute value and steepness towards 95kHz. We found the peak in IID (27dB) at approximately 85kHz (FM of the higher harmonic). Finally, in Peropteryx macrotis the maximum IID (29dB) lay at 55kHz, above which the IID frequency display was steepest (1.9degreesdB−1).
In molossids, the obvious correlation of pinna dimensions (particularly height) with frequency of main call energy was not statistically significant (Fig. 7C,D). The large ears of Otomops martiensseni produced a pinna gain of 12dB at 5kHz and a local minimum in −3 dB acceptance angle at 15kHz which parallels the audible echolocation calls (17–10kHz). Similar drops were evident in the −3 dB acceptance angle of Tadarida pumila at 55kHz and 85–95kHz. The HIID reached above 25dB between 45 and 85kHz and the IID frequency display was steepest (⩽1.9degreesdB−1) at the frontal position between 45 and 65kHz and again at 95–115kHz. Thus, for this species we predict echolocation call frequencies between 35 and 55kHz with a possible second harmonic (Table 1). Tadarida brasiliensis and T. macrotis showed maxima of the IID (25dB and 27dB respectively) and of the frontal inclination of the IID frequency display (0.6degreesdB−1 and 1.9degreesdB−1 respectively) at the dominant component of their echolocation calls (45kHz and approximately 25kHz respectively). The IID frequency display looked comparable in both species, with T. brasiliensis (Fig. 8C) showing the peak slightly higher than T. macrotis.
As in T. pumila, we found no published information about the echolocation calls of Mormopterus jugularis and Molossus pretiosus . The ear data for these species suggest that several spectral regions are important in the echolocation of these bats. Mormopterus jugularis showed minima in the −3 dB acceptance angle (<60° in elevation, <40°in azimuth) between 55 and 95kHz. This also affected the IID, showing maxima (25–30dB) and highest lateral changes per angular displacement (⩽1.9degreesdB−1) in the same spectral region. We expect this species to echolocate with harmonic calls dominated by a 55kHz component. In M. pretiosus the pinna gain (12dB at 25kHz and 10dB at 55kHz) and the slope of the IID frequency display at the frontal position (1.9degreesdB−1 between 35 and 85kHz) suggest broad-band calls with possible harmonic components at 30 and 60kHz.
The rhinopomatid Rhinopoma hardwickei showed a decrease of the −3 dB acceptance angle at 35kHz, coinciding with the main (second harmonic) component of its echolocation calls and the frequency of best hearing. The acceptance angles increased at 45kHz. Furthermore, the IID reached a maximum of 30dB at this frequency, only exceeded by a second peak (32dB) at 85kHz. In general, the IID frequency display (peak and inclination) seemed to be matched to a spectral component at 70–80kHz, coinciding with the fourth harmonic of the call.
The CF or high-duty-cycle echolocating bats
Unlike the other species in the Mormoopidae, Pteronotus parnellii uses long, narrow-bandwidth, high-duty-cycle search-phase echolocation calls to detect fluttering targets. We found an obvious relationship between echolocation call characteristics and pinna structure. The IID frequency display showed a marked increase in steepness from 3.5degreesdB−1 to 1.9degreesdB−1 at approximately 60kHz and the absolute peaks in sensitivity corresponded to the spectral positions of harmonics of the echolocation calls (see Fig. 12E).
The relationship between pinna features and echolocation calls, however, was most evident in the rhinolophids and hipposiderids. Rhinolophids showed a distinct matching of dominant echolocation frequencies and ear characteristics. For example, in Rhinolophus rouxi, a pinna gain of 14dB coincided with the maximum IID (34dB) at 75kHz; pinna gain rose to 24dB at 85kHz. The central inclination of the IID frequency display (Fig. 8F) reached 1.9degreesdB−1 above 35kHz with a local increase to 0.6degreesdB−1 at 75–95kHz. In Rhinolophus clivosus, the IID reached peak values (approximately 30dB) between 75 and 105kHz, and above 75kHz the IID frequency display had an inclination of 0.6degreesdB−1 at 0° azimuth and closely resembled the plot for R. rouxi but with the peak shifted to 95kHz. Echolocation calls of R. clivosus have the dominant harmonic between 70kHz (R. c. deckeni) and 100kHz (R. c. clivosus; J. D. Pye, personal communication). In Rhinolophus eloquens, the peak IIDs occurred at 35kHz (35dB), 55–60kHz and 85kHz (28dB), with drops of 5–10dB between the peaks. The −3 dB acceptance angles showed the usual decrease to a low of 38° at 45–55kHz in elevation and ⩽30° at 45–65kHz in azimuth, but with a subsequent gradual increase to 62° (elevation) and 42° (azimuth) at 85kHz. This produced an IID frequency display comparable to that of R. rouxi but with the peak shifted to 45kHz so that the highest change of IID per degree (0.6degreesdB−1) extended from 45 to 65kHz. The biosonar signals of R. eloquens should have the dominant component between 45 and 55kHz.
Hipposiderids had pinna dimensions and echolocation call frequencies that were significantly correlated (Fig. 7C,D). The main component of the sonar signals of Cloeotis percivali lay at 212kHz (probably the fifth harmonic), but we found distinct features associated with the pinnae at 165kHz (possibly the fourth harmonic), where pinna gain reached its maximum of 20dB, and the HIID amounted to 28dB, a value only exceeded at 125kHz (30dB). The IID frequency display is clearly dominated by the high-frequency range around 170kHz, where the display also reaches its highest inclination (Fig. 3G,H). In Asellia tridens (Fig. 8D), the peak IID (35dB) and a maximum in inclination of the IID frequency display (1.9degreesdB−1) coincided with the spectral range of the echolocation calls (the absolute maximum was 0.6degreesdB−1 at 45–65kHz). In Hipposideros lankadiva, the elevational −3dB acceptance angle showed a low of 20° at 65kHz, followed by a gradual increase to 42° at 105kHz. Highest inclination of the IID frequency display (0.6degreesdB−1) was also located in this spectral range. In Hipposideros caffer, the −3 dB acceptance angle reached a minimum of 24° in elevation (at 145–165kHz) and 27–28° in azimuth (at 135–155kHz), followed by an increase in angle of more than 20°. Again, the highest inclination of the IID frequency display (1.9degreesdB−1) occurred in the range of the sonar signal (145kHz), and in this species additionally at 45–95kHz.
Gleaning bats
Our data included seven species that hunt by listening for prey-generated sounds: Cardioderma cor, Megaderma lyra (Megadermatidae), Nycteris grandis, Nycteris thebaica (Nycteridae), Antrozous pallidus, Myotis evotis (Vespertilionidae) and Trachops cirrhosus (Phyllostomidae). These are large-eared species and some achieve pinna gains of 15–22dB by 15kHz, or in C. cor 13dB at 5kHz (Fig. 13). Trachops cirrhosus showed 5–10dB pinna gain between 5 and 95kHz.
In general, these bats were distinctive because of the high directionality achieved at lower frequencies relative to the bats that depended upon echolocation to find their prey. Trachops cirrhosus showed an HIID of 27dB at 5kHz, the highest value we encountered in this study at the lowest tested frequency. In T. cirrhosus we found an inclination of the IID frequency display of 1.9degreesdB−1 at 0° azimuth between 5kHz and 85kHz. Comparable inclination values for most non-passive listening bats were less than 2 degreesdB−1 only above 45kHz and Myotis evotis was more similar to this group (Fig. 10). Cardioderma cor and N. grandis reached this value at 35kHz, M. lyra, N. thebaica and A. pallidus at 25kHz, suggesting good lateralization perfomance at low frequencies. The inclination showed prominent local drops at intermediate frequencies in N. grandis (1.9degreesdB−1 at 25–35kHz and above 85kHz; 19.3degreesdB−1 at 45kHz), N. thebaica (1.9degreesdB−1 at 35–75kHz; 10degreesdB−1 at 95kHz) and M. lyra (1.9degreesdB−1 at 25–35kHz, 10degreesdB−1 at 55kHz; Fig. 14).
DISCUSSION
Methodological considerations
The methods used to prepare our specimens meant that our measurements were unaffected by the method of preservation used. In Eptesicus fuscus, Noctilio albiventris and Phyllostomus discolor, we compared the two methods of preservation and found −3 dB acceptance angles and HIIDs (Fig. 11A–C) reproducible in different preparations.
Differences found by P. A. Schlegel (unpublished observations) between recently dead and preserved R. rouxi were comparable in size to differences found between repeated measurements taken on the same specimen. The general characteristics appear unchanged unless the pinna starts drying out or is drastically changed in shape. Since desiccation of the pinnae had been shown to increase amplification significantly at higher frequencies (>70kHz), we used short data collection times (40min) to ensure that the preparation did not dry out.
Our data compare well with data obtained from recently dead or live animals. Electrophysiological measurements on Rhinolophus ferrumequinum show a stable position of the acoustic axis at 30kHz, 60kHz and 80kHz within an individual, differing slightly between individuals (15–45° azimuth, 30–60° elevation; Neuweiler, 1970). Comparably, at these frequencies, we found only minor changes in axis position in R. rouxi (>25 kHz: ±4° elevation; ±8° azimuth), R. eloquens (>15 kHz: ±10° elevation; ±15° azimuth) and R. clivosus (>15 kHz: ±7° elevation; ±° azimuth). Electrophysiologically obtained directionality patterns measured at 83.3kHz in Rhinolophus ferrumequinum (Neuweiler, 1970) and at 20kHz, 65.2kHz and 80kHz in R. rouxi (P. A. Schlegel, unpublished results; R. B. Coles, A. Guppy and P. A. Schlegel, unpublished results) look very much like our results in the same or other rhinolophid and hipposiderid species (e.g. Hipposideros caffer at 75kHz, Fig. 1C). In rhinolophids, in the range of their sonar signals, we found −3dB acceptance angles (approximately 20° in azimuth and 30° in elevation) that agree with data from R. ferrumequinum (Neuweiler, 1970) and behavioural results obtained in the same species (Grinnell and Schnitzler, 1977). The HIID values we calculated for R. rouxi (34 dB) and R. eloquens (35 dB; see Table 1) again compare well with values measured on live R. ferrumequinum (approximately 30dB; Neuweiler, 1970) and R. rouxi (40dB; Schlegel, 1977b).
In two FM bats we found HIID values (20dB at 65kHz Myotis nigricans; 22dB at 45kHz M. evotis) that are confirmed by electrophysiologically measured values published by Shimozawa et al. (1974) for Myotis grisescens (approximately 25dB at 35kHz and 55kHz) and M. lucifugus (approximately 15dB at 75kHz and 95kHz).
The actual mounting of the specimen in the centre of the scanned sphere was not completely reproducible. This meant that the position of the acoustic axis showed more variation and this is the reason why we did not compare absolute positions of this axis between preparations. All three measured Lasiurus borealis differed slightly in the position of the acoustic axis, but showed comparable −3 dB acceptance angles and a marked narrowing of the angle in the 35–40kHz range, coinciding with the spectral position of the dominant harmonic in the echolocation calls.
In L. borealis and up to 125kHz in Cloeotis percivali we collected data from both ears and the comparable results justified the calculations of variables that depended upon sagittal symmetry of the sound pressure transformation (e.g. IIDs).
Contribution of pinna effects to sound localization
Most mammals, especially echolocating bats, depend on accurate localization of sound sources. We found that bat pinnae were increasingly directional at frequencies above 5 kHz (Figs 2, 5), and that the position of the acoustic axis shifted towards the sagittal midline and higher elevations, eventually reaching a plateau (Fig. 4). The frequency-dependent shift of the acoustic axis might help monaural localization of frequency-structured sounds, but localization performance improves considerably when completed binaurally (Gourevitch, 1987). Since we could only measure intensity, our estimates of lateralization performances do not consider simultaneous analysis of time and intensity differences.
Because of basic acoustical principles (Fletcher and Thwaites, 1979), we could not find a clear peak in the sound pressure transformation at lower frequencies in small-eared species. The characteristics of the large external ears of Plecotus auritus (Coles et al. 1989), Macroderma gigas and Nyctophilus gouldii (Guppy and Coles, 1988b) accurately match the behaviour of mathematical horns of similar dimensions. We therefore refrain from discussing the results of our study in physical terms and turn our attention to the biological relevance of the measurements.
The directionality of the echolocation system has been measured previously using different methods in a variety of bats (e.g. in Myotis lucifugus and Corynorhinus townsendii, Grinnell, 1963; Eptesicus fuscus, Carollia brevicauda and Pteronotus parnellii, Jen and Chen, 1988; Myotis grisescens, Shimozawa et al. 1974; Rhinolophus ferrumequinum, Grinnell and Schnitzler, 1977; Schnitzler and Grinnell, 1977). In all species, hearing becomes more directional with increasing frequencies. Myotis lucifugus reaches −3 dB acceptance angles of 50° above 40kHz, the larger ears of Corynorhinus townsendii achieve values of 30° above 50kHz (Grinnell, 1963), a value also found by Coles et al. (1989) in Plecotus auritus. Similar values were reported for the three species measured by Jen and Chen (1988). In M. griesecens and R. ferrumequinum, the directionality of sound emission and hearing combine to produce a steep increase of the total directionality of the sonar system in front of the bat (Shimozawa et al. 1974; Grinnell and Schnitzler, 1977). Highest interaural intensity differences (>30dB) occur at around 40kHz in E. fuscus, C. brevicauda, M. lucifugus and C. townsendii; values of 20–30dB are reached in P. parnellii and M. grisescens. Up to 40dB IID can be found in R. ferrumequinum (Schlegel, 1977b). IIDs change at a rate of up to 1.7degreesdB−1 in M. grisescens, and Grinnell (1963) reported single units in the inferior colliculus with slopes of 0.1degreesdB−1 in M. lucifugus. All these data compare favourably with our measurements, although the steepest slopes we encountered were ⩾0.6degreesdB−1.
If bats, like other mammals, have a minimal binaurally detectable intensity difference of 1dB (approximately 1dB at 1kHz in humans, Mills, 1960, and cats, Wakeford and Robinson, 1974), most species should detect differences of less than 5°in lateral position of pure-tone sources, at certain frequencies. In some species, an approximately 1° resolution is predicted from our data (e.g. Rhinolophus rouxi at 85–95kHz, Fig. 8F; cf. Schlegel, 1977b). A value of 1.5–0.25° has been suggested for M. lucifugus (Jen, 1980). In Megaderma lyra, the pure-tone lateralization performance has been measured behaviourally (Witzke, 1989), and the performance curve follows the course of the frontal inclination in the IID frequency display, reflecting the amount of intensity change for a given change in position and a given frequency. The division of the lateralization performance curve (in degrees) by the inclination curve (in degreesdB−1) results in a frequency-independent lateralization threshold which has a value of 1.4±0.6dB (Fig. 14). This value is remarkably close to the 1dB assumed above and, using earphones, one could verify experimentally that this is indeed the minimum binaural intensity difference discernible by M. lyra. These results strongly suggest that bats depend upon intensity cues for sound lateralization. This is further substantiated by the fact that other mammals need decreasing intensity but increasing temporal disparities for sound lateralization at higher frequencies (0.5–3kHz, measured in cats, Wakeford and Robertson, 1974). In M. lyra, the lateralization performance and the slope of the IID frequency display match over the whole frequency range measured. If other bats (such as Artibeus lituratus, Nycteris grandis, Nycteris thebaica and Taphozous hildegardae, see Table 1, VAR) use primarily intensity cues for pure-tone lateralization, we expect their performances to drop at decreasing slopes of the IID frequency display, as in M. lyra.
The three specimens of E. fuscus we measured reached highest inclinations of the IID frequency display of 1.9degreesdB−1 in the range of their echolocation calls. A binaural detection threshold of 1.4dB should allow this species to differentiate pure-tone sources separated by 2.6°. Peff and Simmons (1971) reported horizontal angle resolutions of 6–8° in E. fuscus and 4–6° in Phyllostomus hastatus. In later experiments, employing a different arrangement, however, E. fuscus could discriminate 1.5° in 75% of the experiments (Simmons et al. 1983). The authors discuss these results on the basis of time rather than intensity disparities. Our data suggest that the same performance could be achieved with the use of intensity cues.
Specialization of the mode of orientation relates to ear properties
The data presented here reveal a general trend in all investigated families. Bat species, emphasizing a spectral band in their echolocation calls, tend to show matching specializations of calls and pinnae. In the vespertilionid Miniopterus natalensis, for example, the lowest range of the echolocation calls, which contains most signal energy, coincides with the area of best hearing, the spectral range of highest interaural intensity difference and the highest IID change per degree. In this and other species (L. borealis, R. rouxi, H. lankadiva, etc.) there is obvious matching of pinnae and echolocation calls.
In the Megachiroptera, only two species studied use biosonar and produce short, broad-band signals in a way that differs from that in the Microchiroptera (Suthers, 1988). We found no evidence of pinna specialization in echolocating Megachiroptera. The large head and pinnae of Pteropus giganteus coincided with a relatively smooth rise in IID with azimuth (Fig. 6A). The data from Rousettus amplexicaudatus showed no clear pattern, but R. aegyptiacus showed directionality and IID patterns comparable to those of microchiropteran bats (Fig. 6B). The Megachiroptera appear to be ‘ear generalists’ not differing substantially from other mammals such as cats (Irvine, 1987; Martin and Webster, 1989).
As noted above, echolocating Michrochiroptera can be grouped according to their use of echolocation and their approach to foraging (Table 2). The majority of species use echolocation for medium-to short-range target or obstacle detection. Species foraging at short range, feeding on insects, nectar and pollen, fruit or blood, emit short, broad-band echolocation calls with no clear dominant frequency. In these species, including representatives of the Molossidae, Mormoopidae, Phyllostomidae and Vespertilionidae, we found moderate directionality, IID and pinna gain, generally confirming our hypothesis. But there were hints of specializations in the phyllostomids Desmodus rotundus and Trachops cirrhosus. In D. rotundus, locally increased directionality of pinna sound pressure transformation at 25–35kHz produced a local increase of the pinna gain, coinciding with the sensitivity of neurones in the inferior colliculus that show high sensitivity to breathing noises, containing energy in the range of 25–35kHz (Schmidt et al. 1991). Together, these results strongly suggest that D. rotundus depends upon prey-generated noises to find its targets. The large pinnae of T. cirrhosus should mean large pinna gain at low frequencies, but we found a steep IID slope over the whole frequency range. Trachops may not need high pinna gain to detect loudly calling frogs (Tuttle and Ryan, 1982), a situation demanding accurate localization. The high inclination of the IID frequency display allows accurate lateralization, and the exceptionally high maximum IID at frequencies of frog calls (<4kHz, Tuttle and Ryan, 1982; Tuttle et al. 1985) can enhance this ability. Other species that locate prey by prey-generated noises (Megadermatidae and Nycteridae, see below) are probably not confronted with the problem of localizing a sound source of limited bandwidth. Rustling sounds have a much broader bandwidth than frog calls so the pinnae improve directional hearing and especially amplify at slightly higher frequencies.
Many species are flexible in their sonar behaviour (e.g. Obrist, 1989) and may adapt their signal structure to extend their sonar range (Simmons and Stein, 1980). This is achieved by increasing call duration and limiting energy to a narrow spectral band. Many insectivorous bats change their call design drastically over the course of an attack or an insect pursuit. For example, during a long-range search, L. borealis uses predominantly narrow-band signals (Obrist, 1989) and its pinna properties are correlated with the dominant spectral sonar component (Fig. 9). Matching of the external ears to the frequencies dominating echolocation calls is reflected in many species, and has been assumed in species not considered here (e.g. Euderma maculatum, Guppy and Coles, 1988a). Bats with low-frequency sonar calls should require large pinnae, but this is not the case in Lasiurus cinereus where the pinna matches the second harmonic of the call. The occurrence of L. cinereus at high latitudes may mean keeping ears small to minimize heat loss, and matching the pinna to the second harmonic may reflect this environmental constraint. Since this species also occurs in tropical areas, the strength of this relationship would be questionable.
Specialization for long-range target detection becomes clearer in the high-flying species of the Emballonuridae and especially some Molossidae, both showing more obvious correspondence between directionality patterns and echolocation calls. In some emballonurids, pinna features best matched the FM part of their vocalizations, and in other species they best matched the second harmonic of the call. In the Molossidae, ear features were distinctly related to call characteristics. In the three species for which echolocation calls are known, the IID shows peaks and steepest slope at the dominant sonar component (Fig. 8C). Molossids are presumed to hunt insects at high altitudes in relatively fast flight and to use sonar signals of narrow bandwidth (Simmons et al. 1978) to increase effective range. The rhinopomatid Rhinopoma hardwickei uses comparable foraging and echolocation (Simmons et al. 1984) and shows similar ear characteristics, which should favour the localization of signals, used in long-range target detection.
Two Old World families, the Hipposideridae and the Rhinolophidae, and one representative of the New World Mormoopidae (P. parnellii ) have evolved anatomical and physiological specializations (e.g. Bruns et al. 1976) to detect fluttering insects. These specialisations allow decoding of frequency and amplitude modulations ‘imprinted’ at the rate of the insect wingbeat on the echo returning to the bat (e.g. Kober and Schnitzler, 1990). Using an analogy from the visual system, the hearing world of these bats probably has a slightly ‘monochromatic’ touch, and the characteristics of their pinnae reflect this view. We found an unmistakable ‘tuning’ of the external ears to the CF echolocation call components in Hipposideridae and Rhinolophidae (Fig. 8D–F). The same pattern does not fit all species, however, for some exhibit peak pinna gain, others highest interaural intensity differences and still others steepest IID in the spectral range of the CF part of their echolocation calls. The IID frequency display showed the most consistent specializations, again substantiating the importance of interaural intensity differences for echolocation. However, bats in these families have conspicuous ear movements (e.g. Pye and Roberts, 1970), correlated with sound emissions. These movements seem to be crucial for target localization in the vertical plane (Mogdans et al. 1988). We predict that the movements will affect the IID available to the auditory system. An accurate reconstruction of IIDs at different frequencies and at different positions of the pinnae could provide an interesting comparison. Pteronotus parnellii shows an interesting convergence with hipposiderids and rhinolophids, coinciding with specializations in its auditory pathway, which is tuned to the dominant CF harmonic at 60 kHz (Pye, 1980b). The specializations reflect the use of Doppler-shifted echoes to ensure that returning echoes are at the frequency of greatest sensitivity of its auditory system. Our results indicate that the directionality of sound pressure transformation at the external ear and derived qualities such as the interaural intensity difference and pinna gain are also specialized compared with other mormoopids (Fig. 12).
Finally, species that detect and locate targets by listening to prey-generated noises have large ears that strongly amplify frequencies below 15kHz (Fig. 13). In megadermatids and nycterids, the high slope of the IID frequency display at these frequencies will further improve their localization of sound sources. Arthropods or small vertebrates rustling on a dry substratum create noises with most energy below 25kHz, the spectral range where large pinnae show their advantage in directionality over more orthodox ears (Fig. 13). The matching of directionality, and especially of the IID patterns, with the dominant components of search-phase echolocation calls or prey-generated noises in so many species convinces us of the importance of the external ears for the generation of the intensity cues necessary for sound localization. Pinna structures of bat species reflect the parallel evolution of production, reception and analysis of echolocation sounds and the approach these species have taken to foraging.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Echolocation calls recorded by J. D. Pye were supplied for comparison by the British Library of Wildlife Sounds. We thank M. Licht for his help with figure preparation, Dr P. Witzke for technical assistance and Drs M. Brigham, B. Hickey and D. Pinheiro for helpful comments on the manuscript. This research was made possible by a postdoctoral fellowship of the Swiss National Foundation for the Promotion of Science to M.K.O., an operating grant from NSERC to M.B.F. and the SFB 204 (Gehör) München.