ABSTRACT
Melanin has been extracted from dorsal and ventral components of the skin of English frogs by a simple method involving the leaching out of water-soluble pigments, the peptic digestion of washed skin, and the centrifugation of unaffected pigment. The melanin was estimated in NaOH solution with the aid of an indian-ink colour standard and by the use of a Duboscq colorimeter calibrated against suitable neutral density filters at various wave-lengths.
Melanin varies greatly in amount in dorsal skins, some of which contain ten times as much of the pigment as others. In extreme duskiness and pallor, the difference may be fiftyfold. In spite of such variability, melanin formation seems generally to follow the usual laws of growth.
Ventral pigmentation varies more than dorsal, ranging from an almost pigmentless condition to equality with dorsal pigmentation. The mean amount of melanin in ventral skin is about 27 % of that in dorsal skin.
Melanin occurs in the skin of females at spawning time in large amount. Thus, the pale, red colour of the skin is not due to lack of the pigment. Melanin does not differ significantly in amount in the sexes.
Melanin can be determined by careful visual judgement of the tone of the skin to within one-third of the total amount present in dark skin.
The melanin content of dorsal skin is increased or decreased when frogs are kept for some weeks in equally illuminated light-absorbing (black) or lightscattering (white) surroundings. The skin of the dark-adapted animal contains 60 % more melanin than that of the light-adapted one after 5 weeks of treatment.
The melanin content of the skin of a limb is increased under post-mortem conditions following intramuscular injection of post-pituitary extract into the freshly excised living limb. An increase of 27 % was determined. No effect is produced by adrenaline under these conditions.
Prolonged adaptation to light-absorbing or light-scattering surroundings produces a more fundamental effect than the mere dispersion or aggregation of pre-existing melanin granules in the melanophores, namely, the production of fresh melanin or the degradation of this pigment. The subtractive process, which is evident microscopically, is due to some agent at present unknown.
Frog melanin is shown by its absorption characteristics to be chemically very closely similar to, and possibly absolutely identical with, mammalian melanin and the pigment of melanoma.
INTRODUCTION
Few zoologists can have failed to observe the differences of tone and colour which are evident in the skins of freshly collected English frogs. Such differences of tone may be entirely unrelated to the state of aggregation or dispersion of melanin granules in the melanophores, because frogs which have been kept for some time in a vivarium under identical conditions of light intensity and background can still be grouped as pale, fairly dark, and dark individuals with closely similar melanophoral indices. Nothing is known about the quantitative basis of such tone variations, and one aim of the present work is to evaluate the amounts of melanin normally present in the skin of the frog by the use of relatively simple methods of melanin extraction and estimation.
Most workers on the chromatic response to photic stimulation have confined their attention to the state of aggregation or dispersion of pigment granules in the chromatophores, i.e. to the phenomena of “contraction” and “expansion” of melanophores. The melanophore index devised by Hogben is the means now usually employed for measuring the magnitude of movement evident in granules conceived to exist already. According to the work of Hogben & Slome (1936), the black and white background responses of Amphibia are dependent upon localized retinal elements. A substance “B”, which is secreted by the pars intermedia of the pituitary complex, brings about the dispersion of melanin granules under conditions of overhead illumination in light-absorbing surroundings, while another hormone “W”, which is secreted by the pars tuberalis or by some organ functionally dependent upon it, brings about the aggregation of such granules under conditions of overhead illumination in light-scattering surroundings. Attempts to detect the “W” hormone have not been successful so far (Abramowitz, 1939), and the American school favour a unihormonal theory.
Recent researches have tended to show, however, that the chromatic response has wider implications than merely the movements of pre-existing melanin granules, and involves increase or decrease in the total amount of pigment present in the skin, at least, after prolonged exposure to light-absorbing or light-scattering surroundings. What little evidence happens to be available is based upon counts of melanophores (Babak, 1910-13; Kuntz, 1916; Murisier, 1920-1 ; Ordiorne, 1933, 1936, 1937; and others). While the possibility exists of variability in the amount of melanin in individual melanophores, such evidence cannot be regarded as entirely conclusive. The more satisfactory method of chemical extraction and estimation of melanin has been used in the simplest possible form by Viltner (1931), who experimented with two axolotls on white and black backgrounds respectively, and by Sumner & Doudoroff (1937, 1938), who met with only limited success, so that they were compelled to discard the method in favour of one involving counts of melanophores.
The swiftness with which the chromatic response is evoked in fishes militates against the possibility of processes of synthesis and degradation of melanin, because the tyrosinase-tyrosine reaction is a slow one. It has been shown recently, however (Hogben & Landgrebe, 1940), that the full effect of the chromatic response in Gasterosteus is evident only when time graphs are taken into consideration. The initial stages of the background response are rapid, but the final stages are prolonged. This is compatible with the idea of pigment synthesis and destruction, because it permits of the requisite period for such changes. The present work was executed partly to find out whether or not such additive and subtractive pigmentary changes are involved in the chromatic response.
Another aim of the present work is to compare spectrophotometric characteristics of the melanin of an amphibian with that of hair. Recent work has shown that in all likelihood, melanic pigments of the hair in various mammals and of melanoma are chemically very closely similar if not identical (Zwicky & Almasy, 1935 ; Einsele, 1937; Daniel, 1938; Arnow, 1938). It is of some importance to ascertain whether or not such fundamental resemblances in the nature of melanin extend to the melanin of an amphibian.
MATERIAL AND METHODS
The methods which have been adopted by the writer are not entirely original, but they are free adaptations of such methods as have proved satisfactory for the extraction and estimation of melanin from hair, differing in several particulars. It would seem that much of the difficulty which was experienced by Sumner & Doudoroff (1937, 1938) was due to their use of whole fish for the analyses, which were somewhat cumbersome. Such difficulty has been eliminated in the present work by the use of stripped skin. Melanin extractions are based upon well-known properties of melanin to resist solution in acid or neutral solvents but to dissolve fairly readily in alkalies, properties which were determined for the melanin of the English frog by a series of experiments which need not be specified. The proteins of the skin were broken down by digestion with pepsin in acid solution and not by hydrolysis with strong acid, as is necessary when the melanin is associated with keratin (Einsele, 1937 ; Daniel, 1938; Arnow, 1938). The writer had no opportunity of using a spectrophotometer, and the estimation of extracted melanin was effected by means of a Duboscq colorimeter used in conjunction with suitable spectrum and neutral density filter’s. The use of this instrument may involve many sources of possible error, but all details of procedure were standardized so that any errors inherent in the technique are as uniform as they can be made. The work, it must be emphasized, is a preliminary study, which will be refined and amplified when opportunity permits.
ANIMALS
The English frogs (Rana temporaria) used in general analyses of the normal melanin content of the skin were taken from a vivarium as soon as possible after capture. The animals which were employed for study of the background effects were kept in glass containers wrapped externally with white or black paper respectively. The light-reflecting or light-absorbing surroundings of the animals were subjected to identical conditions of incident daylight. Animals and containers were washed in running water daily. None of the animals took food, but it is presumed that the effects of inanition upon the melanin content of skin, if any, were the same for all. No ill effects of captivity were evident after several weeks, and this may be attributed partly to the fact that any animal of apparent poor condition was rejected at the outset.
SAMPLES OF SKIN USED FOR THE MELANIN DETERMINATIONS
Samples of skin for analysis were obtained as follows. The animal was induced to urinate by gentle squeezing of the abdomen, after which it was blotted of water, weighed, pithed and skinned. The skin was removed in dorsal and ventral regions, which were first of all marked out by a horizontal incision along the sides of the body and the limbs as far as knee and elbow. Such a line roughly separates the more heavily pigmented dorsal from the more lightly pigmented ventral surface. All skin samples were carefully cleared of shreds of connective tissue and blood vessels, blotted of superfluous fluid and weighed. Preliminary experiments showed that drying agents, alcohol or air, impede processes subsequently made use of for breaking down the skin, and dry weights were sacrificed to easier destruction of the skin. Skin weights, it must be mentioned, were far too variable to be of any real value in this work.
The general tone of the skin was noted and the animals roughly classified as “pale”, “medium dark”, or “dark”. The melanophore index of dorsal skin was read consistently, as was that of the skin in the web of the foot. Small fragments of dorsal skin were removed, one from the same situation in each sample, cleared in xylol and mounted in Canada balsam. These were used initially in attempts to determine light transmissions of whole skin samples. Two microscopes were focused and were linked by a comparison eyepiece. The mounted skin was placed on the stage of one microscope, a neutral density filter on the stage of the other. Both objects were covered with a thin slip of matt glass. Neutral density filters were changed one for another until visual matching of the halves of the field was achieved when various filters were used in apposition to the eyepiece. It was thought by this means to find a quick method of ascertaining the approximate density of whole skin; the results showed, however, that it is not possible to make observations to within a density of O-2-O-3, and the observations were discontinued because they proved to be little better than ordinary visual judgements. The mounted fragments of skin came to serve a useful purpose, however, for comparing counts of melanophores with the densities of NaOH solutions of melanin from the same skins.
Excluding preliminary experiments which were devised to fix the necessary standards, analyses were made of the amounts of melanin in the dorsal and ventral skins of sixty-five frogs (thirty-eight males and twenty-seven females). Eleven other animals were employed in experiments to determine whether or not injections of adrenaline and post-pituitary extract produce any post-mortem effects on the melanic pigmentation of skin. Experiments on the effects of hypophysectomy and on the effect of repeated injections of post-pituitary extract were ruined by death of the animals during a period of high summer temperature. In view of the results which have emerged from experiments on the background effects, it now seems extremely likely that well-marked reduction in the amount of melanin would result from long-standing hypophysectomy. This matter awaits further experiments, which will be carried out at some future date.
SAMPLES OF EGGS AND EMBRYOS USED FOR MELANIN DETERMINATIONS
Copulating male and female Rana temporaria were segregated and the fertilized eggs were collected and placed in well-oxygenated tap water. Samples intended for analysis were withdrawn from specific batches of eggs at intervals. At the outset, 120 eggs formed one sample, but this number was reduced to sixty or less in some instances. Altogether, nearly 2000 eggs formed the basis of twenty-three estimations of melanin. The main purpose of these experiments was to prepare curves of log density/wave-length and to compare these with similar curves for melanin from adult skin ; in short, to compare the melanin from these two sources spectrophoto-metrically. Each sample of eggs was boiled for a few minutes in distilled water, dried superficially in a stream of air, weighed and stored temporarily under 0.1N HCl. The samples were treated with the minimum of delay exactly as were samples of skin (vide infra), except that the concentration of pigment in the final NaOH solution was adjusted to meet colorimetric requirements. These solutions were standardized so that 90 c.c. contained the melanin of sixty eggs.
EXTRACTION OF MELANIN
The weighed skin was placed in boiling distilled water and kept boiling for 10 min. This treatment presumably destroys tyrosinase, and it washes out of the skin yellow and greenish pigments contained therein, but leaves melanin unaffected. The skin, now white in colour except where melanophores darken it, was cooled and digested by means of pepsin. The enzyme used (commercial pepsin) was made up in KCl-HCl buffer at pH 2, and each dorsal or ventral skin received an ample quantity(100 c.c.) of the 1% solution, to which toluol was added in standard amount. During the period of incubation, the skins showed no signs of blackening, and it is presumed that post-mortem melanogenesis did not occur. Incubation proceeded at 35° C. for 48 hr. After remaining overnight in the enzyme solution, the skin commenced to disintegrate, slight agitation being sufficient to induce it to break up into small fragments. After 24 hr. the skin was reduced to a fine cellular sediment from which individual melanophores could be picked out with a pipette, and after a further 24 hr. the melanophores were disrupted, and all that remained of the skin was a clear solution and a sediment of fine brown or black melanin granules.
Each digestion mixture was diluted subsequently with a standard volume of distilled water. After settling of the pigment, the supernatant liquid was decanted and set aside for a few minutes, while the remaining melanin was washed in distilled water by a process of decantation and centrifuging. The decanted fluid was then centrifuged and the supernatant liquid was passed through a fine filter. In the case of dark skins, a very fine film of melanin particles collected on the filter ; these were washed and passed through into the final NaOH solution with the alkali. The final solutions were such that the melanin collected from a dorsal or a ventral skin was contained in 50 c.c. 0.4% NaOH (Analar). The pigment granules did not all pass into solution in the cold, but those which remained invariably dissolved shortly after boiling commenced, showing that melanin from the frog is more readily soluble in alkali than melanin extracted from hair. Daniel (1938) found great variability in the time of boiling needed to dissolve whole samples of melanin from the hairs of mice ; this is not the case for melanin obtained from the skins of frogs. Boiling was continued, however, for 15 min., after which the volume of the NaOH solution was brought back to 50 c.c. by additions of distilled water. The resultant solution was never cloudy. Undoubtedly it contained impurities, but these did not colour the solution and in no way affected colorimetric determinations. The colour intensity of these final solutions varied greatly, as the ranges of density to be mentioned presently will show; extracts of ventral skin were straw coloured, while those of dorsal skin varied between golden brown and deep brown. After several weeks in the dark, some melanin granules may emerge from such solutions, but they can be returned to solution by slight boiling, or even warming. Apart from this, the solutions remained perfectly clear and apparently unchanged for several months. Unlike the solutions obtained from fishes by Sumner & Doudoroff (1937, 1938), they were eminently suitable for colorimetric observation.
CALIBRATION AND EXPERIMENTAL USE OF THE DUBOSCQ COLORIMETER
The Duboscq colorimeter was made easier to use by affixing a small mirror obliquely beneath the vernier and its scale. This made it possible to read the instrument from a sitting posture. Lighting and screening arrangements were as follows. A Phillips argenta (opal) lamp (60W., 210V., run at 210V.) was fixed in. from the mirror of the instrument and an Ilford no. 810 light filter was set in the light path in. from the light source. A screen of thick cardboard was built up round the lamp and a light tunnel of the same material was made to complete screening arrangements right up to the instrument, the front of which was enclosed in a screen of black paper. A further screen was placed around the eyepiece. Readings were taken in a room which had black walls and lacked windows, and all possible precautions were taken to ensure accuracy of results. Special care was exercised in allowing sufficient time for dark adaptation of the eye on entering the dark room and in avoiding visual fatigue, as well as other likely sources of error outlined by Yoe (1928). Readings of the density of several test solutions by three independent observers at the outset were in consistently close agreement.1 All readings of the vernier were made in the light of a small hand lamp, which was completely screened during the time when the observations were being made.
Calibration of the Duboscq colorimeter was carried out in the following manner. The right cup of the instrument was used for the colour standard, which was prepared from Reeve’s Indian ink (which contains pure C) in preference to a melanin solution because the latter is said to fade with age (Sumner & Doudoroff, 1937, 1938). Tests showed this estimable fluid to be very stable to rather severe conditions (dilution, heating, treatment with acid, etc.). The strength of the standard chosen after tests with melanin solutions prepared from the darkest and palest skins obtainable was 0.029 % by volume ink. This was prepared by adding 6 drops of the ink from a stalagmometer to 1l. of distilled water, and its strength was determined by weighings of 100 drops of ink from the same stalagmometer and of an equal volume of distilled water. The standard showed no signs of deterioration after many months.
The left cup of the colorimeter was filled to constant level with distilled water and the effective depth adjusted to 1.00 cm. The depth of the colour standard in the right cup was adjusted to produce visual matching of the halves of the field when a set of Ilford neutral density filters were placed one at a time above the left cup and cylinder. In this way readings of the depth of standard in the right cup corresponding to known neutral densities were obtained. Nine neutral density filters were used to cover a suitable range of densities; their density values were o.1, 0.2, 0.3, o.6, 0.9, 1.2, 1.5, 1.8 and 2.1. This range of densities proved to be adequate for all the melanin solutions examined.
Matching of right and left halves of the field of the colorimeter was effected for various wave-lengths of light by the further use of eight Ilford spectrum filters (nos. 601-8), which were set above the eyepiece so as to take in both halves of the field. Three readings of the depth of the colour standard required to match a particular neutral density filter and the 1 cm. depth of distilled water were made for each spectrum filter used, and the mean value was calculated. The results, a set of readings of depths of colour standard corresponding to the specified neutral densities for each spectrum filter, were plotted on squared paper to a large scale (30 × 22 in.). They provided eight smooth curves, which were employed for transposing colorimetric readings subsequently made with melanin solutions into neutral densities, due regard being paid to particular spectrum filters employed at any time.
In the melanin estimations the only difference from the calibration experiments was the substitution in the left cup of the colorimeter of the NaOH solution of melanin in constant amount set at the constant depth of 1.oo cm. for the distilled water and neutral density filter. In these estimations, the mean of three readings for each spectrum filter was used except where individual readings varied by more than 0.05 cm. (which was rarely), in which case three more readings were made and the mean of six calculated. The technique employed minimized movements of the colorimeter cups and the range for the right cup throughout the whole of the work was relatively slight, 1.72 cm. down to 0.07 cm.
THE TERM “DENSITY”
This applies to gases and solutions when dilution does not involve complicating change. It applies to solutions only when the solvent is transparent. In the writer’s estimations, the thickness of the melanin solution was kept constant at unity (1.00 cm.), the quantity of melanin solution per skin sample was kept constant at 50 c.c., and the density was thus directly proportional to the concentration of the melanin derived from a single skin sample. In statements subsequently made concerning the concentrations of melanin, mean density values for the eight spectrum filters are quoted. These must be more accurate figures than could be obtained by the use of any one filter. In graphs, density values and their logarithms are plotted for wave-lengths at which the spectrum filters give maximum transmission, namely, 4290, 4710, 4940, 5200, 5460, 5830, 6160 and 6870 A. The final nought is omitted in readings shown on the wave-length scale of the graphs.
RESULTS
I. The melanin content of the skin in relation to size
If melanin is a product of metabolism which is stored in situations where it is formed, the skin must contain more of this pigment in large frogs than in small ones. The relation between the melanin content of the skin and the size of the body is not clearly shown by the available data because of wide variability in the small number of estimations which could be made. Some of the twenty-four males and twentyfour females which provided the data bearing on the melanin content of dorsal and ventral skin were selected for extreme duskiness or pallor so as to provide a measure of the extent of variability. This partly explains the wide scattering of the points shown in Fig. 1, where densities have been plotted against body weights. The complete range of densities evaluated is 0·13-1·29 (males) and 0·15-1·34 (females). Densities for the dorsal skin of an exceptionally pale and another exceptionally dark male (nos. 20 and 46 respectively) were 0·02 and 1·66 respectively. The density for the dorsal skin of the darkest female was 1-34, while in one other female there was no appreciable amount of melanin in the dorsal skin. Thus, it is plain that some dorsal skins contain ten times as much melanin as others and that in exceptional instances of duskiness and pallor such a difference may be fiftyfold.
Despite such variability as this, it can be shown that melanin formation follows the usual laws of growth. The curves drawn through the points shown in Fig. 1 connect mean values of density for arbitrary size classes (Table 1) ; in either sex, the curve is a portion of a sigmoid curve covering a fourfold increase in body weight. Extreme variation in the amounts of melanin in dorsal skin is probably unconnected with normal growth. But it seems very probable that melanin in the dorsal skin increases relatively more rapidly than body weight. The dorsal skin of the mean male frog of 40 g. weight may contain more than three times as much melanin as the dorsal skin of the mean male of half this weight. Females considered over this size range show less than a threefold difference but more than a twofold one.
II. The pigmentation of the ventral skin
The ventral skin of the English frog is usually very pale, but it may be almost indistinguishable in tone and colour from the dorsal skin. The causes of such variability are unknown. As would be expected after such considerations have been made, melanin solutions prepared from ventral skin show a wide range of densities. In three males (53, 33 and 54) the ratio density of extract of ventral skin/density of extract of dorsal skin was evaluated as 0·04, 0·51 and 0·93 respectively; in three females (74, 26 and 28) the corresponding ratios were 0·09,0·50 and 0·90 respectively. It is true to say, therefore, that ventral pigmentation ranges from equality with dorsal skin down to an almost pigmentless condition. Indeed, in five males and in three females, no melanin could be detected in the ventral skin. In one male, the dorsal skin contained less melanin than the ventral skin. Extreme pallor was the cause of this anomaly, the density for the dorsal skin being as low as 0·13 (no. 59; weight, 14 g.). In most individuals there is more than four times as much melanin in dorsal as in ventral skin ; in thirteen males and fourteen females out of twentyfour individuals of either sex this was the case. Where pigmentation of the ventral skin is well marked, this skin may contain more than half as much melanin as the dorsal skin. The mean melanin content of ventral skin in either sex is 27 % of the amount in dorsal skin (Table 2, lines 1 and 2). For equal weights of skin,1 in either sex, the percentage is 44.
III. The melanin content of the dorsal skins
The males used for estimations of melanin normally present in the skin ranged in weight from 14 to 43 g., the females from 14 to 42 g. (plus a very small female, 5 g. weight). While mean weights are identical unless standard errors are taken into account, mean densities of melanin solutions prepared from the dorsal skin differ in the sexes, the male possessing about 15 % more of this pigment than the female.
Consideration of the standard errors of the densities suggests, however, that the sexual difference in the degree of pigmentation may be more apparent than real (see Table 2, lines 1 and 2). The pertinent conclusion reached when the weight or the area of the skin is taken into account is that there is no significant difference. Fewer dark and more pale females than males were used for the estimations, a fact which may account for the apparent difference.
Ten of the females were used immediately upon completion of the spawning act. They were mostly pale or only medium dark in the tone of the skin, but were tinged with red and in some instances very red. The densities of the NaOH solutions of melanin from the dorsal skins of these individuals greatly exceeded those of the remaining fourteen individuals, the ratio of the two sets of densities being 4/3. The difference may be due to the greater mean weight of the females which had spawned. No significant difference is evident if we calculate the density per unit weight of animal or of skin (Table 2, lines 3 and 4). The important conclusion can be drawn, however, that the pale tone of the skin evident in association with a red coloration in females during the breeding season is not due to the lack of melanin, because this pigment (or one which shares its light-absorption characteristics) is present at such time in full amount.
Plots of log density against wave-length (Fig. 2) yield curves for the various melanin samples of which the slopes are uniform. From this fact, we can infer that no significant chemical difference exists in the nature of the melanin which occurs in the dorsal skin of males and females (D.S.) and females at spawning time (D).). The slopes of the curves for ventral skin (V.S.) are not as steep as those for dorsal skin, but they show the characteristic absorption curve of melanin, namely, gradually increasing transmission from the violet towards the red end of the spectrum.
IV. The correlation between visual judgement and estimation of the amount of melanin in the dorsal skin
Animals were classified in the first instance by visual judgement of the tone of the skin as “dark”, “medium dark”, and “pale” individuals. The density determinations of melanin solutions made, they were reclassified as “DARK”, “MEDIUM DARK”, and “PALE”. The density ranges marking out the latter groups were greater than o.8, 0·8-0·4, and less than 0·4 respectively. Such differences of pigmentation are largely due to growth, the mean dark animal greatly exceeding the mean pale one in weight (Table 2, lines 5.10). This dual treatment showed that the tone of the skin is a fairly reliable if not an infallible index of the amount of melanin contained in the skin, as the data in Table 3 indicate. Of forty-eight individuals which were classified as nine “dark”, twenty “medium dark”, and nineteen “pale”, ten were “DARK”, twenty “MEDIUM DARK” and eighteen “PALE”. The correlation between visual judgements and photometric determinations is not as close as these final numbers suggest, because five individuals classified as “dark” and four classified as “pale” were found to be “MEDIUM DARK”. In only one instance out of forty-eight, however, was visual judgement erroneous by more than one-third of the total amount of melanin present in the dark skin. It is thus possible to judge by superficial examination of the dorsal skin of the amount of melanin contained in it to within one-third of its amount. This holds good whether the individual has been kept on a light-scattering or a light-absorbing background, or on one which has neither of these attributes, in which case the melanophore index provides no reliable measure of pigmentary differences.
The curves relating log density and wave-length for melanin solutions obtainec from “DARK”, “MEDIUM DARK”, and “PALE” individuals differ in their positions 01 the grid of co-ordinates and in their slope. The difference in log density between 4290 and 6870 A. ranges from 0·35 for “DARK” to 0·45 for “PALE” animals, i.e. th< slope is slightly steeper in the latter. This slight departure from the parallel is no sufficient to signify any essential difference in the nature of the melanin present it the dorsal skins of such individuals. The results of such plotting indicate strongb that the essential difference between “DARK” and “PALE” individuals is a pureb quantitative and not a qualitative difference in melanic pigmentation.
V. The effect of black and of white background upon the melanic pigmentation of the dorsal skin
Seventeen individuals, not included in the general estimates (three females amongst them) were kept on equally illuminated white or black backgrounds foi several weeks. Under these conditions, individuals in light-reflecting surroundings pale considerably, while those in light-absorbing surroundings darken. At the end of a period of 5-6 weeks the two sets of individuals show extremes of pallor and duskiness. The aim of these experiments was to ascertain whether or not additive and subtractive processes are at work during this period, i.e. whether or not lightabsorbing and light-scattering surroundings produce significant differences in melanic pigmentation when maintained for some time. Eight individuals (one on them a female) were kept on a black background for a mean period of 36 days ; nine individuals (two of them females) were kept on a white background for a mean period of 33 days.
The outstanding result of these experiments, which are summarized in Table 2 (lines 11 and 12) and Fig. 3, is the much greater density of the melanin solutions prepared from the dorsal skins of the animals which were kept on the black background. The dorsal skin of such animals contains, on the average, nearly 60% more melanin than the dorsal skin of animals which have been kept on a white background for a corresponding period. Some of the excess is doubtless due to the larger size of the mean darkened animal, but there is no ambiguity in the general result obtained. Unit weight of dorsal skin contains 50 % more melanin in the case of the darkened animals than in that of the paled ones. The large standard error is attributable to considerable size differences in all the animals concerned, with consequent large differences in the amount of pigment contained in their skins. Comparison of the two curves relating density and wave-length for the mean animals of these experiments (Fig. 3) with those for the mean animal which was not kept on any particular background (Fig. 2) shows that the former fall respectively above and below the latter. The mean animal (male-female) provides a curve which lies closer to the curve for the white-adapted animal than to that for the black-adapted one. These results show fairly conclusively that the light-scattering or light-absorbing nature of the surroundings exerts a profound effect upon the melanin content of the dorsal skin, the former decreasing and the latter increasing the absolute amount of pigment present. The slight departure from the parallel which these curves for light- and dark-adapted animals show is insufficient to point to any qualitative modification of the pigment. The dual effect of the background is quantitative, additive or subtractive.
Further evidence concerning the effect of white or black backgrounds was sought in counts of melanophores. The situation chosen for the counting was a constant area of the skin of the web of the foot between the last two (post-axial) digits. The results of numerous counts show that in animals which have been kept in black surroundings for periods up to 5 weeks, the number of melanophores is at least 12% and possibly more than 15% greater than the number counted in an equal area of skin from animals which have been kept in white surroundings for corresponding periods. Reliance cannot be placed on the actual figures, because the melanophores of dark-adapted animals with dispersed pigment granules are illdefined and in consequence more difficult to count. But the tendency to undercount “expanded” melanophores would make it necessary to raise the percentage figure, and the general result expressed is all the more likely to be significant. A further observation which is important in relation to light-adaptation is the occurrence in the skin of animals which have been kept for periods of weeks on a white background of “islands” of skin which are devoid of melanophores but contain loose aggregates of melanin granules. Such “islands”, which are very evident in the web of the foot, do not prove that disintegration and destruction of melanin occurs during prolonged light-adaptation, but they strongly suggest that this is the case.
The excess of melanophores of dark-adapted animals over those of light-adapted ones is smaller than might be expected from consideration of the densities of the melanin solutions if, as some writers insist, the numbers of such pigment cells provide reliable criteria by which to evaluate the amount of pigment present in the skin. This apparent discrepancy induced the writer to measure the sizes of the compacted masses of pigment granules in many melanophores of the same and different individuals, under conditions of consistently close packing. The sizes of such granular masses vary considerably. Thus, in a pale individual which had been kept on a white background and consequently gave a low density reading for melanin extracted from the dorsal skin (0·18), the pigment masses were extremely uniform in size and measured approximately 0·019-0·021 mm. in diameter. Another pale individual (also a male) submitted to a similar experimental treatment but containing more melanin (density 0·41) possessed melanophoral pigment masses of extremely variable size ; the smallest ones were spherical and measured 0·021 mm. in diameter, while the majority measured 0-025 mm in diameter and many large ovoid masses, which were not evident in the previous animal, measured as much as 0·048 × 0·037 mm. Were it necessary, these findings could be amplified. They lead to the conclusion that counts of melanophores do not provide reliable information concerning the amounts of pigment contained in the skin unless the sizes of the pigment masses are also taken into account. This would present no difficulty in the case of light-adapted animals with aggregated pigment granules, but it would be wellnigh impossible in the case of dark-adapted animals with dispersed pigment granules. From such counts as were made by the writer it appears certain that larger amounts of pigment in the skin are evident as relatively large aggregates of granules in the melanophores, as well as in enhanced numbers of such cells.
VI. Post-mortem effect of injections of post-pituitary extract and adrenaline on the melanin content of the skin
The following rather crude experiments on a possible post-mortem effect of post-pituitary extract on the melanin content of the skin were carried out in lieu of the experiments with whole animals which failed as a result of death of the animals concerned. Six frogs were pithed and weighed. The hind limbs were severed from the body by a transverse cut immediately anterior to the pubes and were then separated by a median vertical cut through the symphysis. The right limbs, which served as controls, each received an intramuscular injection of 1 c.c. normal saline ; the left limbs were given an intramuscular injection of 1 c.c. post-pituitary extract (Ferris and Co.). The limbs were placed in separate Petri dishes, the atmospheres of which were kept saturated with moisture by means of pieces of damp blotting paper set at a distance from the limbs. The dishes and their contents were incubated at 35° C. for 48 hr., at the end of which time each skin was stripped from its limb in one piece, superficially dried, weighed, and used for a melanin determination in the manner previously described.
The density values of melanin extracts of the skin from pituitary-treated and untreated limbs (Table 4) can scarcely be regarded as showing a significant difference when they are considered in conjunction with their standard errors. Two points call for strong emphasis, however. These standard errors are relatively large because the animals differed markedly in size and because, as a consequence, very different amounts of melanin were under consideration. Further, the density value for the extract of melanin of skin from the pituitary-treated limb was in every instance greater than that of the control. These considerations made, it seems extremely probable that the difference in density on the two sides of the body (27% more melanin per skin of the pituitary-injected limb, or, 33 % more melanin per unit weight of skin from the injected side) is significant, i.e. that intramuscular injection of post-pituitary extract enhances the melanin content of the skin under the conditions specified.
The curves relating density and wave-length for the mean estimates of these experiments (Fig. 4) are similar to those obtained in other instances but are somewhat steeper. This difference would be expected (if not to quite the extent found)from a consideration of the fact that pale individuals were chosen as the likeliest to show any possible effect in these experiments, because, as has been shown, the slope of the curve is steeper in pale than in dark individuals. The fact that the curve for the control animal is of the same steepness shows that this slight departure from the general results obtained cannot be due to the extract which was injected.
Five individuals were treated similarly to those given injections of post-pituitary extract, except that they received 1 c.c. 1/1000 adrenaline, injected intramuscularly into the left limb. After 24 hr., during which no visible difference in the tone of the skin developed (the skin shows obvious darkening after injection of post-pituitary extract), the skins of the left limbs were treated with an additional 1 c.c. of the adrenaline solution and were incubated for a further 24 hr. The densities of the melanin solutions prepared from the skins of adrenaline-treated and untreated limbs (Table 4) show no significant difference when the standard errors are taken into account, though in four out of five instances the density corresponding to the left skin is greater than that corresponding to the right skin. This result is puzzling, because adrenaline produces pallor in the living animal. Another puzzling feature of these experiments is the greater weights of the skins of the left (injected) side, a difference which is not attributable to inequality of size but which may be due to greater powers of water retention in the skin of the limb that received adrenaline. The negative conclusion reached after consideration of these results is that intramuscular injection of adrenaline produces no effect on the melanin content of the skin under the conditions specified. Certainly, there is no evidence of the degradation of melanin.
VII. The melanin content of embryos of Rana temporaria
Melanin estimations were made for embryos from four broods of eggs (A, B, C and D), covering blastula, gastrula and neurula stages, and also an early stage during the formation of external gills. The results (Table 5) show that the amount of melanin present in the egg during the earliest stages of development may vary by more than 15 % and that later stages show varying amounts of melanin, which may arise partly out of initial differences but may be due partly to varying rates of melanin production. Melanin formation, which must be considerable during gastrulation, seems to fall off relatively to weight increase in the gastrula, but to keep pace with weight increase or surpass it in the neurula and later stages. When mean densities are grouped so as to represent the three stages, blastula, gastrula and neurula, without regard to brood, it appears that the transition from blastula to gastrula involves a 25 % increase in the amount of melanin and the transition from gastrula to neurula a further increase of more than 17%. The elongating neurula contains roughly half as much melanin again as the blastula (Table 5).
There is a slight divergence from the parallel in the curves of log density/wave-length at the red end of the spectrum. The mean curve for all stages (Fig. 5 ; see also Fig. 2E) is less steep than the curve for melanin from the dorsal skin of the adult frog. The most substantial departure from the parallel is shown by the curve for blastula A, which is almost identical with (slightly steeper than) the curve for melanin from the dorsal skin of the mean male. These considerations lead to the conclusion that the pigment of these embryos is identical with that of adult skin.
DISCUSSION
In recent years, several workers have brought forward spectrophotometric evidence which proves the chemical identity of several apparently different melanotic pigments. Zwicky & Almasy (1935) prepared alkaline solutions of red, black and white pigments from coloured hairs of the horse, and of the pigment of horse melanoma, and concluded after comparison of light extinction data that all these pigments are spectroscopically indistinguishable, showing gradually increasing light transmission towards the red end of the spectrum but decreasing transmission in the violet and ultra-violet. Arnow (1938) re-interpreted their data and disagreed with their main conclusion, affirming that the distinctive colour of red human hair is due to the presence of an oxidation product of melanin. Neumann (1937) reached the conclusion that black, brown and yellow melanins from the hairs of different races of rabbit are chemically so closely similar that they must have a common origin. Einsele (1937) extracted the melanin from hairs of mice of the albino series, and found that the pigments from different genotypes differ quantitatively in their light absorption as measured by a Duboscq colorimeter. Daniel (1938) made a spectrophotometric study of such melanin samples and found that eight out of eleven genotypes yielded qualitatively identical absorption spectra. She concluded that colour differences in members of this albino series are not due to differences in the chemical nature of the melanin, but must be due to variations in the quantity and distribution of the pigment. The absorption spectra of the three remaining melanin samples of her series were not so dissimilar as to account for pronounced colour differences.
It is both interesting and important to compare and contrast the melanin absorption curves prepared by Daniel (1938) for mice, by Zwicky & Almasy (1935) for the horse, and by the present writer for the frog. Those of the previous writers show certain differences of slope within the limits of the visible spectrum, and one (no. 4; black hair: Zwicky & Almasy) is anomalous, signifying increased transmission in the blue, slightly decreasing transmission in the green and yellow, and further slightly increasing transmission in the red (see their fig. 1). Otherwise, the curves are of gradual slope, those of Daniel being straight lines, except in one instance (AABBcc ; see her fig. 1). Unfortunately, numerical data are not presented and the curves reproduced are too small to permit of accurate comparison. As far as can be made out, the total differences in log density between the violet and red ends of the spectrum are 0·7-0·8 (Daniel) and 0·5 (Zwicky & Almasy). The corresponding difference which has been demonstrated in the present work varies slightly in different samples of melanin ; in melanin from the dorsal skins of normal animals it is about 0·35 while in melanin from the skins of light-adapted animals it is as much as 0·5. In pituitary-treated animals the difference is quite as considerable as in mouse melanin. In general, therefore, the slope of the curve for frog melanin is less steep than that of the curve for mouse or horse melanin, but in certain instances it attains the same steepness. In the general comparison, horse melanin is intermediate between mouse melanin and frog melanin as regards the steepness of the smooth absorption curve. Daniel reached the conclusion that melanin of the mouse is not essentially different from that of the horse. The present writer has shown that the difference between frog melanin and horse melanin is less than that between mouse melanin and horse melanin, when light absorption data form the basis of comparison. The conclusion is reached that melanin of the frog is very closely related to (if not absolutely identical with) melanin of both mouse and horse. Equally close resemblance exists between the melanin of the frog and that formed from “dopa”, as well as the red-brown pigment extracted from red human hair by prolonged boiling with 0-1N HC1 (Arnow, 1938).
Turning now to consider the effects of light- and dark-adaptation on the melanin content of the skin, it must be noted that although an extensive literature exists there is a dearth of satisfactory quantitative studies. The subject has been under recent review (Sumner, 1940b). Most of the results in this field of investigation are expressed in terms of numbers of melanophores. As far as the writer is aware, no attempt has ever been made to evaluate the amount of melanin contained in such cells, so that counts of melanophores provide at best inconclusive data.
What has been stated already about the nature of the background response might be amplified slightly at this point. There is unanimity of opinion that dispersion of melanin granules in the melanophores (black background response) occurs as a result of the secretion by the pars intermedia of the “B” hormone (see Abramowitz, 1939). The response is conceived as a movement of pre-existing granules, a conception which must be extended in the light of evidence which shows clearly that increments of melanin are made when the background response is maintained. Pigment cells of Amblystoma tend to increase in number when kept continually “expanded” and, after a time, may be 50% in excess of pigment cells which are kept continuously “contracted” (Babak, 1910-13). Flounders possess 30% fewer melanophores when kept for some time on a white background than when kept for the same time on a dark one (Kuntz, 1916). Melanophores of the dorsal fin of young trout which have been kept for a period on black and white backgrounds respectively are in the ratio of 680/295, and show an even greater difference in the caudal fin (Murisier, 1920-1). Increase in number of the melanophores does not prove that the absolute amount of melanin is increased, although it is good circumstantial evidence that this is the case. Viltner (1931) estimated the melanin extracted from two axolotls which had been kept for 17 months on black and white backgrounds respectively. Using the method of Piettre (1911), which does not preclude the possibility of post-mortem melanogenesis, he found four times as much melanin in the dark-adapted as in the light-adapted animal. Analyses have been attempted also by Sumner & Doudoroff (1937) working with Gillichthys mirabilis and later (1938) with Gambusia affinis. The rather cumbersome technique which these writers adopted yielded a brown fluid showing some cloudiness, and it had to be discarded before really satisfactory results had been achieved in favour of the method of counting melanophores. By the addition of MgCO3 to the brown fluid extracted from fishes which had been kept for about two months on white, black, and three shades of grey background, and by drying the resultant materials and grinding them with cedarwood oil, these writers obtained a paste which could be placed in special cells for the purpose of taking photometric readings. From such data as they obtained, what they termed “‘qualitative’ (i.e. only roughly quantitative results)”, the conclusion was reached that the amount of melanin varies inversely as the log of the albedo of the background (i.e. the ratio of light impinging on it to that reflected from it). The conclusion was supported by further data based upon counts of melanophores (Sumner, 1940a).
The present work affords conclusive evidence for the first time that prolonged exposure of one of the lower vertebrates to illuminated surroundings which absorb light strongly (black background) results in a marked increase in the melanin content of the skin. After exposure of frogs to such backgrounds for 33-36 days the dorsal skin of the mean animal of 23 g. weight contains roughly 35 % more melanin than the dorsal skin of the untreated animal. The skin of such an animal contains 60 % more melanin (50 % per unit weight of skin) than that of an animal which has been subjected for a similar period to an equally illuminated background which scatters light (white background). The conclusion reached is that prolonged darkadaptation achieves more than the mere dispersal of pre-existing melanin granules in melanophores, namely, the synthesis of fresh melanin.
The next question to be considered is that of the participation of the pituitary complex in this synthesis. Several researchers have shown that the post pituitary secretion increases the intensity of melanic pigmentation. The grafting of the adult hypophysis into larval Amblystoma tigrinum at the stage of sex differentiation is followed by rapid melanosis due to increase in the number of melanophores and also to the deposition of fresh granules of melanin. The animal suffers obliteration of its normal tone pattern and becomes “sooty black” (Burns, 1934). The formation of black or brown pigment from the products of secretion of the hypophysis has been observed by Roussy & Mosinger (1935), who claim that such melanin is actively produced in human beings during old age. The present work provides evidence which tends to show that the “B” hormone is concerned in the frog with the production of fresh melanin, as well as with the dispersal of melanin granules. Under specified post-mortem conditions, injection of a freshly excised living limb with post pituitary extract results during 48 hours in an increase of 27 % per skin (33 % per unit weight of skin) in the amount of melanin.
There is diversity of opinion regarding the nature of the white background response (see Abramowitz, 1939), which is generally visualized, however, as the aggregation of melanin granules in the melanophores by an agent or agents which are still in question. This conception is inadequate, because the present writer and others have produced evidence which proves that prolonged adaptation to illuminated light-scattering surroundings leads to the degeneration of melanophores and the degradation of melanin. Almost one-third of the melanophores of Fundulus are lost during four weeks of treatment on a white background, nearly two-thirds during ten weeks ; a similar if less pronounced effect is produced in Ameiurus (Ordiorne, 1933, 1936, 1937). In the writer’s opinion, this effect must be distinguished from a retardation of development of pigment in young Macropodus opercularis and Gambusia sp. when such animals are kept on a white background (1937). The one case involves the degradation of melanin, the other merely its failure to develop. The destruction of melanin has been observed by other workers, e.g. in Lebistes a few days after transference from a black to a white background by Sumner & Wells (1933). These writers could see with low magnification numerous degenerating melanophores in the skin, and in sections they noted the passage of melanin into the epidermis. The present writer has observed the degradation of melanin microscopically in the skin, and has estimated its amount analytically in melanin solutions. It is difficult to see how a failure of the “B” hormone could result in the degradation of this pigment; presumably, even an inhibitory agent such as has been isolated by Daneel & Shaumann (1938) would fail to produce this effect. This would seem to provide an objection to the unihormonal theory of adaptation to background. The fact upon which the objection could be based scarcely lends support to the conception of dual hormones propounded by Hogben and his co-workers, but if the “W” hormone exists (its existence has not been proved, nor yet disproved by the unihormonal school) it may be the agent which disrupts as well as aggregates melanin granules during prolonged light-adaptation. If not the “W” hormone, then some other powerful agent as yet unknown must be at work under such conditions. The removal of melanin from the body has been observed in human patients with Addison’s disease by Jacobson & Klingk Jun. (1934). Several paths are taken, the tubules and Henle’s loops of the kidney, the reticulo-endothelial cells of liver, spleen and lymph nodes and the colon mucosa, as well as the skin. The means whereby melanin is removed from degenerating melanophores of fishes and amphibia subjected to prolonged white-background treatment are yet to be ascertained.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The writer wishes to express his thanks to Prof. C. M. Yonge for providing him with the facilities for carrying out this research in the Department of Zoology, University of Bristol, and to Mr D. A. Kempson for much technical assistance. Mr P. B. Bradley assisted the writer with the early stages of the research.
REFERENCES
The writer is indebted to Mr D. A. Kempson and Mr P. B. Bradley for their assistance in carrying out this test of visual acuity.
The “ventral” skin invariably exceeded half the weight of the “dorsal” skin, despite the fact that the latter included the pigmented lateral as well as the truly dorsal region. It is likely that the thickness of ventral skin is much more than half that of the dorsal skin, though Francis (1934) provided the figures o-2 mm. and 0-4 mm. respectively.