ABSTRACT
Electromyograms from the elevator and depressor muscles, together with tarsal claw receptor activity, were recorded from the fourth legs of freely walking scorpions. The slope of the depressor burst duration versus step cycle time was less for short cycle times, below about 600 ms, than it was for longer cycles. The opposite was true for the elevator burst duration versus step cycle relationship, and the slope for longer cycle times was not significantly different from zero. The switching of motor activity between antagonists at the stance to swing phase transition was different from that of the swing to stance phase. The depressor burst invariably terminated before the elevator burst, while the elevator burst frequently did not terminate until after the depressor burst had begun. A similar asymmetry of the elevator/depressor motor programme has been described for insect and crustacean preparations. The termination of the depressor muscle burst represents the initial peripheral indicator that the decision to step has been made centrally. The latency between the central decision and the time when the leg is lifted, as determined by tarsal claw receptor burst termination, can be as much as 125 ms. This observation is of importance when considering both intrasegmental and intersegmental neural control mechanisms of scorpion locomotion.
INTRODUCTION
Scorpions possess the most primitive arachnid nervous system, both in terms of the organization of the central neuropile and in the low-level of ganglionic fusion (Bullock & Horridge, 1965). The segmental local control centres for all eight scorpion walking legs are located in the single suboesophageal ganglion (Babu, 1965). This consolidation may be advantageous for the generation of quick, well co-ordinated movements when hunting (Bub & Bowerman, 1979 ; Polis, 1979).
When a scorpion is walking, the contralateral legs within a segment take alternating steps, while the homolateral legs step in a highly specific 4, 2, 3, 1 order (Bowerman, 1975). For each leg the different muscles which control the individual leg joints must contract at particular times, for certain durations, with sufficient intensity achieve the co-ordinated movement of the entire leg (Root & Bowerman, 1978; Bowerman & Root, 1978). Obviously, different leg movements are controlled by different motor programmes (Sherman, Novotny & Camhi, 1977; Ayers & Davis, 1977; Reingold & Camhi, 1977). Locomotor motor programmes have been described in a number of insects and crustaceans, by recording electroneurograms (ENG’s) or electromyograms (EMG’s) in freely walking (Ewing & Manning, 1966; Macmillan, 1975; Delcomyn & Usherwood, 1973; Pearson, 1972; Clarac & Ayers, 1977; Ayers & Davis, 1977; Burns & Usherwood, 1979) or tethered preparations (Evoy & Fourtner, 1973; Delcomyn, 1973; Barnes, 1977; Spirito, Evoy & Barnes, 1972; Clarac, 1978). No comparable studies on arachnid walking motor programmes have been reported.
In this paper, activity of leg muscles is related to stepping movements in freely walking scorpions, Paruroctonus mesaensis. Electromyograms were recorded from the elevator and depressor muscles in the trochanter-femur joint, in the fourth leg, since movements of this joint account for the major elevator-depressor movements in the closely related species Hadrurus arizonensis (Root & Bowerman, 1978). Leg contact with the substrate was monitored by recording from tarsal mechanoreceptors. A similar technique has been employed for insects (Runion & Usherwood, 1968; Burns, 1973) and scorpions (Brownell & Farley, 1979 a, b).
METHODS
Specimens, Paruroctonus mesaensis, were collected near Yuma, Arizona, and maintained in individual gallon jars partially filled with sand. Each was fed a cricket or a cockroach every several weeks.
The tarsal receptor axons and cell bodies were located by cobalt backfills (see Gwilliam & Cole, 1979). The cut end of the nerve was immersed in a pool of distilled water for about 2 min, then 2 % CoCl2 overnight at 4–6 °C. The leg was washed in saline for about an hour to remove any cobalt that had leaked down the leg via the blood vessel that is associated with the leg nerve. In order to expedite fluid exchange, the cuticle was punctured several times in each segment with a minuten pin. Cobalt was precipitated by immersing the preparation for an hour in a solution of (NH4)2S. The cobalt-filled nerves were made visible by dehydrating and clearing the preparation in methyl salicylate. Drawings were made at 25 and 100 power with a dissecting microscope and camera lucida.
EMGs of the trochanter joint elevator and depressor muscles of the right fourth leg were recorded together with activity of the tarsal claw receptors. Differential electrode leads were made from 50 μm diameter insulated copper wires, with the last half mm of insulation removed. The tip of each lead was placed just below the level of the cuticle, through a small hole made with a minuten pin. The EMG leads were secured to the cuticle with wax both at the point of insertion and at a second anchoring site. The tarsal claw leads were fixed in place, in the tarsal 2 segment, with 5-min epoxy instead of wax to avoid heat damage to the receptor nerves. All three pairs of leads were then looped mid-dorsally to near the median eyes for an additional wax anchoring before coursing to the Grass P-15 preamplifiers situated overhead. The scorpion could walk freely in a rectangular plexiglas chamber on a grounded steel plate without pulling the electrode leads taut. Occasionally, the preparation was mechanically perturbed to evoke higher rates of stepping.
Electrical recordings were filmed from an oscilloscope, sometimes after recording on a TEAC four-channel tape recorder. Most measurements from the film were accurate to within ± 5 ms. Data for scoprions walking in a straight line were punched on to cards for subsequent computer analysis. Regression lines were assessed as being significantly different from one another or from zero slope at the 95 % confidence level using Student’s t test.
RESULTS
Anatomy and physiology of tarsal receptors
Sensory receptors of the tarsal claws and the numerous tactile hairs located on the tarsal 2 segment were stained by cobalt backfilling (Fig. 1 A). Axons from these receptors join to form two sensory leg nerves which innervate the anterior and posterior aspects of the tarsal 2 segment (Fig. 1B). The tactile hairs are each innervated by a single bipolar neurone. Clusters of 5–10 smaller cells of unknown function were sometimes noticed close to a tarsal hair cell body. Each tarsal claw has 3·4 bipolar neurones located within the lumen of the claw. One cell was consistently larger than the others (Fig. 1C).
When the leg is in the stance phase of locomotion, the distal aspect of the tarsal segment, the ventral aspect of the tarsal 2 segment, and the tips 1 of the tarsal clavi are generally in contact with the substrate. Activity recorded over this period by electrodes implanted in the tarsal 2 segment, was exclusively sensory (no moto- neurones or muscles are located this far distally) and closely delineated the time period over which the leg was on the substrate. Stimulation of the tarsal claw with a mechanical probe showed that very little activity was recorded unless the tip of the claw was touched, and that this activity was recorded within 4 ms of stimulation (Fig. 2 A). There was very little activity in response to imposed or natural movements of the tarsal claw relative to the tarsal 2 segment. Although the receptors are phasic, they discharge over the entire stance phase provided the animal is moving (Fig. 2B).
EMGs
Electromyograms of the elevator muscle are characterized by several features which distinguish them from those of the depressor muscle (Fig. 2B). For the elevator, the timing of burst onset is clearcut, whereas burst termination is frequently indistinct, particularly at slower stepping rates. Such indistinct bursting has been observed in lobster (Macmillan, 1975) and crayfish (Barnes, 1977). Even though the T-F joint for leg 4 exhibits only one cycle of elevation-depression movement during each step cycle (Root & Bowerman, 1978), the elevator muscle is often activated by a burst phased with the swing phase initiation and a second burst that occurs phased with the depressor burst during the stance phase. The significance of this second burst, which occurs more often at higher rates of stepping, is unknown. A similar absence of strict reciprocity between antagonist walking leg muscles has been described in other arthropods, particularly for more distal joints (Macmillan, 1975; Barnes, 1977; Clarac & Ayers, 1977; Clarac, 1978; Burns & Usherwood, 1979). As noted in Fig. 3, elevator burst durations tend to increase in length at progressively slower rates of stepping. However, since the scattergram levels off at the longer step cycle times, the data were subdivided and treated as two separate populations.
The slopes of regression lines for data points to the left and data points to the right of a particular cycle time were compared for six different dividing values between 500 and 750 ms. The 600 ms value appeared the best choice for an inflection point for all relationships. The relationship of increasing elevator burst duration for increasing cycle times still holds below 600 ms (Fig. 3). Above 600 ms, elevator burst duration is constant on the average, i.e. statistically independent of cycle time.
Depressor muscle activity also occurs in bursts, well defined in times of onset and termination (Fig. 2B). There is seldom any depressor activity interspersed between these well defined bursts. The depressor data were also divided at 600 ms (Fig. 4). Below 600 ms, depressor burst duration increases with cycle time, and does so with a steeper slope than for the comparable elevator burst relationship. For cycle times above 600 ms, a slope of one was calculated, compared with the zero slope for the elevator burst duration over this same range (Fig. 3). Thus the extensors have less influence than the depressors on step cycle time below 600 ms, and no statistical effect above this time.
Swing-stance phase transitions
Given that the elevator and depressor muscles are reciprocally active during the swing and stance phase respectively, how does the activity of one antagonist give way to the other? For the stance to swing phase transition, the latency between depressor burst termination and elevator burst onset is plotted against depressor cycle time (Fig. 5). Two features stand out. First, there is no overlap between depressor and elevator activity. The depressor burst terminates abruptly; then about 40 ms later elevator onset occurs. Secondly, there is a statistically significant relationship such that switching latencies are increasingly greater at longer cycle times, up to 600 ms, where latency values level off.
For the swing to stance phase transition, the latency between time of elevator burst termination and depressor burst onset versus cycle time is illustrated in Fig. 6. Even though the average latency is about 12 ms, there are many instances where depressor onset precedes elevator burst termination.
Clearly, the motor programme is asymmetrical in that the stance to swing phase transition is more tightly controlled with no periods of antagonist co-activation, whereas the swing to stance phase transition is loosely coupled with brief but frequent periods of co-activation.
Timing of EMG’s relative to swing and stance phase movements
Contraction of the elevator muscle is principally responsible for initiating the swing phase. The elevator burst starts before the termination of the tarsal claw burst by an average of about 14 ms. There is a weak but significant relationship so that the latency is slightly greater, on the average, at longer cycle times. This would be expected if there were slower rates of tension development by the elevator muscle for slower rates of walking. Since there is little variation between preparations for this relationship, the specific timing of elevator muscle burst onset represents an accurate and reliable indicator of the time of swing phase initiation.
During the stance phase, both the depressor muscle and tarsal claw receptors are active. Although the depressor burst generally begins before the tarsal claw burst, there is considerable scatter in the relationship (Fig. 8). The depressor muscle may be activated from 60 ms before to 60 ms after re-establishment of leg contact with the substrate. This variation is due to variability both within and between preparations. In those instances where depressor muscle activity lags leg contact, the swing to stance phase switching must be due to the action of muscles controlling other joints and/or the influence of gravity on the leg after the elevator muscles have been turned off.
DISCUSSION
Elevator burst duration/depressor burst duration versus step cycle time
For all arthropods thus far studied, the duration of the stance phase varies more with step cycle duration than does the swing phase duration. These relationships have been described in behavioural studies for an arachnid (Bowerman, 1975), several insects (Graham, 1972; Burns, 1973; Graham, 1978) and crustaceans (Alexander, 1972; Barnes, 1975). It should not be surprising that comparable relationships are found for the depressor burst and elevator burst durations versus step cycle times. While the slope of the depressor relationship is steeper than that for the elevate relationship, neither relationship is truly linear, provided the breadth of cycle times sampled is wide enough. For example, consider the published figures for depressor burst direction versus step cycle time for the terrestrial preparations (Pearson, 1972; Fig. 6; Delcomyn & Usherwood, 1973, Fig. 6; Reingold & Camhi, 1977, Fig. 4; current paper, Fig. 4). In each case, the slope is steeper over the slower step cycles than the faster cycles. Even though there is no clear specific transition point in the scattergram, the slope transition appears to be near 300–400 ms for the cockroach and 500–600 ms for the scorpion. The basic relationship for the aquatic crustacean preparations is basically the same. The slope of the depressor burst duration versus step cycle time is steeper for the longer cycle times studied by Ayers & Davis (1977, Fig. 10) than for the shorter cycle times (Macmillan, 1975, Fig. 2A). The slope transition occurs between cycle times of 1·5 to 2·0 s. At the slower stepping rates, the slope of the elevator burst duration versus step cycle time decreases and may reach zero (Pearson, 1972, Fig. 5; Reingold & Camhi, 1977, Fig. 4; current paper, Fig. 3). Macmillan (1975) did not provide a figure, but stated that elevator burst durations were shorter cycle times. Delcomyn & Usherwood (1973) stated that there was no relationship between elevator burst and cycle time for the cockroach, but did not provide a figure. It would be interesting to see if such a relationship could be demonstrated over the shorter cycle times, since this was the case for the other two studies on cockroach P. americana preparations (Pearson, 1972, Reingold & Camhi, 1977). Clarac (1978, Fig. 3) and Ayers & Davis (1977, Fig. 10) found no relationship for elevator burst versus cycle time in their crustacean preparations. Perhaps this is because the step cycles analysed were longer than the 1·5–2·0 s transition time noted for these slow walking aquatic crustaceans.
The elevator and depressor burst duration versus cycle time relationships are not linear. Protraction time/retraction time ratios (p/r) versus cycle times (Burns, 1973, Fig. 5; Macmillan, 1975, Table 8) and elevator/depressor phase scattergrams (Reingold & Camhi, 1977, Fig. 5) should be considered in this light. For example, the phase of the elevator burst within the depressor cycle time for the scorpion (current paper) is consistent at about 0·70 for cycle times below 600 ms, but becomes progressively larger at longer step cycle times. This is because the elevator burst duration remains at a constant value, so that the increasing depressor burst length occupies a successively greater portion of the total step cycle.
There may be several reasons why the segmental oscillator does not continue to increase the swing phase duration over the slower step cycle times in a linear fashion. To do so would require the expenditure of significant energy, since the swing phase muscles lift the leg against gravitational forces. Also, the more legs supporting the animal, the more stable the situation. Hence, extra time that a leg spends in the swing phase may unnecessarily reduce the animal’s overall stability. As stepping rates increase, there comes a point past which swing phase duration must be progressively reduced in order to achieve the higher rates of stepping. The leg must be repositioned more quickly at these higher rates of stepping. The rates of tension development are presumably increased to account for the same swing phase movements over shorter periods of time. Both frequency modulation and recruitment of motoneurones have been shown to be involved with varying the rate stepping (Pearson, 1972; Burrows & Hoyle, 1973; Delcomyn & Usherwood, 1973;Ayers & Davis, 1977).
Swing-stance phase transitions
There are also striking similarities between the various arthropods studied in the transition of motoneurone activity from one muscle to that of its antagonist. In all cases, the stance to swing phase shift is accomplished by a nearly synchronous depressor burst termination and elevator burst onset. This is noted in arachnids (Fig. 5), insects (Pearson & Iles, 1970, Fig. 10; Delcomyn & Usherwood, 1973, no figure; Reingold & Camhi, 1977, Fig. 6), and crustaceans (Macmillan, 1975, Fig. 30; Ayers & Davis, 1977, Fig. 11). The relationships are relatively tight, i.e. low variation about mean values, with either no periods or only very short periods where both antagonists are active. The different preparations are also similar to one another in the swing to stance phase transition. The timing of the elevator burst termination is generally not as tightly related to the timing of depressor burst onset as for the reciprocal relationship described above (same figures except in the current paper where Fig. 6 instead of Fig. 5). There are also frequent periods of simultaneous cocontraction, with the elevator discharge continuing well into the stance phase in certain instances.
Therefore, for almost all arthropods studied, the output of the segmental oscillator is asymmetrical. There is little or no antagonist co-activation at the stance to swing phase transition and considerably more co-activation at the swing to stance phase transition. It may be that this asymmetry can be appreciated by considering the different functions of the muscles involved. The depressor muscles are active during the stance phase when the propulsive forces for locomotion are generated. They are generally larger and presumably capable of generating greater tensions than the smaller elevator muscles, which just lift the legs. If the tension in the depressor muscles during the stance phase is relatively large, there may be a premium on turning them off before or shortly after the elevator muscles are activated. This may help the elevator muscles to lift the leg as commanded. On the other hand, the tension in the elevator muscles may be relatively small and easily overcome at the end of the swing phase by contraction of the depressor muscles. Also, movements due to the elevator muscle must work against the forces of gravity, while those of the depressor muscles are aided by gravitational forces. Both factors would reduce the need for the elevators to be turned off before the depressor muscles could carry out their responsibilities.
In their models for single leg stepping control in the cockroach (Pearson, 1972; Delcomyn, 1973) proposed that the swing to stance phase transition was accomplished by simultaneous excitation of the elevator motoneurones and inhibition of the depressor motoneurones. A similar model would be consistent with the scorpion data. The only required modification would be the inclusion of a variable delay element in the elevator excitor channel to account for the relationship between switching latency and step cycle time (Fig. 5). The peripheral effect of a strong central inhibition of the depressor motoneurones would, of course, be the termination the depressor EMG burst. This would constitute the earliest peripheral indicator that the decision to step had been made centrally (Fig. 2B). The actual lifting of the leg may lag the central decision to step by over 100 ms at the slower step cycles (Fig. 9). If the segmental oscillator is viewed as a relaxation oscillator (Graham, 1977), then the most important point in the cycle is the time at which the oscillator is reset. This is the time at which the decision to step is made. The leg then completes the swing phase and is maintained in the stance phase until the next step decision is made. Therefore, depressor burst termination represents the best reference point for considering the temporal features of EMG activity for other muscles within the leg. The same is true for consideration of intersegmental stepping phase relationships, where the questions of interest concern the coupling of segmental oscillators.