Accelerative manoeuvres, such as fast-starts, are crucial for fish to avoid predation. Escape responses are fast-starts that include fundamental survival traits for prey that experience high predation pressure. However, no previous study has assessed escape performance in neonate tropical sharks. We quantitatively evaluated vulnerability traits of neonate tropical sharks by testing predictions on their fast-start escape performance. We predicted (1) high manoeuvrability, given their high flexibility, but (2) low propulsive locomotion owing to the drag costs associated with pectoral fin extension during escape responses. Further, based on previous work on dogfish, Squalus suckleyi, we predicted (3) long reaction times (as latencies longer than teleosts, >20 ms). We used two-dimensional, high-speed videography analysis of mechano-acoustically stimulated neonate blacktip reef shark, Carcharhinus melanopterus (n=12), and sicklefin lemon shark, Negaprion acutidens (n=8). Both species performed a characteristic C-start double-bend response (i.e. two body bends), but single-bend responses were only observed in N. acutidens. As predicted, neonate sharks showed high manoeuvrability with high turning rates and tight turning radii (3–11% of body length) but low propulsive performance (i.e. speed, acceleration and velocity) when compared with similar-sized teleosts and S. suckleyi. Contrary to expectations, escape latencies were <20 ms in both species, suggesting that the neurophysiological system of sharks when reacting to a predatory attack may not be limited to long response times. These results provide a quantitative assessment of survival traits in neonate tropical sharks that will be crucial for future studies that consider the vulnerability of these sharks to predation.

Escape behaviours in teleost fishes have been well studied since the 1970s (see Domenici and Hale, 2019) as they include variables that are fundamental traits for fish survival. These behavioural traits are intrinsic of predator–prey interactions, defining its outcome (Domenici, 2010; Walker et al., 2005), thus indirectly affecting fitness (i.e. a functional trait; Schmitz, 2017; Violle et al., 2007). For example, many fish species employ a fast horizontal turn, rapidly changing direction to escape a predatory attack. These accelerative responses are termed fast-starts and occur in a broad range of aquatic species (Domenici and Hale, 2019). The most common motor pattern in fast-starts is the body bending in a ‘C’ shape (termed a C-start) as a result of a unilateral muscle contraction, which corresponds to stage 1 of the escape response (Domenici and Hale, 2019). Stage 1 is usually followed by an opposite body bend, referred to as stage 2 (Domenici and Blake, 1997). Depending on whether stage 2 is present, double-bend or single-bend responses can be defined (Domenici and Blake, 1997). Hence, fast-starts include essential functional traits linked to the prey's ability to escape (i.e. ‘vulnerability’ traits; Klecka and Boukal, 2013).

From an ecological perspective, some escape components (e.g. responsiveness, Fuiman et al., 2006; manoeuvrability, Walker et al., 2005; Webb, 1976a; locomotion, as speed and acceleration, Katzir and Camhi, 1993; Walker et al., 2005; escape latency, McCormick et al., 2018) are determinants of escape success, and hence, indicators of vulnerability to predation (Domenici and Hale, 2019). For example, sharks may seek refuge in discrete nearshore habitats where predation is assumed to be reduced (i.e. shark nursery grounds; Heithaus, 2007; Heupel et al., 2007). The shallows may offer refuge by physically excluding large predators (Guttridge et al., 2012), especially in tidally influenced environments (Wetherbee et al., 2007). Yet, several studies have reported high mortality rates in neonate sharks (Gruber et al., 2001; Heupel and Simpfendorfer, 2002; Manire and Gruber, 1993), which is most likely due to direct predation (Heupel et al., 2007). This view assumes that prey are passive victims, since it does not consider their ability to evade an attack (e.g. vulnerability traits). Hence, several components of the fast-start escape response can be fundamental vulnerability traits for neonate sharks experiencing high predation pressure. However, the components of the escape responses are poorly understood in chondrichthyans (i.e. sharks, rays and chimaeras), limiting our understanding of their vulnerability to predation during early life stages within ecologically relevant habitats.

Studying vulnerability traits (e.g. escape behaviours and their components) can help overcome our limitations to understand how neonate sharks exploit nearshore habitats with high predation pressure. However, despite the extensive work on escape behaviours in teleosts, there is a lack of detailed kinematic studies on taxa that include large individuals, such as chondrichthyans (but see Domenici et al., 2004; Seamone et al., 2014). This is most likely because sharks occupy high trophic levels and are therefore often seen as predators rather than prey (Estes et al., 2016; Ferreira et al., 2017), along with the myriad experimental constraints that come with testing larger animals. For example, neonates of blacktip reef (Carcharhinus melanopterus) and sicklefin lemon (Negaprion acutidens) sharks use very shallow (<1 m), nearshore habitats likely for predator avoidance (Bouyoucos et al., 2020; George et al., 2019). In fact, in Mo'orea, French Polynesia, the terrestrial reef flats are potential nurseries for both species (Bouyoucos et al., 2022). Nonetheless, there is little evidence that indicates that neonate sharks benefit from reduced predation risk in these shallow waters (e.g. see Baker and Sheaves, 2007). These two species are likely preyed on by adult conspecifics (J.E.T., personal observations) and other larger teleosts (e.g. potentially giant travelly Caranx ignobilis; McPherson et al., 2012). Several shark species that are confined in shallow nearshore habitats during early life are even vulnerable to predation by birds (e.g. see Russo, 2015). Consequently, it is likely that fast-start escape responses allow both species to exploit nearshore habitats around Mo'orea where predation is still a threat.

Several studies have focused on turning performance in sharks (Hoffmann and Porter, 2019; Hoffmann et al., 2019; Kajiura et al., 2003; Porter et al., 2011); yet, few have examined the turning kinematics (Domenici et al., 2004; Seamone et al., 2014) and latency (Schakmann et al., 2021) of shark escape responses. Domenici et al. (2004) showed that adult Pacific spiny dogfish (Squalus suckleyi; originally reported as S. acanthias; see Ebert et al., 2010) exhibit turns comparable to manoeuvre specialists (e.g. angelfish, Pterophyllum eimikei), but are slower in speed and acceleration (Domenici and Blake, 1991). High manoeuvrability but low locomotor performance in adult S. suckleyi was attributed to aspects of their body design (Domenici et al., 2004). Further studies have revealed that S. suckleyi modify their escape responses depending on predator size, speed and approach orientation (Seamone et al., 2014), indicating high plasticity in escape ability. Schakmann et al. (2021) found long latencies, averaging 97.8 ms after mechano-acoustic stimulation, in adult S. suckleyi, much longer than most teleosts. The authors suggested this is in line with previous histological observations that indicate that Mauthner neurons (M-cells) are absent in adult elasmobranchs (Bone, 1977), which typically control escape responses in most fishes (Eaton et al., 1977; Zottoli, 1977). Although typical escape latencies in teleosts range from 5 to 150 ms (Domenici and Hale, 2019), faster reaction times often confer a competitive advantage to the prey (McCormick et al., 2018), and M-cells usually reduce this latency (Eaton et al., 2001). Work on zebrafish Danio rerio shows that, by unilaterally removing one of the two Mauthner axons, escape latency increases from 8–10 ms to around 10–20 ms (Hecker et al., 2020). Our current knowledge suggest that M-cells are absent in adult elasmobranch specimens studied to date, such as Mustelus vulgaris, Scyllium stellare, Scyliorhinus canicula, Raja punctata and Torpedo ocellata (Bone, 1977; Stefanelli, 1980). However, a transient Mauthner apparatus is present in early ontogenetic stages of Squalus acanthias (embryos) and Dalatias licha (Bone, 1977).

Work on similar-sized teleosts and elasmobranchs predicts high turning agility, low locomotor performance and long escape latencies in neonate sharks. Experiments have shown that sharks are capable of tight turning radii (<10% of body length) and high turning rates (Domenici et al., 2004; Hoffmann and Porter, 2019; Kajiura et al., 2003; Porter et al., 2011). Although turning rate may be highly variable (Domenici et al., 2004), the length–turning rate relationship in aquatic vertebrates (fish and marine mammals; Domenici, 2001) predicts turning rates of around 783 and 695 deg s−1 for the indicative average sizes observed in neonate C. melanopterus (57.3 cm total length) and N. acutidens (66.5 cm total length) in Mo'orea, respectively. The high flexibility of the anterior part of the body (Kajiura et al., 2003) facilitates such high manoeuvrability (Aleyev, 1977). Indeed, turning performance is predicted by postural reconfiguration in sharks (Porter et al., 2011). However, pectoral fins increase drag and decelerate the shark during a turn (Hoffmann and Porter, 2019; Hoffmann et al., 2019; Hoffmann et al., 2020), contrary to teleosts, which typically press the pectoral fins against their body during an escape response, with some exceptions of pectoral fin extension (Domenici and Blake, 1997; Eaton et al., 1977). This drag-based turning mechanism likely limits propulsive performance (e.g. speed and acceleration) in sharks during an escape response, as observed in S. suckleyi (Domenici et al., 2004). With respect to the timing of the response, long escape latencies (>20 ms) are predicted for sharks owing to differences that are expected in the neural control mechanisms compared with teleosts and in line with recent evidence on adult S. suckleyi (Schakmann et al., 2021).

In this study, we examined the fast-start escape responses of neonate C. melanopterus and N. acutidens to quantitatively evaluate their vulnerability traits. We used mechano-acoustic stimulation and high-speed videography analysis of the startle responses to assess both non-locomotor and locomotor performance. We predicted performance based on the following hypotheses: compared with previous work on teleosts, neonate tropical sharks will show (1) high turning rates and tight turning radii, but (2) low propulsive performance (e.g. speed, acceleration and velocity) during fast-start escape responses, and (3) long escape latencies (>20 ms). We used indicative performance ranges for each variable reported in Domenici and Hale (2019), the data reported for S. suckleyi (Domenici et al., 2004; Schakmann et al., 2021), predictions from Domenici (2001), and data reported in the literature for similar-sized teleosts (e.g. pike Esox lucius, Harper and Blake, 1990; Frith and Blake, 1991; rainbow trout Oncorhynchus mykiss, Webb, 1976a,b; Harper and Blake, 1990).

All procedures were approved under James Cook University Animal Ethics Committee protocol A2394 based on the Australian government's guidelines for the care and use of animals for scientific purposes. Shark research in French Polynesia was approved under Arrêté no. 11491 issued by the Ministrère de la Promotion des Langues, de la Culture, de la Communication et de l'Environnement of the French Government on 16 October 2019 authorizing collection, possession and transportation of sharks and their tissues. These guidelines, from both Australian and French governments, were complementary and did not conflict with each other.

Collections

Neonate blacktip reef sharks [Carcharhinus melanopterus (Quoy & Gaimard 1824), n=12] and sicklefin lemon sharks [Negaprion acutidens (Rüppell 1837), n=8] were collected between November 2019 and January 2020 around Mo'orea island (17°32′0″S, 149°50′0″W) in French Polynesia using monofilament gillnets (50×1.5 m with 5 cm mesh size) at dusk (17:00–20:00 h). Neonates were identified by their umbilical scar stage, which is used to estimate age classes (Chin et al., 2015; Weideli et al., 2019). Neonates were then transported by car in insulated coolers filled with aerated seawater to the Centre de Recherches Insulaires et Observatoire de l'Environnement (CRIOBE) (Bouyoucos et al., 2018). Total transport time was under 90 min before arriving at the CRIOBE facilities, and no injuries associated with this capture/transport method were recorded. Total length (LT) and body mass (mb) were 56.9±1.1 cm (mean±s.e.m.; range: 48.0 to 60.2 cm) and 0.98±0.02 kg in blacktip reef sharks, and 66.0±1.4 cm (range: 61.2 to 71.8 cm) and 1.31±0.07 kg in sicklefin lemon sharks, respectively. The morning following capture (day 1), sharks were marked with passive integrated transponder (PIT) tags (Biomark; www.biomark.com) inserted below the first dorsal fin for individual identification.

Husbandry

Sharks were maintained in the laboratory for 9 days in 1250 l circular tanks (∼1.5 m diameter) with flow-through filtered seawater and aeration, and covered with 60% shade cloth. Blacktip reef sharks were maintained in groups of three, and sicklefin lemon sharks were maintained in groups of two because of their larger size. The open-sided facilities provided a natural photoperiod. Feeding started at day 2 in captivity. We fed sharks every second day with fresh tuna at 3–5% of body mass (Chin et al., 2015). To control for satiation, sharks were fasted for 48 h prior to experiments starting. Water was maintained at ambient temperature (26–29°C) similar to their natural daily temperature range (see Bouyoucos et al., 2021). At days 9 and 10 in captivity, water temperature was stabilized to 29.2±0.07°C. Escape trials took place on day 10 and sharks were release back to their original site of capture after a recovery period of 2–3 days (i.e. feeding resumed immediately after experiments and sharks remained undisturbed until release), using the same transporting procedures as for the collections, within the parturition season, and during the day, when predators are expected to be less active.

Escape trail

On day 10, a single shark was randomly selected from the holding tank and transferred with a hand net to an adjacent 3.4 m diameter circular pool (referred to hereafter as the test arena, Fig. 1A; wall height ∼1.5 m) with the same blue coloration as the holding tanks. Transferer times were under 30 s. The test arena was filled with aerated, filtered seawater at the same temperature as the holding tank (29.2±0.07°C). To restrict movement in the vertical plane, water was maintained at a depth of 16 cm, which just allowed the whole body of the shark to be submerged (see Fig. 1D for caudal fin height and body depth reference). Before the escape trial, we allowed the shark to acclimate to the novel environment (the test arena) for 2 h. We determined this acclimation period based on ethograms obtained during pilot tests over a 3-h observation period (18 h of observation in total). We analysed average swimming speed and space use to control for variability in their activity level (the pre-startle state). See Fig. S1 for details on analysis and Fig. S2 for results. We also recorded spontaneous turns after acclimation, but before stimulation, to validate escape responses against un-stimulated turns (see Domenici et al., 2004).

We used a sudden (mechano-acoustic) stimulus to elicit a startle response because it has been shown to elicit stronger responses compared with other types of stimuli (Domenici and Hale, 2019). The stimulus consisted of a tapered steel weight (560 g) that was released by an electromagnet from 1.58 m above the test arena onto the surface of the water through a PVC pipe to avoid a premature response (Allan et al., 2014; Dadda et al., 2010). A mirror attached to the end of the pipe at 45 deg allowed visualization of the stimulus onset (Fig. 1A). Four reference lengths (20 cm long) were placed on the bottom of the test arena, around the impact point (i.e. at the same level of the shark). These were used for video calibration and to delimit the target zone. The target zone was 0.6 m in diameter and its edge was 1.4 m away from the wall (Fig. 1B). Sharks were only startled when they were within the target zone. Stimulus angle (SA) was 74.26±4.51 deg (range: 15.73–130.99 deg) and stimulus distance was 22.08±1.39 cm to the COM and 24.62±1.56 cm to the tip of the head. On no occasion did the stimulus touch the sharks or the bottom of the test arena. Each shark was stimulated at least three times and allowed to re-habituate for 20 min between each stimulation and its pre-startle state re-assessed. See timeline in Fig. S1 and results in Fig. S2. Each response was recorded at high speed (240 frames s−1) using a GoPro Hero7 Black camera with wide field of view (see section below for details on lens distortion correction). Three sharks were tested per day. To control for a potential temporal signal, trials took place during three different periods of the day, such that one individual was tested during the morning (9:00–11:00 h), one at midday (12:00–14:00 h) and one in the afternoon (15:00–18:00 h).

Lens distortion correction and parallax effects

We used a GoPro Hero7 Black camera with a wide field of view (FOV) and the following video parameters: (1) equivalent focal length: 15–30 mm, (2) aspect ratio: 4:3, (3) pixel resolution: 1280×960, (4) vertical FOV: 94.4 deg, (5) horizontal FOV: 122.6 deg and (6) diagonal FOV: 149.2 deg. Prior to analysis, we used Adobe Premier Pro v22.2.0 to correct for wide-angle lens distortion. The lens distortion algorithm calibrated for GoPro cameras applied a –31 curvature correction to each video. This parameter is already established by the software as it is already calibrated for the camera model/lens type. For this reason, calibration parameters (e.g. k1, k2, k3, k4) cannot be accessed. The curvature of straight lines in the distorted image was rectified in the resulting undistorted image. This correction reduces the magnification effect of the centre of the image by reducing radial distortion. Tangential distortion (or de-centring distortion) was negligible. Parallax effects, in which a moving object appears to move a shorter or longer distance than it actually does, were also negligible because (1) we restricted the vertical movement of the sharks (see above), (2) we placed the reference lengths at the same level of the sharks, and (3) our camera was at the same angle and distance with respect to the bottom of the test arena throughout the trials. Finally, we exported corrected image sequences from the undistorted videos at 240 frames s−1 for escape trials and 120 frames s−1 for spontaneous turns.

Video analysis

Two body landmarks were predefined for manual tracking purposes: the tip of the head and the origin of the first dorsal fin (see Fig. 1C). The second landmark was representative of the centre of mass (COM), based on the assumption that pre-first dorsal fin length, which is approximately 32% of LT in C. melanopterus for 427–525 mm LT individuals (Garrick, 1982), is similar to the calculated position of the COM in previous studies (e.g. 33% of LT in Squalus suckleyi, Domenici et al., 2004; ∼35% of LT in Salmo gairdneri, Webb, 1976a; 38% of LT in Clupea harengus, Domenici and Batty, 1997).

We used the manual tracking plugin in ImageJ v2.0.0 for a two-dimensional video analysis frame by frame using the corrected image sequences. The corrected image resolution (1280×960 pixels) yielded ∼2.7 pixels cm−1. This resolution was enough for accurate visual analysis of zoomed frames when necessary (e.g. for estimation of latency). Using the reference lengths, a value of ∼0.3703 was applied for x-axis (forward displacement) and y-axis (lateral displacement) calibration. Calibration of the z-axis (vertical displacement) was not necessary, as water depth was sufficiently shallow to minimize shark movements in this plane. We digitized the x,y coordinates (tracking points) of the predetermined body landmarks. Distance between tracking points and instantaneous speed (between two successive frames) were obtained from the tracking software. For the escape responses, tracking was initiated one frame before the first detectable movement of the tip of the head and continued for 100 frames (i.e. 416 ms). For spontaneous turns, tracking started one frame before the first detectable movement of the head and finished with the end of the turn. Tracking coordinates were then imported to the R environment for further analysis (see below).

Responsiveness, directionality and escape latency

Responsiveness was measured as the proportion of escape responses out of the total number of stimulations for each shark. Directionality was defined as the proportion of responses in which the body bent in a direction away from the stimulus (i.e. ‘away responses’ as opposed to ‘towards responses’) out of the total number of responses, and did not necessarily indicate the final escape trajectory (Domenici et al., 2011). Escape latency was measured as the time between stimulation and the onset of the escape response (as the first detectable movement of the head) in milliseconds for all successful escape responses in which the weight was visible in the mirror (see Fig. 1A). We then calculated the mean and minimum latencies for each individual (Schakmann et al., 2021).

Durations and stages

Stage 1 (S1) and stage 2 (S2) were defined by the turning of the anterior segment of the body (tip of head to COM; Domenici et al., 2004; Kasapi et al., 1993). These are body bends in opposite directions. The time taken to form each body bend corresponded to stage 1 and stage 2 durations (TS1 and TS2), respectively. To define TS1 and TS2, we used the smoothed turning rate versus time curve (see below) and applied the function uniroot.all from the package stats (https://www.r-project.org/) to find the time points defining the stages. The first time point (T0), at the start of the tracking, was one frame before the first detectable movement of the head. Stage 1 started at T0 and finished with a reversal of turn direction, at T1, indicated by the smoothed turning rate curve crossing 0 deg s−1. Stage 2 started at T1 and finished with a stop or reversal of turning direction, at T2; that is, when the turning rate curve crossed 0 deg s−1 again. Total escape response duration (TEscape) was the sum of TS1 and TS2. The mean duration of all responses recorded, by species, was defined as TMean. Although variable, continuous swimming or coasting have been defined as the final stage (S3) in some fish species (Rand and Lauder, 1981; Webb and Skadsen, 1980; Weihs, 1973). Here, stage 3 started at T2, and although the end was not defined, we used the tracking points until frame 100 as an arbitrary time frame for stage 3 analysis (see below). Turning rate data paired with video inspections were used for stage 3 analysis.

Turning kinematics: angles, turning radius and turning rate

Angles were measured using the angle tool in ImageJ as the relative positions of the line projecting from the tip of the head to the COM between the start and end of each stage (stage 1 angle: θS1; stage 2 angle: θS2). The rotation of this line was measured in degrees. Because stage 2 corresponds to a body bend opposite to that of stage 1, θS2 bears a negative sign.

Turning rate was defined as the angular velocity of the anterior segment of the body (tip of head to COM) in deg s−1. The maximum turning rate for each stage was extracted from smoothed turning rate data (ωS1 and ωS2, respectively; see ‘Data processing and statistical analyses’ for details on smoothing methods). Mean turning rate was obtained as the ratio between angle and duration for stage 1 (ωMean; Domenici et al., 2004).

Turning radius (RTurn) was calculated using the formula in Domenici and Blake (1991):
(1)

where is the mean instantaneous distance moved (see below) and is the mean instantaneous angle of turn of the COM during stage 1. The result was divided by LT, as it has been demonstrated to be proportional to body length (Howland, 1974; Webb, 1976a).

We also measured turning angle, duration and mean turning rate using image sequences (at 120 frames s−1) of spontaneous turns during routine swimming for unstimulated individuals for both species.

Distance–time variables: distance, displacement, velocity, speed and acceleration

We measured two different distance variables: distance travelled and displacement. We used a fixed time (TMean) to measure these variables to avoid any performance bias owing to differences in escape duration (Domenici and Blake, 1991; Domenici et al., 2004; Webb, 1976b). The cumulative distance travelled (DTMean) was calculated as the length of the path of the COM in metres from T0 until TMean. Displacement (DMean; m) was measured as a straight line between the positions of the COM at T0 and at TMean (see Fig. 1C). Velocity (ν; m s−1) was obtained by dividing DMean by TMean. Average speed (UAvg, m s−1), maximum speed (UMax; m s−1) and maximum acceleration (αMax; m s−2) were calculated from the smoothed distance–time data obtained from ImageJ during the escape response (i.e. TEscape; see ‘Data processing and statistical analyses’ for details on smoothing methods). Velocity differed from speed (UAvg and UMax), as the former was based on how fast the shark displaced its body from point A (one frame before stimulation) to point B (TMean), and the latter was derived from the path of the COM throughout the escape sequence (TEscape).

Data processing and statistical analyses

All data processing and statistical analyses were performed in the R statistical environment (https://www.r-project.org/). For the analysis of turning rate, speed and acceleration, we used smoothed curves for each of these variables. We applied a locally estimated scatterplot smoothing (LOESS) to the raw data using the loess function. The degree of the polynomial (1 or 2) and the size of the neighbourhood (α) were optimized using a 5-fold cross-validation procedure specific to each escape response using the kfold function from the dismo package (https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=dismo). The root mean square error (RMSE) was obtained from the cross-validation results for each model combination with the function rmse from the package hydroGOF (https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=hydroGOF) such that the fit with the minimum RMSE was used for smoothing.

Because each individual was stimulated at least three times, we checked differences between stimulations using a one-way ANOVA using complete cases from blacktip reef shark data. We found no significant differences for turning radius (F2,28=0.56, P=0.60), TS1 (F2,29=0.84, P=0.44), TS2 (F2,24=0.17, P=0.84), TEscape (F2,24=1.23, P=0.31), ωS1 (F2,29=1.24, P=0.30) or ωS2 (F2,25=0.35, P=0.71) between stimulations in neonate blacktip reef sharks. This indicated no habituation effect or muscle fatigue over the consecutive stimulations (Marras et al., 2011). Hence, all responses were used in the results. The smaller sample size and unbalanced structure of the data for sicklefin lemons sharks precluded a similar analysis for this species. However, our results for blacktip reef sharks and results from other studies (i.e. Marras et al., 2011) were sufficient evidence to also pool all the data for sicklefin lemon sharks.

We pooled all data for summary results but used linear mixed-effects models (LMM) for all regressions to account for the non-independence owing to the repeated measures in the data. This approach allowed us to also account for the unbalance structure of the data (i.e. not all sharks had three startles). We used the lme function from the nlme package (https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme) and included the stimulus number as a random effect (three levels as we performed three stimulations for each individual). To investigate the relationship between turning performance and locomotor performance, we built LMMs with velocity (m s−1) as the dependent variable with either θS1S2, ωS1 or ωS2 as the explanatory (fixed effect) variable. Stimulus number was included as a random effect. Model validating was done via visual inspection of residuals following Zuur et al. (2009).

Individuals were deemed acclimated to the test arena after 2 h, as evidenced by undisturbed swimming. Sharks were startled three times, 20 min after each stimulation. Normal swimming activity resumed within the 20 min (see results in Fig. S2). On average, blacktip reef sharks were swimming at 0.36±0.01 m s−1 and sicklefin lemon sharks at 0.46±0.03 m s−1 before being startled. Average swimming speed was not significantly different between pre-startle states in blacktip reef sharks (one-way ANOVA: F1,30=3.9, P=0.06) or in sicklefin lemon sharks (one-way ANOVA: F1,12=2.8, P=0.12). All individuals resumed similar space use following a startle, and space use was not significantly different between pre-startle states in either species (see one-way ANOVA results in Fig. S2). Blacktip reef sharks spent 85.7±2.7% of the time in open spaces, whereas sicklefin lemon sharks divided their time almost equally between areas (percentage time in open space: 50.2±3.6%). This difference in space use was statistically significant between species (one-way ANOVA: F1,43=57.6, P<0.05).

We were able to successfully video record 32 responses from 12 blacktip reef sharks and 20 responses from seven sicklefin lemons sharks. Both species responded to the mechano-acoustic stimulus with a fast unilateral contraction bending the body to one side, followed by a body bend in the opposite direction (see escape sequences in Fig. 2A,B). However, two responses recorded in sicklefin lemon sharks were not fast-starts and were removed from analysis. Responsiveness was 100% for blacktip reef sharks (32 out of 32 responses) and 64.6% for sicklefin lemon sharks (11 out of 18 responses). This difference in responsiveness between the two species was statistically significant (one-way ANOVA: F1,18=7.98, P<0.05). Only one escape response, and only in blacktip reef sharks, was towards the weight, such that similar directionality (97% and 100%) was observed in blacktip reef sharks and sicklefin lemon sharks, respectively.

The first whole-body bend had a characteristic C-shape (i.e. stage 1, Domenici and Blake, 1997). The second, contra-lateral body bend, clearly defined from changes in the direction of the head, constitute stage 2 (sensuDomenici and Blake, 1997). These double-bend escape responses are illustrated in the turning rate data (Fig. 3A–C) with the first positive peak representing the formation of the C-shape and a subsequent depression with negative values representing the opposite body bend. All escape responses in blacktip reef sharks were double-bends (n=32 escape response). In sicklefin lemon sharks, the turning rate data of 11 escape responses showed that eight responses were double-bends and three of them were single-bends (Fig. 2C and Fig. 3D). We have therefore described double-bends separately from single-bend responses in sicklefin lemon sharks.

Escape latency

Escape latency ranged from 8.33 to 75.0 ms (n=30 responses; Fig. 4A,B) in blacktip reef sharks and from 4.17 to 37.5 ms (n=9 responses; Fig. 4A,B) in sicklefin lemon sharks. Latency values were less than 20 ms in 40% of the responses in blacktip reef sharks and in 55% of the responses in sicklefin lemon sharks. The three single-bend responses in sicklefin lemon sharks had latencies of 8.33, 20.8 and 37.5 ms (all different individuals). We observed a peak in frequency that was below the 20 ms mark in both species, that is at 13 ms in blacktip reef sharks and at 9 ms in sicklefin lemon sharks (Fig. 4B). Mean latency – the mean of all successful trials for a single individual – was 27.9±4.96 ms in blacktip reef sharks (n=12 individuals) and 15.9±3.18 ms in sicklefin lemon sharks (n=6 individuals; Fig. 4C). On average, minimum latency – the shortest latency an individual achieved across all its successful trials – was 19.4±3.74 ms in blacktip reef sharks (n=12 individuals) and 11.8±2.93 ms in sicklefin lemon sharks (n=6 individuals). We found no significant differences in mean (one-way ANOVA: F1,16=3.2, P=0.093) or minimum latencies (one-way ANOVA: F1,16=2.34, P=0.146) between blacktip reef sharks and sicklefin lemon sharks (Fig. 4C).

Double-bend response kinematics

During their first body bend (stage 1), both species showed fast angular velocities (i.e. ωS1 and ωMean; Table 1). Angular velocities observed during stage 2 (ωS2) in blacktip reef sharks had a much broader range than in sicklefin lemon sharks (Fig. 5). It is important to note, however, that the sample size for the sicklefin lemon sharks was smaller than that for the blacktip reef sharks. Both species had a wide range of stage 1 turning angles (Fig. 2F,G), averaging around 90 deg (Table 1) but the maximum observed were relatively large (Table S1). Angles of turn during the second body bend (θS2) were smaller (Table 1) and with a narrower range (Fig. 2F,G). The first body bend had a tight turning radius, the smallest reported (that is from a single event) was 0.01L in both species. The frequency distribution of turning radius was skewed toward values that were lower than the mean in both species (Fig. S3). During spontaneous turns, both species turned with slower angular velocities (ωMean) at smaller turning angles (θS1) than escape responses (Tables S1 and S2).

All durations (TEscape, TS1 and TS2) are summarized in Table 1, and frequency distributions are shown in Fig. S3. The escape response duration (TEscape) was comparable to that of S. suckleyi but longer compared with similar-sized teleost species (Table S1). Longer durations were observed during spontaneous turns in both species (Tables S1 and S2). There was no significant effect of LT on TS1 (LMM: F1,30=0.273, P=0.605, R2=−0.02) or TEscape (LMM: F1,30=0.001, P=0.9734, R2=−0.03) in blacktip reef sharks. Similarly, LT did not affect TS1 (LMM: F1,6=0.33, P=0.587, R2=−0.106) or TEscape (LMM: F(1,6)=0.56, P=0.482, R2=−0.067) in sicklefin lemon sharks. TS1 was linearly correlated with the angle of turn during stage 1 (θS1) in both species (Fig. 6A,B and Table S4). Double-bend escape responses occupied a different section of the graph in Fig. 6A compared with spontaneous turns, for both species.

The COM reached maximum speeds right at the end or after stage 2 (e.g. Fig. 8). Then, the COM exhibited a slight decrease in speed before 416.66 ms (i.e. end of the tracking sequence). Both species reached similar speeds during the escape response (Table 1), that were comparable to those of similar-sized teleosts, but higher than that of S. suckleyi (Table S1). UMax did not correlate with ωS1 (LMM: F1,25=3.16, P=0.09, R2=0.07) or ωS2 (LMM: F1,25=0.015, P=0.90, R2=−0.04) in blacktip reef sharks. Unfortunately, the lower sample size in sicklefin lemon sharks precluded the possibility to draw a reliable correlation. Peak acceleration occurred at the beginning of the first stage, and it was similar in both species (see Table 1) but decreased during the escape response (not shown).

In a straight line, both species displaced away from their initial position by a similar distance (Table 1). The cumulative distance travelled by the path of the COM (at the mean escape response duration) was comparable in both species (Table 1; see Fig. 2D for an example of the path of the COM). In line with the above, both species displaced with a similar velocity (ν; Table 1). In blacktip reef sharks, ν was negatively affected by the θS1. When the angle of the turn during stage 1 was large, sharks displaced more slowly (Fig. 7A). In contrast to θS1, θS2 was positively correlated with velocity (Fig. 7B). Note that these correlations were built using velocity measured at TMean, such that there is no effect of the longer duration of turns with larger angles.

Single-bend response kinematics

Single-bend responses were only observed in three sicklefin lemon shark individuals and not in blacktip reef sharks. In all cases, stage 1 was followed by coasting and no stage 2 was visible in the turning rate data (Fig. 3D). Single-bend kinematics are summarized in Table 1. We were unable to obtain a reliable correlation between TS1 and θS1 owing to the lower number of single-bends (n=3). However, all single-bend responses occupied an area in the graph closer to that of double-bend escape responses and not to routine turns (Fig. 6). Therefore, these three responses were clearly of a higher intensity than routine turns, but less intense than double-bends. For example, UMax was not much higher than UAvg in single-bend responses (Fig. 8E and Table 1) and the fastest of all single-bend responses reached a UMax of only 1.70 m s−1 (62 cm LT individual). Displacement and velocity were calculated at the mean TS1 (Table 1). Velocity was 2 and 1.6 times slower than blacktip reef and sicklefin lemon sharks' double-bends, respectively. Unfortunately, the low number of single-bends (n=3) precluded the possibility to statistically test for differences between single-bends and double-bends in sicklefin lemon sharks. See Table S3 for data on all escape responses.

Stage 3

We observed a consistent pattern in the motor characteristics that followed stage 2 in double-bend responses. This pattern (defined as stage 3) was observed and analysed in 31 and 5 responses in blacktip reef and sicklefin lemon sharks, respectively. In the remaining responses, we were unable to determine whether stage 3 was present as the sharks were in a blind spot for the camera during this section of the tracking. One or two tail beats were followed by coasting, in which the tail aligned with the midline during stage 3. This pattern was defined in the turning rate data by a positive signal followed by a negative-going signal of similar intensity (Fig. 3A–C). The start of stage 3 was marked by the first positive signal at the end of stage 2 (Fig. 3A–C). The end of stage 3 was undefined. However, tail beats were always completed, so that the tail aligned with the midline (and coasting followed), before 416.66 ms (i.e. before 100 frames), regardless of whether there were one or two tail beats. Notably, speed did not change, as no peaks were observed in the speed curve at this stage, regardless of whether these tail beats were present.

Our results allowed us to quantitatively assess the escape performance of neonate sharks through several components (vulnerability traits) of their escape behaviour. We used neonates of two highly mobile tropical reef shark species and successfully obtained escape kinematic data under laboratory conditions. Both species engaged in fast-start escape responses typical of those observed previously in teleosts and in adult S. suckleyi (Domenici et al., 2004). Specifically, and for the first time in neonate tropical reef sharks, we describe C-start single-bend and double-bend escape responses. Our results showed that neonates of both species of tropical shark had higher than predicted turning rates (1671–1844 deg s−1), and tight turning radii (3–11% of body length). This is in line with a high turning manoeuvrability (hypothesis 1; see Introduction). We also showed that speeds (4.1–5.2 BL s−1) and accelerations (34.3–36.5 m s−2) were at the lower end for both species, when compared with published data in similar size species of teleosts, in line with hypothesis 2. Contrary to our expectations for shark reaction times (>20 ms, hypothesis 3), we reported short escape latencies (<20 ms). Finally, we emphasize the relevance of a third stage (stage 3), rarely described before (Weihs, 1973), to characterize the escape ability in sharks (i.e. mainly for propulsive performance) beyond the commonly studied kinematic stages in escape responses. Our results are relevant to understand how neonate sharks may exploit nearshore habitats where predation pressure is high.

High turning performance, based upon high turning rates, tight turning radius and large maximum turning angles, was observed in neonates of both species of sharks. Both species also exceeded theoretical expectations (783 and 695 deg s−1 expected values for 57.3 and 66.5 cm long individuals, respectively; versus 1148 and 972 deg s−1 observed in blacktip and sicklefin lemon sharks, respectively) based on the length-turning rate relationship in aquatic vertebrates (fish and marine mammals; Domenici, 2001). In contrast to previous work on S. suckleyi and similar-sized teleosts, our study demonstrated that the turning rate (ωMean) for double-bend responses was 2.43 (blacktip reef shark) and 2.06 (sicklefin lemon shark) times higher than that of fast responses reported for adult S. suckleyi of a similar length (Domenici et al., 2004). This difference may be due to the difference in ambient temperature, which differed greatly between this study (29°C) and Domenici et al. (2004) (12°C). Indeed, other traits associated with turning performance, namely muscular contraction time (Wakeling, 2005), flexibility (Domenici, 2001) and postural reconfiguration (Porter et al., 2011), are likely to be affected by temperature. Unfortunately, we were not able to measure these traits in either species. Large maximum turning angles are also a characteristic of flexible species (Domenici, 2001) and typically range from 0 to 180 deg (Domenici and Hale, 2019). Neonate tropical sharks reached turning angles as large as 178.9 deg (blacktip reef shark double-bends), 138.28 deg (sicklefin lemon shark double-bends) and 190.7 deg (sicklefin lemon shark single-bends). This ability to turn with large angles, combined with a large range of escape angles, is a potential advantage as this will allow for a wide range of escape trajectories, independent of the angle of the approaching predator (Domenici and Hale, 2019; Domenici et al., 2011).

Another characteristic of high manoeuvrability was the tight turning radius observed in both species (3–11% of body length), equivalent to turning specialists (∼6%; Domenici and Hale, 2019), and in contrast to the larger turning radius observed in cruising specialists with rigid bodies such as yellowfin tuna, Thunnus albacares (∼40%; Blake et al., 1995). Tight turning radii can be used by prey animals for their advantage (Weihs and Webb, 1984) and can be a key parameter in determining the outcome of predator–prey interactions (Webb, 1976a). We observed asynchronous movements of pectoral fins in both species studied here, like in S. suckleyi (Domenici et al., 2004) and in unstimulated juvenile bonnethead sharks, Sphyrna tiburo (Kajiura et al., 2003). Therefore, it is possible that C. melanopterus and N. acutidens use pectoral fin extension to reduce turning radius as suggested by Domenici and Blake (1997). Pectoral fin activation seems to occur even a few milliseconds before any detectable movement of the head in C. melanopterus and N. acutidens (J.E.T., personal observations). Pectoral fins are likely an important control surface for shark escape responses. Indeed, Kajiura et al. (2003) found that S. tiburo reduced turning radius using the pectoral fin inside the body curvature as a pivot point, unlike juvenile sandbar Carcharhinus plumbeus and scalloped hammerhead Sphyrna lewini. This high manoeuvrability will be advantageous in structurally complex environments, such as coral reefs. Further, high manoeuvrability represents a competitive advantage for neonate sharks over larger predators (e.g. potentially larger individuals of the same species or closely related species; J.E.T., personal observations), because turning performance decreases with body size (Wakeling, 2005). Taken together, these results suggest that neonate tropical reef sharks can outmanoeuvre their larger predators, as they are capable of rapid changes in direction, a wide range of escape trajectories, and tight turns in narrow spaces.

It is also possible that the extent to which certain mechanisms that increase turning performance used by the sharks (e.g. pectoral fins acting as rudders) may cause a relatively low locomotor performance owing to the considerable drag costs associated (Domenici and Blake, 1997). In fact, other species of shark were observed to actively rotate pectoral fins, which increases drag during a turn, but this causes a deceleration (Hoffmann and Porter, 2019; Hoffmann et al., 2019; Hoffmann et al., 2020). When compared with previous work on teleost species, propulsive performance in tropical neonate sharks appears to be relatively low compared with most teleosts of a similar size. Although speed and acceleration were higher in C. melanopterus and N. acutidens compared with S. suckleyi, such differences may be attributable to difference in temperature (29°C compared with 12°C for S. suckleyi). However, speed and acceleration in neonate sharks was in the lower end of the indicative range for these parameters in teleosts (see Domenici and Hale, 2019), despite the considerable higher temperature in this study compared with those used in previous studies on teleosts. Interestingly, most teleosts press their pectoral fins against the body during escape responses (with some exceptions; Domenici and Blake, 1997) and some ‘accelerator’ specialists (e.g. Esox lucius) can achieve both high manoeuvrability and acceleration. Hence, future studies may need to focus on addressing the root causes for the low propulsive performance observed so far in the escape responses of sharks when compared with teleosts.

The high manoeuvrability during stage 1 may lead to a reduction in velocity in tropical neonate sharks as in many other teleosts (Domenici and Blake, 1991; Domenici and Blake, 1993). This trade-off is not restricted to sharks or teleosts (e.g. see Wynn et al., 2015) and can in part be compensated by stage 2 kinematics in tropical neonate sharks. Stage 2 corresponded to the main propulsive phase in neonate sharks as in teleosts (Domenici and Hale, 2019). A large turning angle and high turning rate during stage 2 was associated with an increase in velocity. Therefore, although an increase in the rate of body bending during stage 1 can cause a decrease in muscle force and power production (Wakeling et al., 1999), the tail sweep after stage 2 may be an important source of thrust in tropical neonate sharks. In fact, thrust can increase with postural curvature (Turesson et al., 2009). Experiments on escape success paired with fast-start kinematics would be needed to investigate the effect of different escape kinematics on vulnerability to predation. Another method of partial compensation for low locomotor performance may result from the power stroke of stage 3 tail beats. Because sharks are unable to generate forward thrust by paddling their pectoral fins (Wilga and Lauder, 2000, 2001, 2004), propulsion is mainly the result of the rearward propagating wave of the body curvature. Therefore, extra thrust generated by the tail beats observed during stage 3 can generate an increase in speed but may incur higher energetic costs. Undulatory motions (e.g. both from stage 2 and 3) seem to be an important source of thrust for neonate tropical sharks escape responses. Hence, it would be relevant to investigate the role of undulatory reconfiguration (Long et al., 2010) in escape success in sharks.

Escape responses are energetically expensive and, therefore, overall escape performance must meet a compromise with energy expenditure. For example, escapes lacking stage 2 (i.e. single-bend responses) may be less energetically costly to undertake than double-bend responses (Domenici and Hale, 2019). In fact, under challenging environmental conditions, such as hypoxia (Domenici et al., 2007) or increased temperature (Allan et al., 2015), or during physiological challenges such as starvation (Yan et al., 2015), single-bend responses are common and more frequent (Lefrançois et al., 2005). We found a broad range of ωS2 values in both species, and single-bends (i.e. stage 2 absent) were reported in three cases in sicklefin lemon sharks. This plasticity in stage 2 might confer an energetic advantage to the prey when responding to a predatory threat under different contexts. For example, when the danger is not imminent, a low-energy response may be sufficient to avoid predation.

Work on teleosts indicates that the use of a two-gear, fast or slow, response system may be another way of energy saving depending on the strength of the perceived threat (Domenici and Hale, 2019). Slow or fast escape responses depend on the stage 1 turning rates (ωS1 or ωMean), which are driven by different muscle contraction speeds and/or neural pathways (Domenici and Batty, 1994; Nissanov et al., 1990). Domenici et al. (2004) found that S. suckleyi displayed both slow and fast escape responses, based on bimodal distributions of ωS1 and on ωMean and significantly different slopes found for the correlation between θS1 and TS1 for fast and slow responses. This indicates that sharks can also take advantage of such a fast-or-slow system. In our study, ωS1 did not show a clear bimodal distribution. The kernel density smooth applied to the distribution of ωS1 did not show signs of multimodality, not even when single-bends were included in the data for sicklefin lemon sharks (Fig. S4). The correlation between θS1 and TS1 showed a single slope with no distinct groups deviating from the rest of the data. Experimentally, the occurrence of fast or slow responses may be determined by the distance or speed of the mechanical stimuli (Bhattacharyya et al., 2017; Domenici and Batty, 1997). These two factors were maintained within a small range in our study (see Materials and Methods). Therefore, our results suggests that tropical neonate sharks did not respond with a clear fast-or-slow system, at least under the experimental conditions we used. Notably, because the faster turns observed were approximately twice as fast as the slowest turns, as found in the fast versus slow escapes of S. suckleyi (Domenici et al., 2004), and because of the low number of single-bends, we cannot discard the possibility of a fast-or-slow system in tropical neonate sharks. Slow responses would be advantageous when responding to low-risk threats in order to minimize the energetic costs associated with escape responses.

The level of threat is likely very high in early stages of life but may vary among shark species. Although both species studied here showed no differences in escape latency (and were very low), the lower responsiveness in sicklefin lemon sharks (64.6%), compared with blacktip reef sharks (100%), suggests that the level of perceived threat varies between them. In fact, behavioral trials showed that neonate sicklefin lemon sharks and grey reef sharks (Carcharhinus amblyrinchos) dominated blacktip reef sharks (O. C. Weideli, personal communication), indicating that the level of threat is much higher in blacktip reef sharks than in sicklefin lemon sharks. Furthermore, there are reports of giant trevally (Caranx ignobilis; ∼120 cm LT) ramming and mortally injuring adult blacktip reef sharks (∼200 cm LT; McPherson et al., 2012). Thus, especially during early life stages, blacktip reef sharks may experience a higher frequency of predator attacks and therefore may be more prone to engage in a high-energy response than the more dominant sicklefin lemon sharks. It is possible that, at this stage of life, blacktip reef sharks relay in an all-or-nothing strategy, which would explain why we did not observe a clear fast-or-slow system as in adult S. suckleyi.

Short-latency responses may signify a competitive advantage to small neonate sharks that likely rely on rapidly moving away from an approaching, larger predator. Neonate sharks were fast to react to the stimulus; their escape latency was shorter than previous observations on adult sharks (67 ms; Schakmann et al., 2021) and similar to those of many teleosts (Domenici and Hale, 2019). Many possible reasons could explain such short latencies when compared with previous work on elasmobranchs, and similar to latencies found in teleosts with M-cells (Domenici and Hale, 2019). One reason may be the relatively high temperature, as it is known that temperature affects latencies (Domenici et al., 2019), but other reasons may include species-specific differences and methods. Our methods, however, were similar to those of many previous studies that have tested for escape latency in teleosts. Given the importance of M-cells in minimizing escape latency in teleosts (Hecker et al., 2020), we cannot dismiss that neonate individuals of these tropical shark species may also possess M-cells. In fact, early ontogenetic stages of S. acanthias (embryos) and Dalatias licha show a transient Mauthner apparatus (Bone, 1977). Future anatomical studies would be necessary to test this hypothesis. An alternative explanation is that sharks use a different neural pathway to generate short-latency responses. Short-latency responses in teleosts can also be activated without M-cells (i.e. by other startle neurons, namely MiD2cm and MiD3cm; Liu and Fetcho, 1999). Regardless of whether M-cells are involved, a system that facilitates short latencies is fundamental to allow a small prey to rapidly change its direction, moving perpendicular to an approaching predator (Abrahams, 2006). Therefore, future studies that couple behavioural evidence with neural control in neonate sharks would be useful in order to understand the functional link between neural commands and escape performance in sharks.

This study improves our knowledge on escape behaviours by reporting, for the first time, detailed escape kinematics for neonate and tropical sharks. Laboratory studies impose logistical limitations on studying large taxa. Here, we successfully applied a common methodology to two shark species to understand behaviours that are important for fitness. We highlighted the main components of the escape response, such as high turning agility and fast reaction times, and their potential contribution toward survival. It is important to emphasize that future studies on the neuromuscular control of escape responses in sharks will be of particular value. Understanding how neonate sharks exploit coastal habitats where predation pressure is high, and how future environmental change will affect escape ability in sharks, is important. For instance, rising sea surface temperatures owing to anthropogenic climate change (Bindoff et al., 2019) are likely to alter predator–prey dynamics (Allan et al., 2015; Allan et al., 2017; Domenici et al., 2019). Hence, understanding escape behaviour and its physiological and morphological constraints in neonate sharks is essential for assessing how global change will shape future fish communities (Wheeler et al., 2020).

The authors would like to thank the team of The Physioshark Project and the staff at CRIOBE for their support of experiments and in the field; and Kim Eustache, Alexandra Schoen and Julien Gasc for assistance in catching and transporting sharks. We thank the two anonymous reviewers for constructive feedback that greatly improved the quality of this manuscript.

Author contributions

Conceptualization: J.E.T., W.J.R., J.L.R., B.J.A.; Methodology: J.E.T., J.L.R., B.J.A.; Validation: P.D.; Formal analysis: J.E.T.; Investigation: J.E.T., I.B.; Resources: S.P., J.L.R.; Writing - original draft: J.E.T.; Writing - review & editing: J.E.T., I.B., W.J.R., P.D., S.P., J.L.R., B.J.A.; Supervision: J.L.R., B.J.A.; Funding acquisition: J.E.T., J.L.R., B.J.A.

Funding

J.E.T. was supported by a University of Otago Doctoral Scholarship and a Journal of Experimental Biology Travelling Fellowship from The Company of Biologists. J.L.R. was supported by the Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence for Coral Reef Studies. Centre de Recherches Insulaires et Observatoire de l'Environnement personnel contributed in providing local facilities and equipment to ensure feasibility of the experiments. Open access funding provided by University of Otago. Deposited in PMC for immediate release.

Abrahams
,
M
. (
2006
).
The physiology of antipredator behaviour: what you do with what you've got
. In
Behaviour and Physiology of Fishes
(ed.
K. A.
Sloman
,
R. W.
Wilson
and
S.
Balshine
), pp.
79
-
102
.
Academic Press
.
Aleyev
,
Y. G.
(
1977
).
Neckton
, 1st edn (ed.
W.
Junk
).
Dordrecht: Springer
.
Allan
,
B. J. M.
,
Miller
,
G. M.
,
McCormick
,
M. I.
,
Domenici
,
P.
and
Munday
,
P. L.
(
2014
).
Parental effects improve escape performance of juvenile reef fish in a high-CO2 world
.
Proc. R. Soc. B
281
,
20132179
.
Allan
,
B. J. M.
,
Domenici
,
P.
,
Munday
,
P. L.
and
McCormick
,
M. I.
(
2015
).
Feeling the heat: the effect of acute temperature changes on predator–prey interactions in coral reef fish
.
Conserv Physiol
3
,
cov011
.
Allan
,
B. J. M.
,
Domenici
,
P.
,
Watson
,
S. A.
,
Munday
,
P. L.
and
McCormick
,
M. I.
(
2017
).
Warming has a greater effect than elevated CO2 on predator-prey interactions in coral reef fish
.
Proc. R. Soc. B
284
,
20170784
.
Baker
,
R.
and
Sheaves
,
M.
(
2007
).
Shallow-water refuge paradigm: conflicting evidence from tethering experiments in a tropical estuary
.
Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser.
349
,
13
-
22
.
Bhattacharyya
,
K.
,
McLean
,
D. L.
and
MacIver
,
M. A.
(
2017
).
Visual threat assessment and reticulospinal encoding of calibrated responses in larval zebrafish
.
Curr. Biol.
27
,
2751
-
2762
.
Bindoff
,
N. L.
,
Cheung
,
W. W. L.
,
Kairo
,
J. G.
,
Arístegui
,
J.
,
Guinder
,
V. A.
,
Hallberg
,
R.
,
Hilmi
,
N.
,
Jiao
,
N.
,
Karim
,
M. S.
,
Levin
,
L.
et al. 
. (
2019
).
Changing ocean, marine ecosystems, and dependent communities
. In
IPCC Special Report on the Ocean and Cryosphere in a Changing Climate
(ed.
H.-O.
Pörtner
,
D. C.
Roberts
,
V.
Masson-Delmotte
,
M. T. P.
Zhai
,
E.
Poloczanska
,
K.
Mintenbeck
,
A.
Alegría
,
M.
Nicolai
,
A.
Okem
and
J.
Petzold
et al. 
), pp. 477-587. Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change.
Blake
,
R. W.
,
Chatters
,
L. M.
,
Domenici
,
P.
(
1995
).
Turning radius of yellowfin tuna (Thunnus albacares) in unsteady swimming manoeuvres
.
J. Fish Biol.
46
,
536
-
538
.
Bone
,
Q.
(
1977
).
Mauthner neurons in elasmobranchs
.
J. Mar. Biol. Assoc. UK
57
,
253
-
259
.
Bouyoucos
,
I. A.
,
Weideli
,
O. C.
,
Planes
,
S.
,
Simpfendorfer
,
C. A.
and
Rummer
,
J. L.
(
2018
).
Dead tired: evaluating the physiological status and survival of neonatal reef sharks under stress
.
Conserv. Physiol.
6
,
1
-
13
.
Bouyoucos
,
I. A.
,
Romain
,
M.
,
Azoulai
,
L.
,
Eustache
,
K.
,
Mourier
,
J.
,
Rummer
,
J. L.
and
Planes
,
S.
(
2020
).
Home range of newborn blacktip reef sharks (Carcharhinus melanopterus), as estimated using mark-recapture and acoustic telemetry
.
Coral Reefs
39
,
1209
-
1214
.
Bouyoucos
,
I. A.
,
Trujillo
,
J. E.
,
Weideli
,
O. C.
,
Nakamura
,
N.
,
Mourier
,
J.
,
Planes
,
S.
,
Simpfendorfer
,
C. A.
and
Rummer
,
J. L.
(
2021
).
Investigating links between thermal tolerance and oxygen supply capacity in shark neonates from a hyperoxic tropical environment
.
Sci. Total Environ.
782
,
146854
.
Bouyoucos
,
I. A.
,
Simpfendorfer
,
C. A.
,
Planes
,
S.
,
Schwieterman
,
G. D.
,
Weideli
,
O. C.
and
Rummer
,
J. L.
(
2022
).
Thermally insensitive physiological performance allows neonatal sharks to use coastal habitats as nursery areas
.
Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser.
682
,
137
-
152
.
Chin
,
A.
,
Mourier
,
J.
and
Rummer
,
J. L.
(
2015
).
Blacktip reef sharks (Carcharhinus melanopterus) show high capacity for wound healing and recovery following injury
.
Conserv. Biol.
3
,
cov062
.
Dadda
,
M.
,
Koolhaas
,
W. H.
and
Domenici
,
P.
(
2010
).
Behavioural asymmetry affects escape performance in a teleost fish
.
Biol. Lett.
6
,
414
-
417
.
Domenici
,
P.
(
2001
).
The scaling of locomotor performance in predator–prey encounters: from fish to killer whales
.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Mol. Integr. Physiol.
131
,
169
-
182
.
Domenici
,
P.
(
2010
).
Context-dependent variability in the components of fish escape response: integrating locomotor performance and behavior
.
J. Exp. Zool. A Ecol. Genet. Physiol.
313A
,
59
-
79
.
Domenici
,
P.
and
Batty
,
R. S.
(
1994
).
Escape manoeuvres of schooling Clupea harengus
.
J. Fish Biol.
45
,
97
-
110
.
Domenici
,
P.
and
Batty
,
R. S.
(
1997
).
Escape behaviour of solitary herring (Clupea harengus) and comparisons with schooling individuals
.
Mar. Biol.
128
,
29
-
38
.
Domenici
,
P.
and
Blake
,
R. W.
(
1991
).
The kinematics and performance of the escape response in the angelfish (Pterophyllum eimekei)
.
J. Exp. Biol.
156
,
187
-
205
.
Domenici
,
P.
and
Blake
,
R. W.
(
1993
).
The effect of size on the kinematics and performance of angelfish (Pterophyllum eimekei) escape responses
.
Can. J. Zool.
71
,
2319
-
2326
.
Domenici
,
P.
and
Blake
,
R. W.
(
1997
).
The kinematics and performance of fish fast-starts swimming
.
J. Exp. Biol.
200
,
1165
-
1178
.
Domenici
,
P.
and
Hale
,
M. E.
(
2019
).
Escape responses of fish: A review of the diversity in motor control, kinematics and behaviour
.
J. Exp. Biol.
222
,
jeb166009
.
Domenici
,
P.
,
Standen
,
E. M.
and
Levine
,
R. P.
(
2004
).
Escape manoeuvres in the spiny dogfish (Squalus acanthias)
.
J. Exp. Biol.
207
,
2339
-
2349
.
Domenici
,
P.
,
Lefrançois
,
C.
and
Shingles
,
A.
(
2007
).
Hypoxia and the antipredator behaviours of fishes
.
Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.
362
,
2105
-
2121
.
Domenici
,
P.
,
Blagburn
,
J. M.
and
Bacon
,
J. P.
(
2011
).
Animal escapology I: Theoretical issues and emerging trends in escape trajectories
.
J. Exp. Biol.
214
,
2463
-
2473
.
Domenici
,
P.
,
Allan
,
B. J. M.
,
Lefrançois
,
C.
and
McCormick
,
M. I.
(
2019
).
The effect of climate change on the escape kinematics and performance of fishes: implications for future predator-prey interactions
.
Conserv. Physiol.
7
,
coz078
.
Eaton
,
R. C.
,
Bombardieri
,
R. A.
and
Meyer
,
D. L.
(
1977
).
The Mauthner-initiated startle response in teleost fish
.
J. Exp. Biol.
66
,
65
-
81
.
Eaton
,
R. C.
,
Lee
,
R. K. K.
and
Foreman
,
M. B.
(
2001
).
The Mauthner cell and other identified neurons of the brainstem escape network of fish
.
Prog. Neurobiol.
63
,
467
-
485
.
Ebert
,
D. A.
,
White
,
W. T.
,
Goldman
,
K. J.
,
Compagno
,
L. J. V.
,
Daly-Engel
,
T. S.
and
Ward
,
R. D.
(
2010
).
Resurrection and redescription of Squalus suckleyi (Girard, 1854) from the North Pacific, with comments on the Squalus acanthias subgroup (Squaliformes: Squalidae)
.
Zootaxa
2612
,
22-40
-
22-40
.
Estes
,
J. A.
,
Heithaus
,
M.
,
McCauley
,
D. J.
,
Rasher
,
D. B.
and
Worm
,
B.
(
2016
).
Megafaunal impacts on structure and function of ocean ecosystems
.
Annu. Rev. Environ. Resour.
41
,
83
-
116
.
Ferreira
,
L. C.
,
Thums
,
M.
,
Heithaus
,
M. R.
,
Barnett
,
A.
,
Abrantes
,
K. G.
,
Holmes
,
B. J.
,
Zamora
,
L. M.
,
Frisch
,
A. J.
,
Pepperell
,
J. G.
,
Burkholder
,
D.
et al. 
(
2017
).
The trophic role of a large marine predator, the tiger shark Galeocerdo cuvier
.
Sci. Rep.
7
,
7641
.
Frith
,
H. R.
and
Blake
,
R. W.
(
1991
).
Mechanics of the startle response in the northern pike
.
Esox lucius.
69
,
2831
-
2839
.
Fuiman
,
L. A.
,
Rose
,
K. A.
,
Cowan
,
J. H.
and
Smith
,
E. P.
(
2006
).
Survival skills required for predator evasion by fish larvae and their relation to laboratory measures of performance
.
Anim. Behav.
71
,
1389
-
1399
.
Garrick
,
J. A. F
. (
1982
).
Sharks of the genus Carcharhinus
. NOAA Technical Report NMFS CIRC 445.
George
,
L. W.
,
Martins
,
A. P. B.
,
Heupel
,
M. R.
and
Simpfendorfer
,
C. A.
(
2019
).
Fine-scale movements of juvenile blacktip reef sharks Carcharhinus melanopterus in a shallow nearshore nursery
.
Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser.
623
,
85
-
97
.
Gruber
,
S. H.
,
de Marignac
,
J. R. C.
and
Hoenig
,
J. M.
(
2001
).
Survival of juvenile lemon sharks at Bimini, Bahamas, estimated by mark-depletion experiments
.
Trans. Am. Fish. Soc.
130
,
376
-
384
.
Guttridge
,
T. L.
,
Gruber
,
S. H.
,
Franks
,
B. R.
,
Kessel
,
S. T.
,
Gledhill
,
K. S.
,
Uphill
,
J.
,
Krause
,
J.
and
Sims
,
D. W.
(
2012
).
Deep danger: intra-specific predation risk influences habitat use and aggregation formation of juvenile lemon sharks Negaprion brevirostris
.
Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser.
445
,
279
-
291
.
Harper
,
D. G.
and
Blake
,
R. W.
(
1990
).
Fast-start performance of rainbow trout Salmo gairdneri and northern pike. Esox lucius
.
J. Exp. Biol.
150
,
321
-
342
.
Hecker
,
A.
,
Schulze
,
W.
,
Oster
,
J.
,
Richter
,
D. O.
and
Schuster
,
S.
(
2020
).
Removing a single neuron in a vertebrate brain forever abolishes an essential behavior
.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
117
,
3254
-
3260
.
Heithaus
,
M.
(
2007
).
Nursery areas as essential shark habitats: a theoretical perspective
.
Am. Fish. Soc. Symp.
50
,
3
-
13
.
Heupel
,
M. R.
and
Simpfendorfer
,
C. A.
(
2002
).
Estimation of mortality of juvenile blacktip sharks, Carcharhinus limbatus, within a nursery area using telemetry data
.
Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci.
59
,
624
-
632
.
Heupel
,
M. R.
,
Carlson
,
J. K.
and
Simpfendorfer
,
C. A.
(
2007
).
Shark nursery areas: concepts, definition, characterization and assumptions
.
Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser.
337
,
287
-
297
.
Hoffmann
,
S. L.
and
Porter
,
M. E.
(
2019
).
Body and pectoral fin kinematics during routine yaw turning in bonnethead sharks (Sphyrna tiburo)
.
Integr. Org. Biol.
1
,
obz014
.
Hoffmann
,
S. L.
,
Donatelli
,
C. M.
,
Leigh
,
S. C.
,
Brainerd
,
E. L.
and
Porter
,
M. E.
(
2019
).
Three-dimensional movements of the pectoral fin during yaw turns in the Pacific spiny dogfish, Squalus suckleyi
.
Biol. Open
8
,
bio037291
.
Hoffmann
,
S. L.
,
Buser
,
T. J.
and
Porter
,
M. E.
(
2020
).
Comparative morphology of shark pectoral fins
.
J. Morphol.
281
,
1501
-
1516
.
Howland
,
H. C.
(
1974
).
Optimal strategies for predator avoidance: the relative importance of speed and manoeuvrability
.
J. Theor. Biol.
47
,
333
-
350
.
Kajiura
,
S. M.
,
Forni
,
J. B.
and
Summers
,
A. P.
(
2003
).
Maneuvering in juvenile carcharhinid and sphyrnid sharks: the role of the hammerhead shark cephalofoil
.
Zoology
106
,
19
-
28
.
Kasapi
,
M. A.
,
Domenici
,
P.
,
Blake
,
R. W.
and
Harper
,
D.
(
1993
).
The kinematics and performance of escape responses of the knifefish Xenomystus nigri
.
Can. J. Zool.
71
,
189
-
195
.
Katzir
,
G.
and
Camhi
,
J. M.
(
1993
).
Escape response of black mollies (Poecilia sphenops) to predatory dives of a pied kingfisher (Ceryle rudis)
.
Copeia
1993
,
549
.
Klecka
,
J.
and
Boukal
,
D. S.
(
2013
).
Foraging and vulnerability traits modify predator-prey body mass allometry: freshwater macroinvertebrates as a case study
.
J. Anim. Ecol.
82
,
1031
-
1041
.
Lefrançois
,
C.
,
Shingles
,
A.
and
Domenici
,
P.
(
2005
).
The effect of hypoxia on locomotor performance and behaviour during escape in Liza aurata
.
J. Fish Biol.
67
,
1711
-
1729
.
Liu
,
K. S.
and
Fetcho
,
J. R.
(
1999
).
Laser ablations reveal functional relationships of segmental hindbrain neurons in zebrafish
.
Neuron
23
,
325
-
335
.
Long
,
J. H.
,
Porter
,
M. E.
,
Root
,
R. G.
and
Liew
,
C. W.
(
2010
).
Go reconfigure: how fish change shape as they swim and evolve
.
Integr. Comp. Biol.
50
,
1120
-
1139
.
Manire
,
C. A.
and
Gruber
,
S. H.
(
1993
).
A preliminary estimate of natural mortality of age-0 lemon sharks, Negaprion brevirostris
.
NOAA Tech. Rep. NMFS
115
,
65
-
71
.
Marras
,
S.
,
Killen
,
S. S.
,
Claireaux
,
G.
,
Domenici
,
P.
and
McKenzie
,
D. J.
(
2011
).
Behavioural and kinematic components of the fast-start escape response in fish: individual variation and temporal repeatability
.
J. Exp. Biol.
214
,
3102
-
3110
.
McCormick
,
M. I.
,
Fakan
,
E.
and
Allan
,
B. J. M.
(
2018
).
Behavioural measures determine survivorship within the hierarchy of whole-organism phenotypic traits
.
Funct. Ecol.
32
,
958
-
969
.
McPherson
,
D. L.
,
Blaiyok
,
K. V.
and
Masse
,
W. B.
(
2012
).
Lethal ramming of sharks by large jacks (Carangidae) in the Palau Islands, Micronesia
.
BioOne
66
,
327
-
333
.
Nissanov
,
J.
,
Eaton
,
R. C.
and
DiDomenico
,
R.
(
1990
).
The motor output of the Mauthner cell, a reticulospinal command neuron
.
Brain Res.
517
,
88
-
98
.
Porter
,
M. E.
,
Roque
,
C. M.
and
Long
,
J. H.
(
2011
).
Swimming fundamentals: turning performance of leopard sharks (Triakis semifasciata) is predicted by body shape and postural reconfiguration
.
Zoology
114
,
348
-
359
.
Rand
,
D. M.
and
Lauder
,
G. V.
(
1981
).
Prey capture in the chain pickerel, Esox niger: correlations between feeding and locomotor behavior
.
Can. J. Zool.
59
,
1072
-
1078
.
Russo
,
R. A.
(
2015
).
Observations of predation and loss among leopard sharks and brown smoothhounds in San Francisco Bay, California
.
Calif. Fish Game
101
,
149
-
157
.
Schakmann
,
M.
,
Becker
,
V.
,
Søgaard
,
M.
,
Johansen
,
J. L.
,
Steffensen
,
J. F.
and
Domenici
,
P.
(
2021
).
Latency of mechanically stimulated escape responses in the Pacific spiny dogfish, Squalus suckleyi
.
J. Exp. Biol.
224
,
jeb230698
.
Schmitz
,
O.
(
2017
).
Predator and prey functional traits: understanding the adaptive machinery driving predator–prey interactions
.
F1000Res
6
,
1767
.
Seamone
,
S.
,
Blaine
,
T.
and
Higham
,
T. E.
(
2014
).
Sharks modulate their escape behavior in response to predator size, speed and approach orientation
.
Zoology
117
,
377
-
382
.
Stefanelli
,
A
. (
1980
).
I neuroni di Mauthner degli ittiopsidi: valutazioni comparative morfologiche e funzionali
.
Accademia Nazionale dei Lincei
.
Turesson
,
H.
,
Satta
,
A.
and
Domenici
,
P.
(
2009
).
Preparing for escape: anti-predator posture and fast-start performance in gobies
.
J. Exp. Biol.
212
,
2925
-
2933
.
Violle
,
C.
,
Navas
,
M. L.
,
Vile
,
D.
,
Kazakou
,
E.
,
Fortunel
,
C.
,
Hummel
,
I.
and
Garnier
,
E.
(
2007
).
Let the concept of trait be functional!
Oikos
116
,
882
-
892
.
Wakeling
,
J. M
. (
2005
).
Fast-start mechanics
. In
Fish Biomechanics
(ed.
R. E.
Shadwick
and
G. V.
Lauder
), pp.
333
-
368
.
Academic Press
.
Wakeling
,
J. M.
,
Kemp
,
K. M.
and
Johnston
,
I. A.
(
1999
).
The biomechanics of fast-starts during ontogeny in the common carp Cyprinus carpio
.
J. Exp. Biol.
202
,
3057
-
3067
.
Walker
,
J. A.
,
Ghalambor
,
C. K.
,
Griset
,
O. L.
,
McKenney
,
D.
and
Reznick
,
D. N.
(
2005
).
Do faster starts increase the probability of evading predators?
Funct. Ecol.
19
,
808
-
815
.
Webb
,
P. W.
(
1976a
).
The effect of size on the fast-start performance of rainbow trout Salmo gairdneri, and a consideration of piscivorous predator-prey interactions
.
J. Exp. Biol.
65
,
157
-
177
.
Webb
,
P. W.
(
1976b
).
The effect of size on the fast-start performance of rainbow trout Salmo gairdneri, and a consideration of piscivorous predator-prey interactions
.
J. Exp. Biol.
65
,
157
-
177
.
Webb
,
P. W.
and
Skadsen
,
J. M.
(
1980
).
Strike tactics of Esox
.
Can. J. Zool.
58
,
1462
-
1469
.
Weideli
,
O. C.
,
Papastamatiou
,
Y. P.
and
Planes
,
S.
(
2019
).
Size frequency, dispersal distances and variable growth rates of young sharks in a multi–species aggregation
.
J. Fish Biol.
94
,
789
-
797
.
Weihs
,
D.
(
1973
).
The mechanism of rapid starting of slender fish
.
Biorheology
10
,
343
-
350
.
Weihs
,
D.
and
Webb
,
P. W.
(
1984
).
Optimal avoidance and evasion tactics in predator-prey interactions
.
J. Theor. Biol.
106
,
189
-
206
.
Wetherbee
,
B.
,
Gruber
,
S.
and
Rosa
,
R.
(
2007
).
Movement patterns of juvenile lemon sharks Negaprion brevirostris within Atol das Rocas, Brazil: a nursery characterized by tidal extremes
.
Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser.
343
,
283
-
293
.
Wheeler
,
C. R.
,
Gervais
,
C. R.
,
Johnson
,
M. S.
,
Vance
,
S.
,
Rosa
,
R.
,
Mandelman
,
J. W.
and
Rummer
,
J. L.
(
2020
).
Anthropogenic stressors influence reproduction and development in elasmobranch fishes
.
Rev. Fish Biol. Fish.
30
,
373
-
386
.
Wilga
,
C. D.
and
Lauder
,
G.
(
2000
).
Three-dimensional kinematics and wake structure of the pectoral fins during locomotion in leopard sharks Triakis semifasciata
.
J. Exp. Biol.
203
,
2261
-
2278
.
Wilga
,
C. D.
and
Lauder
,
G.
(
2001
).
Functional morphology of the pectoral fins in bamboo sharks, Chiloscyllium plagiosum: benthic vs. pelagic station-holding
.
J. Morphol.
249
,
195
-
209
.
Wilga
,
C.
and
Lauder
,
G
. (
2004
).
Locomotion in sharks, skates and rays
. In
In Biology of Sharks and their Relatives
(ed.
J.C.
Carrier
,
J.
Musick
and
M.
Heithaus
), pp.
139
-
164
.
Boca Raton
:
CRC Press
.
Wynn
,
M. L.
,
Clemente
,
C.
,
Nasir
,
A. F. A. A.
and
Wilson
,
R. S.
(
2015
).
Running faster causes disaster: trade-offs between speed, manoeuvrability and motor control when running around corners in northern quolls (Dasyurus hallucatus)
.
J. Exp. Biol.
218
,
433
-
439
.
Yan
,
G. J.
,
He
,
X. K.
,
Cao
,
Z. D.
and
Fu
,
S. J.
(
2015
).
Effects of fasting and feeding on the fast-start swimming performance of southern catfish Silurus meridionalis
.
J. Fish Biol.
86
,
605
-
614
.
Zottoli
,
S. J.
(
1977
).
Correlation of the startle reflex and Mauthner cell auditory responses in unrestrained goldfish
.
J. Exp. Biol.
66
,
243
-
254
.
Zuur
,
A. F.
,
Ieno
,
E. N.
,
Walker
,
N. J.
,
Saveliev
,
A. A.
and
Smith
,
G. M.
(
2009
).
Mixed Effects Models and Extensions in Ecology with R
.
New York
:
Springer
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

Supplementary information