Life underground has shaped the auditory sense of subterranean mammals, shifting their hearing range to low frequencies. Mole-rats of the genus Fukomys have, however, been suggested to hear at frequencies up to 18.5 kHz, unusually high for a subterranean rodent. We present audiograms of three mole-rat species, Fukomys anselli, Fukomys micklemi and the giant mole-rat Fukomys mechowii, based on evoked auditory brainstem potentials. All species showed low sensitivity and restricted hearing ranges at 60 dB SPL extending from 125 Hz to 4 kHz (5 octaves) with most-sensitive hearing between 0.8 kHz and 1.4 kHz. The high-frequency cut-offs are the lowest found in mammals to date. In contrast to predictions from middle ear morphology, F. mechowii did not show higher sensitivity than F. anselli in the low-frequency range. These data suggest that the hearing range of Fukomys mole-rats is highly restricted to low frequencies and similar to that of other subterranean mammals.

African mole-rats of the genus Fukomys spend their entire life in underground tunnel systems. The aphotic subterranean ecotope has led to manifold sensory adaptations (Burda et al., 1990). The underground acoustic environment is characterized by quick attenuation of high frequencies, whereas low-frequency airborne sound waves are less attenuated and under some conditions are slightly amplified in certain frequency windows (stethoscope effect at 200–800 Hz; Heth et al., 1986; Lange et al., 2007; Quilliam, 1966).

Several studies have addressed the hearing capabilities of subterranean and fossorial mammals of different genera (Begall et al., 2004, 2007; Brückmann and Burda, 1997; Heffner and Heffner, 1990, 1992, 1993; reviewed in Begall et al., 2017). In comparison to similar-sized epigeic mammals, audiograms of subterranean mammals are characterized by lower frequencies of most-sensitive hearing and extremely restricted high-frequency hearing. Furthermore, absolute sensitivities are low, which has been interpreted as either a degeneration of the auditory sense (Heffner and Heffner, 1992, 1993) or an adaptation to the stethoscope effect (Burda, 2006; Lange et al., 2007). For example, while the most-sensitive frequency of hearing in the naked mole-rat Heterocephalus glaber was found at 4 kHz with a threshold of 35 dB sound pressure level (SPL) (Heffner and Heffner, 1993), epigeic animals of comparable body size typically have hearing ranges between 500 Hz and 70 kHz, with best frequencies well above 6 kHz and absolute sensitivities near to or even below 0 dB SPL (Vater and Kössl, 2011). The overall hearing range (at 60 dB SPL) in the naked mole-rat spans seven octaves between 65 Hz and 12.8 kHz (Heffner and Heffner, 1993). The blind mole-rat Spalax ehrenbergi has been shown to hear between 54 Hz and 5.9 kHz (<7 octaves, best frequency 1 kHz), representing the lowest high-frequency hearing limit found in a mammal to date (Heffner and Heffner, 1992). Interestingly, however, behavioural tests of the bathyergid mole-rat Fukomys anselli, which is closely related to the naked mole-rat, suggested an astonishingly broad hearing range from below 225 Hz (the lowest frequency tested) up to 18.5 kHz (Brückmann and Burda, 1997). Apart from the high-frequency limit, F. anselli exhibited the typical subterranean characteristics with high absolute thresholds, most-sensitive hearing at 800 Hz and an interesting anatomical adaptation for low-frequency hearing termed the acoustic fovea. While in other mammals the width of the basilar membrane increases more or less continuously from the cochlear base to the apex, in F. anselli around 50% of the basilar membrane length has a constant width, representing the cochlear place–frequency map from 600 Hz to 1000 Hz (Müller et al. 1992; Kössl et al., 1996). These cochlear place–frequency maps and electrophysiological recordings from the auditory brainstem did not support the suggested high-frequency limit of 18.5 kHz in F. anselli, as both found steep decreases above 5–12.6 kHz (Müller and Burda, 1989; Müller et al., 1992; note that when this work was published, F. anselli was referred to as Cryptomys hottentotus; the genus Fukomys emancipated from the genus Cryptomys in 2006; Kock et al., 2006). Findings from evoked otoacoustic emissions (Kössl et al., 1996) suggested that the deep anaesthesia used during brainstem recordings selectively affected high-frequency thresholds. It thus remained unclear whether high-frequency sensitivity (for a subterranean mammal) is an artefact produced by experimental techniques, or rather is an accurate reflection of the auditory sensory system and represents a specific trait of the genus Fukomys.

To date, no behavioural audiograms of mole-rats (genus Fukomys) with individuals individually tested in a well-defined sound field are available. As the animals do not drink water or readily accept liquid food, the gold standard conditioned avoidance procedures are difficult to apply, and therefore only a single behavioural audiogram based on group responses is available for F. anselli (Brückmann and Burda, 1997). The minimally invasive recording of auditory brainstem responses (ABRs) offers an alternative that allows for repeated testing of individuals in a controlled environment (Müller and Burda, 1989; Shvarev, 1994; Martin et al., 2012; Brandt et al., 2013). ABRs are early acoustically evoked potentials which contain all the retrocochlear information needed to establish an audiogram of a species (e.g. Lucke et al., 2016). Using this method, we set out to obtain additional audiograms from Fukomys mole-rats. To address the above-mentioned high-frequency limit issue, we retested F. anselli and compared it with two closely related species: Fukomys micklemi, which is of similar body size to F. anselli, and Fukomys mechowii, which is of a much larger body size and exhibits notable differences in middle ear morphology (Lange and Burda, 2005). We hypothesized that (1) our ABR measurements in the three Fukomys species, despite using far lower anaesthetic doses, resemble the previous data obtained by evoked potential recordings in F. anselli with a high-frequency cut-off much lower than 18 kHz, and (2) F. mechowii has a higher hearing sensitivity at low frequencies than F. anselli and F. micklemi, promoted by a larger middle ear cavity and a higher transformation ratio of the middle ear.

Animals and housing conditions

We tested six Ansell's mole-rats [Fukomys anselli (Burda, Zima, Scharff, Macholán and Kawalika 1999)], five Micklem mole-rats [Fukomys micklemi (Chubb 1909)] and five giant mole-rats [Fukomys mechowii (Peters 1881)]. All animals were adult but not senescent (1–3 years of age; Table S1) and were visually inspected before testing, showing no signs of illness or auditory disorder. Note that the average lifespan is approximately 4–6 years in these species with maximum lifespans in reproductively active animals being even longer (Dammann and Burda, 2006; Dammann et al., 2011). The animals were born at the Department of General Zoology at the University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany. They were housed on a 12 h:12 h light:dark cycle, 24±1°C constant temperature and 40–50% humidity in terraria lined with wood shavings and enriched with clay pots. The size of the terraria varied with the size of the colonies. Carrots and potatoes were provided ad libitum and regularly supplemented with salad and dry rodent food. All animals were returned to their colonies immediately after the sessions.

Recordings of ABRs

The experiments took place between August and November 2015, except for animals F8 and F9, which were tested in July and August 2017. Animals were anaesthetized via intramuscular injection of ketamine and xylazine (see Table S3 for detailed doses; Garcia Montero et al., 2015) and placed in a custom-made anechoic chamber (see Malkemper et al., 2015). During recordings, body temperature was maintained by a non-electric Deltaphase isothermal heating pad (Braintree Scientific, Braintree, MA, USA) and regularly controlled with a rectal electrode. All procedures were approved by the North Rhine-Westphalia State Environment Agency (permit number: 84-02.04.2015.A383). Stimulus generation and presentation as well as the amplification and digitalizing of recorded responses were performed with a Tucker-Davis Technologies (TDT, Alachua, FL, USA) System 3 RZ6. Stimuli were presented via a Tannoy Arena Satellite speaker (frequency response 80 Hz to 54 kHz) positioned 15 cm from the left pinna of the animals at an angle of 90 deg (the angle of sound incidence was 0 deg; see Fig. S1). For frequencies below 125 Hz, a subwoofer (Punch HE Rockford Fosgate, Tempe, AZ, USA) was used and placed on the foam-lined floor of the anechoic chamber below the level of the animal. Both speakers were driven by the built-in amplifier of the RZ6 multi I/O processor and calibrated with a ¼ in free field microphone (type 4939 with preamplifier 2669 C and conditioning amplifier Nexus 2692-A, Brüel & Kjær, Nærum, Denmark; frequency response 4 Hz to 100 kHz) placed at the position of the subject's ear. A digital oscilloscope (Picoscope 4224, Pico Technology Ltd, St Neots, UK) connected to the output of the conditioning amplifier served to check the frequency content of the stimuli. The built-in calibration tool of the BioSig RZ software (v5.7.0, TDT) was used to check the SPL. The BioSig RZ software system in turn was calibrated using a Brüel & Kjær 4230 sound level calibrator with a ¼ in microphone adaptor (B&K DB 0310). During sound field calibration, a dummy was placed at the position of the animal to simulate the acoustic environment during the recordings as closely as possible. Auditory stimuli were 5 ms (1 ms rise/fall times) pure tones presented 12 times per second. Alternating starting phases were used to reduce stimulus artefacts. During tests, the tone bursts were presented 256 times at each SPL and brainstem responses were averaged afterwards. Sound intensities were decreased in 10 dB steps between 80 dB SPL and 50 dB SPL and in 5 dB steps between 50 dB SPL and 20 dB SPL. All calibrations and measurements were performed within a grounded aluminium Faraday cage (23.5×23.5×20 cm) placed inside the anechoic chamber, housing the individual as well as the headstage and preamplifier. We tested 16 frequencies in a range from 50 Hz to 36 kHz (50 Hz, 125 Hz, 250 Hz, 354 Hz, 500 Hz, 630 Hz, 800 Hz, 1000 Hz, 1400 Hz, 2000 Hz, 4000 Hz, 8000 Hz, 12,500 Hz, 16,500 Hz, 18,500 Hz, 36,000 Hz). To pick up brainstem potentials, 27 gauge, 13 mm, subdermal, stainless steel recording electrodes (Rochester Electro-Medical Inc., Lutz, FL, USA) were used. The active electrode was placed at the vertex of the animal. The reference was placed at the brainstem and the ground at the mastoid (Fig. S1). The electrodes were coupled to a RA4LI low impedance headstage (TDT). The signals were pre-amplified by a Medusa RA4PA (TDT), fed to the RZ6 multiprocessor via a fibre optic cable and averaged within the BioSig software (v5.7.0, TDT).

Threshold determination

Averaged ABR waveforms for all tested intensities were printed for each frequency and thresholds were manually determined by three independent observers who were blind to the frequency condition. Threshold was defined as the mean between the lowest stimulus level at which a response could be visually detected and the next stimulus level below this (Fig. 1). The mean threshold of the three observers was calculated and only accepted as the threshold of a session if the standard deviation between the observers was less than 10 dB. If the standard deviation was higher than 10 dB, the recording for the respective frequency was repeated.

Statistics

Interspecies comparisons of threshold levels for the tested frequencies were performed with SPSS Statistics v24.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). Normal distribution of threshold levels for each frequency was tested with the Shapiro–Wilk test. Multiple comparisons of normally distributed data were calculated with one-way ANOVA followed by the Bonferroni post hoc test for pairwise comparison. Data that did not follow a normal distribution were analysed with the Kruskal–Wallis test followed by the Dunn–Bonferroni post hoc test for pairwise comparison.

Hearing ranges

The mean hearing range (at 60 dB SPL) of the three tested Fukomys species extended from 125 Hz to 4 kHz (Fig. 2, Table 1; for individual thresholds, see Table S2). Overall sensitivities were low: F. anselli and F. micklemi showed highest sensitivity (F. anselli: 29 dB SPL, F. micklemi: 37 dB SPL) at 1 kHz, and F. mechowii showed highest sensitivity at 1 kHz and 1.4 kHz (33 dB SPL). These hearing ranges, absolute sensitivities and frequencies of most-sensitive hearing are in good agreement with reported hearing parameters of other subterranean mammals (reviewed in Begall et al., 2017). High absolute thresholds compared with similar sized terrestrial mammals and most-sensitive hearing at low frequencies seem to be ecophysiological adaptations to the subterranean ecotope.

Variation between the individuals of each species was generally low and more pronounced at high frequencies. Only F. micklemi showed inter-individual variation over the entire frequency range. Fukomys micklemi was also the least sensitive of the three species. Its thresholds were significantly higher at 500 Hz (one-way ANOVA, F=22.6, P<0.0001), 630 Hz (F=11.1, P=0.002) and 1 kHz (only different from F. anselli, F=4.1, P=0.042). No significant differences were found between the thresholds of F. anselli and F. mechowii. We also tested all species at 36 kHz. None of the tested animals showed any measureable brainstem potentials as a response to these ultrasounds.

Comparison with existing audiograms

Our data are partially consistent with earlier studies of F. anselli, the subterranean rodent with the best-characterized auditory system to date. Both the frequency of the greatest hearing sensitivity (1 kHz) and the lowest perceived SPL at this frequency (28 dB SPL) are similar to the collective behavioural audiogram from previous studies (800 Hz, 24 dB SPL, Table 1; Brückmann and Burda, 1997). This finding is startling as thresholds determined by ABRs are usually considerably higher than behavioural thresholds (see below). This means either that our system is sensitive enough to pick up potential changes at the absolute threshold of perception in these animals or that the actual thresholds of F. anselli were underestimated by Brückmann and Burda (1997). Only well-controlled individual behavioural audiograms will solve this issue.

The high-frequency limit of F. anselli

These similarities demonstrate that our results are comparable with the collective behavioural audiogram, raising questions about the discrepancy in the higher frequency range. The high-frequency cut-offs of the three Fukomys species we studied are the lowest reported for mammals. Partly, this might be related to the applied method. In mammals, auditory thresholds determined by ABR are on average 10–30 dB SPL higher than those determined behaviourally (Gorga et al., 1988). However, the relationship is not linear across frequencies, generally being smallest at high frequencies, and in some cases ABR have even yielded lower thresholds than behavioural assessment (Gorga et al., 1988; Szymanski et al., 1999). Still, it is likely that the actual hearing range of the Fukomys species tested here is slightly broader than our ABR results, which agrees with an earlier ABR audiogram of F. anselli (Table 1; Müller and Burda, 1989). However, even if we liberally correct the thresholds to account for the lower sensitivity of ABRs by 15 dB SPL at each frequency, the 60 dB SPL high-frequency cut-off in F. anselli only shifts up to 12.5 kHz. This upper limit would be similar to that of the closely related naked mole-rat and would match cochlear place–frequency maps established for F. anselli (Müller et al., 1992), but still is in disagreement with the 18.5 kHz suggested by the collective behavioural audiogram of Brückmann and Burda (1997) and the otoacoustic emissions of Kössl et al. (1996). Two explanations have been suggested for discrepancies regarding the upper hearing limit of Fukomys mole-rats (Kössl et al., 1996). (1) Deep anaesthesia selectively influences high-frequency thresholds; with 90 mg kg−1 ketamine, otoacoustic emissions quickly declined above 4 kHz, but after reducing the dose to 50 mg kg−1, they were measureable up to 18 kHz (Kössl et al., 1996). However, in the current study, we used anaesthetic doses nearly tenfold lower than 50 mg kg−1 (Table S3), and therefore it is more likely that (2) the outer ear of mole-rats with its tight meatus filled with hair and cerumen acts as a low-frequency filter that is bypassed in otoacoustic measurements, which are performed at the tympanic membrane (Kössl et al., 1996).

If true, how could we explain the 18.5 kHz upper hearing limit determined in the behavioural audiogram of Brückmann and Burda (1997)? We think this might not reflect the true auditory range for two reasons. First, there are general problems associated with the sound calibration in a collective audiogram as a result of the study design. A group of animals is tested and the researcher uses the first response of an animal in a group as a measure of sound perception. The actual SPL at the ear of an animal is, however, highly dependent on the exact position and orientation of the animal within the sound field, two factors that are hard to control when group responses are measured. Furthermore, the microphone (B&K 4145) that Brückmann and Burda (1997) used to calibrate the sound field in the collective audiogram study had an upper frequency response limit at 18 kHz. Operating at or above the limit increases the likelihood of underestimating the actual SPL: differences in the angle of incidence can lead to differences of up to 20 dB when using this microphone to measure such high frequencies (the free field correction curves for the B&K 4145 are available at https://www.bksv.com/-/media/literature/Product-Data/bp2032.ashx). As these problems do not apply to our setup (the microphone B&K 4939 was never operating near the limit and the position of each animal was identical across all sessions), we conclude that high-frequency hearing of Fukomys mole-rats is at least as restricted as in other strictly subterranean mammals.

Further corroborating our results, Fukomys mole-rats are highly vocal social mammals and the hearing ranges of the three mole-rat species obtained in the present study are in good agreement with the fundamental frequencies of their vocalizations (reviewed in Begall et al., 2017). A great majority of calls in the vocal repertoire of Fukomys have main frequencies between 0.4 kHz and 2.5 kHz (F. anselli: Credner et al., 1997; F. mechowii: Bednářová et al., 2013; F. micklemi: Vanden Hole et al., 2013). Importantly, no calls with fundamental frequencies higher than 10 kHz have been reported, supporting the idea that higher frequencies may not be ecologically/functionally relevant to the animals.

Functional correlations with ear morphology

The morphology of the mole-rat auditory periphery has been well studied (e.g. Lange and Burda, 2005; Mason et al., 2016), allowing us to make some predictions about the hearing abilities of our study species. In small mammals, the volume of the middle ear cavity is one of the main factors determining sensitivity in the low-frequency range (Mason, 2016a). Mammals adapted to low-frequency hearing, e.g. gerbils, possess enlarged middle ear cavities (Webster and Webster, 1975; Mason, 2016b). While the middle ear cavities of mole-rats in general are not particularly enlarged, the middle ear cavity of F. mechowii is more than two times larger than that of the two other study species (Lange and Burda, 2005; Mason et al., 2016). Furthermore, the transformation ratio, the product of the lever ratio of the malleus and incus and the area ratio of the tympanic and oval window membrane, often used as a proxy for the biomechanical transmission efficiency of the middle ear, is higher in F. mechowii (Lange and Burda, 2005). On this basis, we predicted F. mechowii to show higher sensitivity of hearing than F. anselli and F. micklemi, especially at low frequencies. Instead, we found that the hearing thresholds of F. mechowii are similar to those of F. anselli over the whole range of frequencies. This again demonstrates that morphological predictors of hearing ability need to be interpreted with extreme care (Mason et al., 2016). For mole-rats, it is likely that the hearing range is primarily restricted by the filter properties of the outer ear and perhaps the cochlea or higher order processing, while the middle ear seems to be a broadband transmitter (Mason et al., 2016; Gessele et al., 2016). Notably, the region of most-sensitive hearing around 1 kHz fits within the region of the auditory fovea in F. anselli (Müller and Burda, 1992; Kössl et al., 1996; Pleštilová et al., 2016). Given the small differences in auditory sensitivity between the three species, we would expect F. mechowii and F. micklemi to also possess an auditory fovea. So far, no morphological or electrophysiological data have been published that would enable us to test this hypothesis.

Sound localization and high-frequency hearing in mammals

Small mammals rely on intensity differences created by the shading of the incoming sound by the head for sound localization. However, their small heads only attenuate high-frequency sounds effectively (Heffner and Heffner, 2016). Mole-rats with the functional head size (the minimal time a sound needs to travel from one ear to the other) of F. anselli (mean±s.d.: 78±7 µs, n=10) and F. micklemi (80±8 µs, n=10) would have to hear up to 70 kHz (Heffner and Heffner, 2016) to detect such intensity differences. The high-frequency limit of the larger F. mechowii (115±10 µs, n=10) would be expected to be around 60 kHz. Our data clearly demonstrate the independence of mole-rats from this relationship. Life in a more or less one-dimensional environment does not require accurate sound localization, thus releasing species from the need for sound localization, and therefore reduces the selective pressure for high-frequency hearing (Heffner and Heffner, 1992, 1993, 2016).

In summary, we present data on the hearing of three closely related subterranean Fukomys species, demonstrating low sensitivity and highly restricted high-frequency hearing.

We thank Hynek Burda for providing lab space, access to mole-rats and scientific guidance, Karin Knaup for drawing the mole-rat in Fig. S1, Sandra Malewski, Christin Osadnik and Sabine Martini for help with the data analysis and Michael S. Painter for proofreading the manuscript.

Author contributions

Conceptualization: E.P.M.; Methodology: P.G., E.P.M.; Validation: Y.H., S.B., E.P.M.; Formal analysis: P.G., Y.H., S.B., E.P.M.; Investigation: P.G.; Data curation: P.G., Y.H., S.B.; Writing - original draft: E.P.M.; Writing - review & editing: P.G., Y.H., S.B., E.P.M.; Visualization: P.G., E.P.M.; Supervision: E.P.M.; Project administration: E.P.M.; Funding acquisition: E.P.M.

Funding

This research was funded by a starting grant of the University of Duisburg-Essen (to E.P.M.).

Bednářová
,
R.
,
Hrouzková-Knotková
,
E.
,
Burda
,
H.
,
Sedláček
,
F.
and
Šumbera
,
R.
(
2013
).
Vocalizations of the giant mole-rat (Fukomys mechowii), a subterranean rodent with the richest vocal repertoire
.
Bioacoustics
22
,
87
-
107
.
Begall
,
S.
,
Burda
,
H.
and
Schneider
,
B.
(
2004
).
Hearing in coruros (Spalacopus cyanus): special audiogram features of a subterranean rodent
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
190
,
963
-
969
.
Begall
,
S.
,
Lange
,
S.
,
Schleich
,
C. E.
and
Burda
,
H.
(
2007
).
Acoustics, audition and auditory system
. In
Subterranean Rodents–News from Underground
(
ed. S. Begall, H. Burda and C.E. Schleich
), pp.
97
-
111
.
New York
:
Springer
.
Begall
,
S.
,
Malkemper
,
E. P.
and
Mason
,
M. J.
(
2017
).
Bioacoustical studies on subterranean mammals
. In
Exploring Animal Behaviour through Sound Vol. 2: Applications of Bioacoustical Studies
(ed.
J.
Thomas
and
Andrea
Simmons
).
New York
:
Springer
.
Brandt
,
C.
,
Malmkvist
,
J.
,
Nielsen
,
R. L.
,
Brande-Lavridsen
,
N.
and
Surlykke
,
A.
(
2013
).
Development of vocalization and hearing in American mink (Neovison vison)
.
J. Exp. Biol.
216
,
3542
-
3550
.
Brückmann
,
G.
and
Burda
,
H.
(
1997
).
Hearing in blind subterranean Zambian mole-rats (Cryptomys sp.): collective behavioural audiogram in a highly social rodent
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
181
,
83
-
88
.
Burda
,
H.
(
2006
).
Ear and eye in subterranean mole-rats, Fukomys anselli (Bathyergidae) and Spalax ehrenbergi (Spalacidae): progressive specialisation or regressive degeneration?
Anim. Biol.
56
,
475
-
486
.
Burda
,
H.
,
Bruns
,
V.
and
Müller
,
M.
(
1990
).
Sensory adaptations in subterranean mammals
. In
Evolution of Subterranean Mammals at the Organismal and Molecular Levels
(ed.
E.
Nevo
and
O. A.
Reig
), pp.
269
-
293
.
New York
:
Wiley-Liss
.
Credner
,
S.
,
Burda
,
H.
and
Ludescher
,
F.
(
1997
).
Acoustic communication underground: vocalization characteristics in subterranean social mole-rats (Cryptomys sp., Bathyergidae)
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
180
,
245
-
255
.
Dammann
,
P.
and
Burda
,
H.
(
2006
).
Sexual activity and reproduction delay ageing in a mammal
.
Curr. Biol.
16
,
R117
-
R118
.
Dammann
,
P.
,
Šumbera
,
R.
,
Maßmann
,
C.
,
Scherag
,
A.
and
Burda
,
H.
(
2011
).
Extended longevity of reproductives appears to be common in Fukomys mole-rats (Rodentia, Bathyergidae)
.
PLoS ONE
6
,
e18757
.
Garcia Montero
,
A.
,
Burda
,
H.
and
Begall
,
S.
(
2015
).
Chemical restraint of African mole-rats (Fukomys sp.) with a combination of ketamine and xylazine
.
Vet. Anaesth. Analg
.
42
,
187
-
191
.
Gessele
,
N.
,
Garcia-Pino
,
E.
,
Omerbašić
,
D.
,
Park
,
T. J.
and
Koch
,
U.
(
2016
).
Structural changes and lack of HCN1 channels in the binaural auditory brainstem of the naked mole-rat (Heterocephalus glaber)
.
PLoS ONE
11
,
e0146428
.
Gorga
,
M. P.
,
Kaminski
,
J. R.
,
Beauchaine
,
K. A.
and
Jesteadt
,
W.
(
1988
).
Auditory brainstem responses to tone bursts in normally hearing subjects
.
J. Speech Lang. Hear. R.
31
,
87
-
97
.
Heffner
,
R. S.
and
Heffner
,
H. E.
(
1990
).
Vestigial hearing in a fossorial mammal, the pocket gopher (Geomys bursarius)
.
Hear. Res.
46
,
239
-
525
.
Heffner
,
R. S.
and
Heffner
,
H. E.
(
1992
).
Hearing and sound localization in blind mole rats (Spalax ehrenbergi)
.
Hear. Res.
62
,
206
-
216
.
Heffner
,
R. S.
and
Heffner
,
H. E.
(
1993
).
Degenerate hearing and sound localization in naked mole-rats (Heterocephalus glaber), with an overview of central auditory structures
.
J. Comp. Neurol.
331
,
418
-
433
.
Heffner
,
H. E.
and
Heffner
,
R. S.
(
2016
).
The evolution of mammalian sound localization
.
Acoustics Today
12
,
20
-
27
.
Heffner
,
H. E.
,
Heffner
,
R. S.
,
Contos
,
C.
and
Ott
,
T.
(
1994a
).
Audiogram of the hooded Norway rat
.
Hear. Res.
73
,
244
-
247
.
Heffner
,
R. S.
,
Heffner
,
H. E.
,
Contos
,
C.
and
Kearns
,
D.
(
1994b
).
Hearing in prairie dogs: transition between surface and subterranean rodents
.
Hear. Res.
73
,
185
-
189
.
Heth
,
G.
,
Frankenberg
,
E.
and
Nevo
,
E.
(
1986
).
Adaptive optimal sound for vocal communication in tunnels of a subterranean mammal (Spalax ehrenbergi)
.
Experientia
42
,
1287
-
1289
.
Kock
,
D.
,
Ingram
,
C. M.
,
Frabotta
,
L. J.
,
Burda
,
H.
and
Honeycutt
,
R. L.
(
2006
).
On the nomenclature of Bathyergidae and Fukomys n.gen. (Mammalia: Rodentia)
.
Zootaxa
1142
,
51
-
55
.
Kössl
,
M.
,
Frank
,
G.
,
Burda
,
H.
and
Müller
,
M.
(
1996
).
Acoustic distortion products from the cochlea of the blind African mole rat, Cryptomys spec
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
178
,
427
-
434
.
Lange
,
S.
and
Burda
,
H.
(
2005
).
Comparative and functional morphology of the middle ear in Zambezian mole-rats (Coetomys–Cryptomys, Bathyergidae)
.
Belg. J. Zool.
135
,
5
-
10
.
Lange
,
S.
,
Burda
,
H.
,
Wegner
,
R. E.
,
Dammann
,
P.
,
Begall
,
S.
and
Kawalika
,
M.
(
2007
).
Living in a “stethoscope”: burrow-acoustics promote auditory specializations in subterranean rodents
.
Naturwissenschaften
94
,
134
-
138
.
Lucke
,
K.
,
Hastie
,
G. D.
,
Ternes
,
K.
,
McConnell
,
B.
,
Moss
,
S.
,
Russell
,
D. J. F.
,
Weber
,
H.
and
Janik
,
V. M.
(
2016
).
Aerial low-frequency hearing in captive and free-ranging harbour seals (Phoca vitulina) measured using auditory brainstem responses
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
202
,
859
-
868
.
Malkemper
,
E. P.
,
Topinka
,
V.
and
Burda
,
H.
(
2015
).
A behavioral audiogram of the red fox (Vulpes vulpes)
.
Hear. Res.
320
,
30
-
37
.
Martin
,
K. J.
,
Alessi
,
S. C.
,
Gaspard
,
J. C.
,
Tucker
,
A. D.
,
Bauer
,
G. B.
and
Mann
,
D. A.
(
2012
).
Underwater hearing in the loggerhead turtle (Caretta caretta): a comparison of behavioral and auditory evoked potential audiograms
.
J. Exp. Biol.
215
,
3001
-
3009
.
Mason
,
M. J.
(
2016a
).
Structure and function of the mammalian middle ear. II: Inferring function from structure
.
J. Anat.
228
,
300
-
312
.
Mason
,
M. J.
(
2016b
).
Structure and function of the mammalian middle ear. I: Large middle ears in small desert mammals
.
J. Anat.
228
,
284
-
299
.
Mason
,
M. J.
,
Cornwall
,
H. L.
and
Smith
,
E. S. J.
(
2016
).
Ear structures of the naked mole-rat, Heterocephalus glaber, and its relatives (Rodentia: Bathyergidae)
.
PLoS ONE
11
,
e0167079
.
Müller
,
M.
and
Burda
,
H.
(
1989
).
Restricted hearing range in a subterranean rodent, Cryptomys hottentotus
.
Naturwissenschaften
76
,
134
-
135
.
Müller
,
M.
,
Laube
,
B.
,
Burda
,
H.
and
Bruns
,
V.
(
1992
).
Structure and function of the cochlea in the African mole rat (Cryptomys hottentotus): evidence for a low frequency acoustic fovea
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
171
,
469
-
476
.
Pleštilová
,
L.
,
Hrouzková
,
E.
,
Burda
,
H.
and
Šumbera
,
R.
(
2016
).
Does the morphology of the ear of the Chinese bamboo rat (Rhizomys sinensis) show “Subterranean” characteristics?
J. Morphol.
277
,
575
-
584
.
Quilliam
,
T. A.
(
1966
).
The mole's sensory apparatus
.
J. Zool. Lond.
149
,
76
-
88
.
Shvarev
,
Y. N.
(
1994
).
Brainstem auditory evoked potentials characteristics in mice: the effect of genotype
.
Hear. Res.
81
,
189
-
198
.
Szymanski
,
M. D.
,
Bain
,
D. E.
,
Kiehl
,
K.
,
Pennington
,
S.
,
Wong
,
S.
and
Henry
,
K. R.
(
1999
).
Killer whale (Orcinus orca) hearing: auditory brainstem response and behavioral audiograms
.
J. Acoustic. Soc. Am.
106
,
1134
-
1141
.
Vanden Hole
,
C.
,
Van Daele
,
P. A. A. G.
,
Desmet
,
N.
,
Devos
,
P.
and
Adriaens
,
D.
(
2013
).
Does sociality imply a complex vocal communication system? A case study for Fukomys micklemi (Bathyergidae, Rodentia)
.
Bioacoustics
23
,
143
-
160
.
Vater
,
M.
and
Kössl
,
M.
(
2011
).
Comparative aspects of cochlear functional organization in mammals
.
Hear. Res.
273
,
89
-
99
.
Webster
,
D. B.
and
Webster
,
M.
(
1975
).
Auditory systems of Heteromyidae: functional morphology and evolution of the middle ear
.
J. Morphol.
146
,
343
-
376
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

Supplementary information