We examined non-linear effects of the presence of one object on the electric image of another placed at the foveal region in Gymnotus omarorum. The sensory consequences of object mutual polarization on electric images were also depicted using behavioral procedures. Image measurements show that objects whose electric image is not detectable may modify the electric image of another placed closer to the fish and suggest that detection range and discrimination parameters used for one object may be affected when the presence of others enriches the scene. Behavioral experiments confirm that these changes in object images resulting from mutual polarization may be exploited for improving perception. While conductive objects close to the skin allow the fish to detect other objects placed out of the active electrodetection range, non-conductive objects may hide objects that otherwise show clear electric images. This suggests that fish movements may orient the self-generated field to exploit object mutual polarization, increasing or decreasing the active electrolocation range. In addition, images of a nearby object may be modulated by the presence of another object placed outside the detection range and the corresponding behavioral responses suggest that a moving or impedance-changing context may modify a fish’s discrimination abilities for closer objects.

Electric fish detect, discriminate and perhaps recognize objects using active electroreception. These animals create an electric field by activating an electric organ (EO) (Bennett, 1971; Caputi, 2011). The characteristics of the field created by this discharge (EOD) depend on the degree of polarization of the elements of the scene – which in turn depend on objects’ impedance in relation to the conductivity of the water and the fish – as well as on the relative position of the elements, and the field geometry (Lissmann and Machin, 1958; Nelson and MacIver, 2006; Caputi et al., 1998; Budelli et al., 2002; Nelson and MacIver, 1999; Nelson, 2005; Chen et al., 2005; Caputi and Budelli, 2006; Snyder et al., 2007; Pereira and Caputi, 2010).

Theoretical analyses suggest that objects with a conductance different from water are polarized by electric fields in such a way that they behave as a virtual electric source (Lissmann and Machin, 1958). Because of this theoretical argument they can be replaced by an electric source having an electromotive force whose time course is a function of the local field in the absence of the object. This source has been called the ‘object’s stamp in a given scene’ (Caputi et al., 2008; Pereira and Caputi, 2010). The corollary of the concept of ‘stamp’ is that the presence of every polarized object may also polarize the rest of the objects in the scene with a ponderance inversely dependent on a power function of the distance between the objects. This concept of electric image formation provides some suggestions about how object images might be modified by the presence of other objects in the scene (Sicardi et al., 2000; Rother et al., 2003; Chen et al., 2005; Caputi and Budelli, 2006; Nelson and MacIver, 2006) and how clues that objects offer to an active electrolocating agent vary with context (Pereira et al., 2007; Pereira and Caputi, 2010; Sanguinetti et al., 2011).

The stamp of one object depends on the sum of fields of two different kinds: primary fields directly created by the fish and secondary fields resulting from the polarization of the elements of the scene.

A minimal scene consists of the fish (i.e. the first object affecting the field) and a second object. Analysis of this type of scene show important effects of geometry and conductance of the fish’s body on the spatial profile and amplitude of the image (Migliaro et al., 2005). Theoretical predictions and experimental results indicate that the image of that second object is modified by the presence of a third object (besides the object generating the image and the fish’s body). Modeling analysis of this type of scene predicted that: (a) the presence of a third object in a scene may cause a change in the clues that another object offers to the agent (Rother et al., 2003; Chen et al., 2005) and (b) contextual objects may contribute with parallax references for object active electrosensing (Babineau et al., 2006; Babineau et al., 2007). However, these studies focused mainly on the side of the fish and not on the foveal region, where the electroreceptor density and central representation are more important (Castelló et al., 2000). In this region, experimental results indicate that a third object may affect the fish’s body stamp and also the stamp of the second object besides the fish’s body (as is the case of a fish inside a tube) (Pereira et al., 2005).

In this study, we examined the effects of the presence of a third object on the electric image of another placed at the foveal region in Gymnotus omarorum (Richer de Forges et al., 2009), a pulse-emitting gymnotiform fish. The sensory meaning of these observations on electric images was also behaviorally explored using the novelty response. In a previous study we showed that the amplitude of the novelty response scales with the amplitude of the electric image and this allowed us to use it here for the assessment of the sensory effects of object interplay (Caputi et al., 2003). Physical measurements also showed that objects that otherwise are out of the detection range can be detected if another conductive object is intercalated between the distant object and the fish. In addition, non-conductive objects hide other objects that otherwise show clear electric images. Moreover, they show that distant objects out of the detection range may modify the electric image of an object introduced into the scene, closer to the fish. Behavioral experiments showed that G. omarorum sense these effects as a result of object interplay.

Animals

Experiments were performed in 15 fish (15–30 cm total length) following the guidelines of the CHEA (Comisión Honoraria de Experimentación Animal, ordinance 4332-99, Universidad de la República Oriental del Uruguay). Experiments were approved by the Animal Ethics Committee of the Instituto de Investigaciones Biológicas Clemente Estable (protocol number 001/03/2011). Fish were captured at Laguna del Cisne (Maldonado, Uruguay), 1–4 months before the experiment, kept in individual aquaria under a natural light cycle and fed with insect larvae. In those experiments in which the fish could experience pain or discomfort, animals were anesthetized with pentobarbital (0.5–1 mg, i.m.), repeated on demand until we reached and maintained the EOD rate below 10 Hz at 20°C and a slow but stable respiration. At the end of these experiments animals were killed by pentobarbital (10 mg, i.m.).

Image measurements

The experimental strategy was to compare the images of one object in either the presence or the absence of another similar nearby object. Images were measured in anesthetized fish with the fish’s body straight, halfway between the bottom and the water surface in a 33×48 cm tank filled with 100±10 μS cm–1 water to a depth of 10 cm.

In order to keep the fish’s body straight, we implanted a fine cotton thread along the midline between the muscular masses and above the spine using a long steel needle. This procedure was conducted when the fish reached the anesthetic plane where the EOD rate was unresponsive to visual, vibratory, electric or nociceptive stimuli. The thread came out of the body just behind the occiput and at the limit between the caudal and the center-caudal quarter of the fish length. The ends of the thread were firmly attached to two wooden poles vertically attached to an iron framework. The diameter of the rostral pole (1 cm) was reduced to 3 mm below the level of the water. The diameter of the caudal pole was 1 cm. The rostral pole was placed just above the occiput (Fig. 1A).

A nichrome wire (100 μm diameter) coated except on a knot tied at its center was implanted subcutaneously. The wire entered through one side below the mandible and left symmetrically through the other side. After wire insertion we verified that the knot was at the center of the mandible; the two wire tips were bent up and twisted together above the head forming a ring. The two ends of this reference electrode were connected together to one input of a differential amplifier. The exploring electrode (a 150 μm enamel-coated tungsten electrode with a blunted bare tip) was placed on the skin at different points along a curve on a horizontal plane following the shape of the mandible. This electrode was connected to the other input of the differential amplifier (Caputi et al., 2011). We considered that the drop of voltage between the exploring and reference electrode was a good estimator of the transcutaneous voltage at the fovea.

Recording of electric images was achieved by placing the objects at steady positions and moving the exploring electrode along a series of points on the skin at the foveal region with a resolution of 100–200 μm. The tip of the electrode was moved step by step along a series of previously defined points using a computer-controlled XY plotter (HP 7015A, Hewlett-Packard Corp.). Objects were similar to those previously described for analyzing fish discrimination ability (von der Emde, 1990; von der Emde and Ronacher, 1994; Caputi et al., 2003; Aguilera and Caputi, 2003; Engelmann et al., 2009). We chose this kind of object because they are also suitable for discrimination studies (Caputi et al., 2003). They consist of plastic cylinders having carbon plugs at each end. The carbon bases were connected through wires coming out of the tank and connected outside to a desired resistor. As conductive bases can be connected through an external impedance, objects have anisotropic impedances (infinite lateral, controllable longitudinal). The Appendix provides a more complete justification of object selection.

The electric image was estimated as follows. First we calculated the mean value of all recordings performed at each recording point and generated an average curve. Second, we calculated the electric image as the convolution of the average curve with a median filter (5 points). Thus, each point of the electric image profile was estimated as the median of the surrounding five points of the average curve.

We performed three series of experiments. In the first and second series we used two identical objects (each 7 mm diameter, 11 mm height; as a reference a similar water cylinder has 35 μS of longitudinal conductance) aligned along the midline, one facing the skin 1 mm away and the other placed with its front end 13 mm away (1 mm apart from the other). In the third series of experiments we used two thinner objects (4 mm diameter) of two different lengths (7 and 14 mm). Cylinders were aligned, separated by 1 mm, following a field line (determined for each object in the absence of the other as the angle that yields the maximum drop of voltage between the bases). Water cylinders equivalent to 4 mm objects had different longitudinal resistance (7 and 14 mm lengths correspond to 22 and 11 μS, respectively).

In the first series of experiments the bases of each object were connected through resistors of 2.5 MΩ or 1 kΩ. We used the short cuts ‘open circuit’ for 2.5 MΩ and ‘short circuit’ for 1 kΩ because these resistors were resistive or conductive enough to provoke the same electric image as an ‘open’ or a ‘short’ circuit. The possible combinations of these loads resulted in eight possible experiments, four performed with both objects (open–open, open–short, short–open, short–short) and four control experiments in the absence of one of the objects.

In the second and third series of experiments, connections between the bases of one of the objects were made by either 2.5 MΩ or 1 kΩ resistors while the resistance between the bases of the other object was set to several possible values (ranging between 0.3 and 2500 kΩ).

Signals were digitized at a minimum of 20 kHz per channel and amplified enough to have at least 12 bit resolution (AM systems-1800, 10–10,000 Hz band pass; Carlsborg, WA, USA). Data acquisition was made in epochs of 550–700 ms, starting 100 ms after the electrode movement ceased. Four channels were recorded in each experiment: (a) the head to tail EOD, recorded between two electrodes placed on the main axis of the fish at opposite ends of the tank; (b) the local field (LEOD); and (c) the X- and Y-positions of the image-measuring active electrode in the horizontal plane. We made 3–5 runs of the electrode along the same trajectory. For each of these runs, we obtained several (between 5 and 10) LEODs per recording position. LEOD waveforms were time averaged with reference to a fixed moment of the head to tail EOD. Thus, for each position we got 3–5 averaged LEOD waveforms.

Behavioral experiments

Novelty responses – characterized as transient acceleration just after the presentation of a new object or a change in the object impedance – proved to be a good index of object detection (Caputi et al., 2003). In addition, we have shown that the maximal reduction in EOD interval (i.e. the amplitude of the novelty response) is a good indicator of a change in the local image (Caputi et al., 2003; Pereira et al., 2007). Here, we used novelty responses as indicators of detection of an otherwise non-detected object (response vs non-response) and also to evaluate whether the image of one object was changed by the presence of the other (comparing the amplitude of the response).

Fish were kept in a tank containing water at 100±10 μS cm–1 (33×48 cm tank filled to a depth of 10 cm), restrained inside a nylon mesh, tightly attached to wood poles fixed to the smallest opposite walls of the tank. Two electrodes placed near the wooden supporting poles and connected to a differential amplifier (high input impedance, ×100, 10 Hz to 10 kHz band-pass range) were used to record the head to tail field generated by the EOD. In five fish we used two pairs of exploring objects consisting of plastic cylinders having carbon conductive bases as described above. Both pairs of objects were the same as those used for image measurements. In two other fish we explored the effects of a cube (either copper or plastic, either behind or intercalated between the object and the fish’s skin, 9 mm side) on the novelty response evoked by a step from open to short circuit of the connection between the bases of one of the cylinders described above (7 mm diameter × 11 mm length). Objects were manipulated by attached wood handles (1 mm width) that when soaked with water had negligible images (Gómez et al., 2004). In these experiments different longitudinal resistances were used.

Novelty responses were elicited by a step reduction of the cylinder longitudinal resistance over a period of 2 s every 30 s. The same fish was exposed from a minimum of 10 trials under the same conditions to a maximum of 30 trials depending on the variability of the responses. The 28 s interval between changes in object impedance was enough to avoid habituation as shown by testing the lack of difference of the responses between the first and last trial of the series (difference between medians different from zero, sign test, P<0.01, N=10). The amplitude of each novelty response was calculated as the difference between the basal interval (I0), defined as the mean of the five intervals immediately previous to the resistance step, and the minimum interval value (Im, in general the 2nd or 3rd after the beginning of the resistance step), divided by the basal interval:

We tested the hypothesis that the presence of a third object in a scene may cause a change in the amplitude of the novelty response in five ways: (1) by eliciting novelty responses in the presence and absence of a cube either with the cube between the test object and the fish (N=2 copper cube, N=2 plastic cube) or with the test object between the cube and the fish fovea; (2) by eliciting novelty responses by open to short changes in longitudinal conductance of one of the large objects (7 mm diameter, 11 mm length) in the presence or absence of a twin object aligned along the same field lines – we compared the mean amplitudes of the novelty responses evoked by maximal steps in resistance of the nearest object with the farthest object either in open or short circuit; (3) by studying the amplitude of the novelty response evoked by the change of the nearest object conductance from open circuit to a given predetermined value when the other object was either in conductive or non-conductive conditions; (4) by studying the amplitude of the novelty response evoked by the change in conductance from open to short circuit of a small object (4 mm diameter and 7 mm length) when the conductance of the farthest object (4 mm diameter and 14 mm length) was controlled (statistical parameters of the amplitudes of the evoked novelty responses were calculated); (5) by studying the amplitude of the novelty response evoked by the change in conductance from open to short circuit of the farthest object (4 mm diameter and 14 mm length) conductance when the conductance of the nearest object (4 mm diameter and 11 mm length) was controlled.

Statistical analysis of image differences

To validate the results across fish, we compared the peak amplitudes of the electric image in each recording condition using the sign test. We tested the following hypotheses: (a) images generated by the nearest conductive object in the presence of a farther away conductive object were smaller than or equal to the image of the nearest object alone; (b) images generated by the nearest conductive object in the presence of a farther away non-conductive object were larger than or equal to the image of the nearest object alone; (c) images generated by the nearest non-conductive object in the presence of a farther away object being either non-conductive or conductive were not equal to the image of the nearest object alone. In the first two cases, the results always opposed the hypothesis; thus, we estimated the probability of error as (1/2)5.

We validated the results of the behavioral analysis across fish in the following manner. First we tested the distribution of the novelty response for normality (Kolmogorov–Smirnoff test P<0.05). Second, we compared the amplitude of the novelty responses for each fish using a t-test for independent samples in each pair of conditions: (i) amplitude of the novelty response evoked by a maximum step in the nearest object with a 1000 μS conductive farthest object vs amplitude of the novelty response evoked by a maximum step in the nearest object with a 0.4 μS non-conductive farthest object; (ii) amplitude of the novelty response evoked by a maximum step in the farthest object with a 1000 μS conductive nearest object vs amplitude of the novelty response evoked by a maximum step in the farthest object with a 35 μS ‘water-conductive’ nearest object (nearest objects of 0.4 μS completely abolished novelty responses). Third, we tested the null hypothesis that the mean amplitudes of the novelty responses in cases where the context object was 1000 μS conductive in experiments of types i or ii were smaller than or equal to those in the other contexts, and calculated the type I error (αper comparison) in each case. Then we evaluated the family-wise error rate [1–Π(1–αper comparison)], which was 0.03 and 0.04, respectively.

We studied the effect of the conductance of one of the objects on the electric image in different scene configurations. In order to be able to change object impedance along the object dimension without changing the object geometry, we used pairs of non-isotropic objects consisting of cylinders having non-conductive sides and conductive bases aligned with the field lines.

Fig. 2 shows the images of the two objects together (green, red, magenta and cyan) compared with the images of only one of them (black, blue and brown). Both cylinders were placed with their main axes aligned with the fish axis and the field lines. The base of one of the cylinders was 1 mm away from the skin and the distance between cylinders was also 1 mm.

When the conductive bases of a test object closer to the fish were short circuited (green, black and red lines), the object funneled the current through its axis, creating an upward bell-shaped image as described above. The image resulting from the presence of the closest object alone (black line) was altered by the introduction of the second object. When the farthest object was non-conductive (i.e. open circuit between its bases), the image of the nearest object decreased (Fig. 2, red line). The peak image in the presence of the farthest object was always smaller than the control without the farthest object at the same site in all 5 fish [sign test, P=(1/2)5, N=5 fish]. When the farthest object was conductive (i.e. short circuit between its bases), the image of the nearest object always increased (Fig. 2, green line). The peak image in the presence of the farthest object was always larger than the control without the farthest object at the same site in all 5 fish [sign test, P=(1/2)5, N=5 fish]. It is important to note that the farthest object alone did not generate images even in the short circuit condition (Fig. 2, brown line).

When the conductive bases of a test object closer to the fish were disconnected, the object blocked the current through its axis, creating a downward bell-shaped image. The image obtained with the closest object alone (blue line) was not altered by the presence of the second cylindrical object, independent of its conductive or non-conductive condition (Fig. 2, compare blue, magenta and cyan lines, Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, P=0.625 and P=0.625, respectively, N=5 fish). When the farthest object was conductive (i.e. short circuit between its bases), the image of the nearest object always increased.

These results show a contextual effect of an object placed beyond the electroreceptive range implying an increase of the electroreceptive range by the interposition of a third object between the fish and another object.

Image amplitude as a function of the impedance of the farthest object

In order to pinpoint contextual effects, we explored the image amplitude of a close very conductive test cylinder (1 kΩ) when changing the conductance of a farther away cylinder. The example in Fig. 3 shows the amplitude at the center of the image as a function of the longitudinal conductance of the context object when it was placed 1 mm behind the closest object. The axes of both objects were aligned with the local field. The introduction into the scene of a farthest object causes a mild relative increment in the amplitude of the image generated by the closest object alone. This increment was either positive (farther objects more conductive than water) or negative (farther objects less conductive than water). In all fish, this change depended in a linear manner on the impedance of the farthest object.

In addition, the change of the image caused by a maximal step in conductance of the nearest object from open to short circuit was larger when the farthest object was in the short circuit condition than when it was in the open circuit condition (median values 0.22 and 0.12, respectively, Wilcoxon rank sum test, P=0.008, N=5), which indicates that there was a relative increase in image amplitude when the impedance of the nearest object was changed from open to short circuit.

Image amplitude as a function of the impedance of the closest object

Data from Fig. 1 also indicate that objects unable to yield images when placed alone in front of the fish may cause changes in the pattern of transcutaneous self-generated profiles when a conductive object is interposed between them and the fish. To explore the effects of the impedance of the interposed object on the image of the farthest object, we measured the changes in image amplitude when changing the object’s conductance in the presence of the context cylinder placed outside the detection range for single objects. We compared two conditions of the farthest object (1 kΩ and 2.5 MΩ). Fig. 4 shows the amplitude at the center of the image as a function of the longitudinal conductance of the intercalated object.

We found a sigmoidal relationship [Boltzmann fitting function y=A2+(A1A2)/(1+ek[log(x/xo)]]. The maxima (A2) obtained from the 5 fish in the short circuit conditions of the contextual cylinder were significantly larger than those in the open circuit conditions [sign test, P=(1/2)5, N=5 fish]. We also observed that in the open circuit condition the slope was slightly shifted to smaller values and the midpoint (xo) was shifted to a larger conductivity value. It is important to note that the differences in the curves were larger than 5% only for very conductive intercalated objects.

Behavioral evidence shows sensory consequences of object interplay

We studied the sensory consequence of the changes of the physical images caused by the presence of another object by using the novelty responses evoked by sudden reductions in object resistance. These responses proved to be a good index of object detection (Caputi et al., 2003). In addition, we have shown that the maximal reduction in EOD interval (i.e. the amplitude of the novelty response) is a good indicator of the change in the local image (Caputi et al., 2003; Pereira et al., 2007). Here, we used novelty responses as indicators of detection of an otherwise non-detected object (response vs non-response) and also to evaluate whether the image of one object is changed by the presence of the other (comparing the amplitude of the response).

When the test object was placed at a distance of 1 mm from the skin and a copper cube was placed 2 mm away from its distal end (Fig. 5A, inset), the amplitude of the novelty response increased by 25% compared with the control (Fig. 5A, unpaired samples t-test, P<0.01, d.f.=18). A plastic cube instead of the copper one caused a significant reduction (34%) of the amplitude of the novelty response compared with the control (Fig. 5A, unpaired samples t-test, P<0.01, d.f.=18).

When a test object was placed 23 mm from the skin, the maximal change in resistance did not a evoke novelty response (Fig. 5B, open symbols). When a copper cube was placed between the object and the skin (Fig. 5B, inset), the maximal change in resistance of the object provoked a very clear novelty response (Fig. 5B, filled symbols). This indicates that an object beyond the detectable range is detected by the interposition of the copper cube.

To pinpoint the effects of object interplay on the ability to sense the change in impedance of an object, we performed three other series of experiments in 5 fish each.

In the first series we compared extreme responses across fish. These responses were obtained with a non-conductive farthest object (2.5 MΩ resistance, corresponding to 0.4 μS conductance) and a very conductive farthest object (1 kΩ, corresponding to 1000 μS). Statistical analysis confirmed our prediction that the responsiveness of the fish to the step in conductance of the nearest object is context dependent (Table 1). Novelty responses were always larger for farther away objects loaded by a smaller resistor (Fig. 6A).

In the second series we studied the amplitude of the novelty response as a function of the step amplitude in the conductance of the nearest object (Fig. 6B). As in the previous experiment, we compared the amplitude of the novelty responses evoked when the farthest object was in either the non-conductive or very conductive condition. The amplitude of the novelty response increased with the step in conductance of the nearest cylinder. In every fish, the curve obtained in the non-conductive context (Fig. 6B, 2.5 MΩ) was below that obtained in the conductive context (Fig. 6B, 1 kΩ).

Using two objects of smaller diameter that have a larger effect on the image, we compared the amplitude of the novelty response provoked by a maximal step (from non-conductive to very conductive) of the nearest object as a function of the change in conductance of the farthest context object (Fig. 7). We found in 5 out of 5 fish that the mean amplitude of the novelty response evoked by a maximal change in conductance of the nearest object was an increasing function of the conductance of the farthest object. Moreover, increasing the resistance of the nearest object caused a shift of the curve to lower values (Fig. 7B).

Finally, we evaluated the funneling or blocking effects of an object intercalated between a test object and the skin by loading the intercalated object with different resistors. For thick objects (7 mm diameter × 11 mm length) we only obtained novelty responses for very conductive conditions of the near object (loading resistors 1 and 10 kΩ, corresponding to 1000 and 100 μS, respectively). Thus, to find the relationship we used narrower cylinders (4 mm diameter) of different lengths (nearest object 7 mm, farthest object 14 mm). Under these conditions, the amplitude of the novelty response evoked by a large conductance step at the farthest object (between 0.4 and 1000 μS) depended on the longitudinal conductance of the interposed object in all 5 fish (Fig. 8) increasing gradually as the conductance increased. In all fish, novelty responses were abolished by a loading resistance of the nearest object of above 500 kΩ (equivalent to 5 μS, Fig. 8, data from 2 of the studied fish), suggesting a blocking effect of the non-conductive objects.

To validate these effects among fish we choose two conductance values (short circuit, 1000 and 35 μS) to compare mean amplitudes of the novelty responses. For every fish the mean amplitude of the novelty response at 1000 μS was larger than the mean amplitude of the novelty response at 35 μS. Statistical analysis (Table 2) suggests a facilitation of the novelty response by the current funneling through the nearest object conductance.

Theoretical predictions (Rother et al., 2003; Migliaro et al., 2005; Caputi and Budelli, 2006; Pereira and Caputi, 2010) indicate that the presence of one object modifies the field in which it is placed. Thus, the field polarizing an object potentially introduced into the scene is dependent on the presence of neighboring objects. This was clearly shown by modeling in the study by Migliaro et al. when considering the presence of the fish’s body as an object (Migliaro et al., 2005) and in our previous analytical studies (Pereira and Caputi, 2010).

Here, we experimentally confirm predictions made by Rother et al. (Rother et al., 2003) that when one object is intercalated between another object and the fish’s body, there is mutual polarization and, consequently, the image is modified in a non-additive manner. If the two objects are very close (according to our results, ≤2 mm apart) their mutual polarization results in changes of the image of a nearby object even though the farthest object alone is unable to yield a distinguishable image (see Figs 1 and 2).

This result has two implications. First, the behavioral results confirm that object mutual polarization has significant effects on the detectability of objects. Single objects have a range of detection of about the size of the object (Pereira and Caputi, 2010). An object placed beyond such a distance is not detectable when it is alone but its movements or its changes in impedance can be detected if a conductive nearby object is intercalated between the first object and the fish’s body. The opposite (hiding of an object that is within the active electrolocation range) occurs if a non-conductive object is interposed between the object and the fish.

In addition, the presence of a far contextual object may also increase or decrease by small amounts the image of objects more conductive than water (see Figs 3 and 4), affecting object perception. This prediction was confirmed by showing that the function relating the amplitude of the novelty response to the increase in conductance of the closest object changed in the presence of non-conductive and very conductive farthest objects. In fact, although the novelty response’s maximum change was only 10%, in all paired experiments there was a systematic difference in the amplitude range and slope of the curves when comparing the two plots (see Fig. 6B). Moreover, the responses evoked by maximum conductance changes were smoothly modulated by the impedance of the farthest object.

These effects of mutual polarization may be interpreted by a sensory agent in an ambiguous way. On the one hand, the farthest object increases the impedance discriminability of the closest object; on the other hand, the closest object modifies the range of electrosensory detection as a function of its impedance, shape and size. This ambiguity may be crucial for object recognition. Nevertheless, an important clue could be the presence of movements of the fish or object. As stationary images are removed by the presence of an adaptive filter, probably at the electrosensory lobe (Caputi et al., 2003; Caputi et al., 2008), a fish may take advantage of relative movements in two ways: (1) use its own movements to maintain a closer conductive object stationary, working as a ‘telescopic device’ that artificially extends the fish’s body and consequently increases the detection range; and (2) to better discriminate the attributes of a closer object in the presence of a moving context behind the object (as for example leaves and roots of water plants) (Babineau et al., 2007; Behr et al., 2011).

Our experiments confirm the proposed short range of active electroreception and the importance of the fish’s body for active electrolocation (Migliaro et al., 2005; Pereira and Caputi, 2010; Caputi et al., 2011). Objects near the fish may sometimes act as ‘accidental auxiliary devices’ for perceiving other objects beyond the active electroreception range. The potential effects of a fish’s movements on scene polarization – thereby altering objects’ mutual polarization – is analogous to exploring an object by pressing it between the finger tip and a background object. It is virtually impossible to evaluate the consistency of a rubber eraser floating in water, but is very easy when it is placed on a table. This is because the buoyancy caused by sinking the object is much smaller and behaves differently from the reaction force of the table. We have previously reported some similarities between active electrolocation and haptic senses (Caputi et al., 2011). This is another one.

Reasons for using cylindrical probes and the choice of their dimensions

As we have already reported (Pereira and Caputi, 2010), every object modulates the basal field (the field in the absence of the object) as if a set of electric sources were placed in it instead of the object. This set is called the stamp of the object. The difference between the field with the object and the basal field is called the object perturbing field (Lissmann and Machin, 1958). The electric image is a transcutaneous object perturbing field. For objects equally located relative to the fish it holds that larger stamps generate larger images.

The stamp of an object depends on its imprimence, a parameter that depends on object size, shape and impedance. Therefore, only very simple objects are suitable for manipulation as experimental tools – among these are spheres and the cylindrical probes used in this study. These probes only conduct current in the longitudinal direction. When they are aligned to the local field their stamp has the same orientation as the field. In addition, the longitudinal conductance can be controlled at will by connecting different resistors between the bases. Objects more conductive than water facilitate the flow of current while those less conductive than water reduce the flow of current, causing an increase or a reduction of the electric image (Fig. A1A). The stamp of the object (S) is proportional to the difference between the object resistance (Ro) and the resistance of an equal-sized cylinder of water (Rw). The proportionality constant depends on the scene parameters ‘as seen’ from the object: the electromotive force (Es) and the internal resistance (Rs), and also on Ro:

Shape affects S in two ways: a doubling of diameter reduces by four times the Ro that matches Rw (compare green and blue lines in Fig. A1B). A doubling of cylinder length increases by two times the Ro value that matches Rw and increases also Es (compare red and blue lines in Fig. A1B). Bearing in mind that the electrical significance of the object loading resistance (Ro) is different for every shape, we carried out two different series of experiments; in the first we used identical objects and in the second we used thinner objects of different length to amplify the effects. Here, we only wanted to show a qualitative effect, so we did not perform a detailed study of the effect of the shape of the objects in object interplay.

We would like to thank Lic. Marcela Piffaretti for English correction of a first draft.

FUNDING

This work was partially funded by European Commission Information, Society and Media Future and Emergent Technologies (FET) [grant no. 231845] and Programa de Desarrollo de las Ciencias Básicas (PEDEClBA) (Uruguay).

Aguilera
P. A.
,
Caputi
A. A.
(
2003
).
Electroreception in G. carapo: detection of changes in waveform of the electrosensory signals
.
J. Exp. Biol.
206
,
989
998
.
Babineau
D.
,
Longtin
A.
,
Lewis
J. E.
(
2006
).
Modeling the electric field of weakly electric fish
.
J. Exp. Biol.
209
,
3636
3651
.
Babineau
D.
,
Lewis
J. E.
,
Longtin
A.
(
2007
).
Spatial acuity and prey detection in weakly electric fish
.
PLoS Comput. Biol.
3
,
e38
.
Behr
K.
,
Neusel
G.
,
von der Emde
G.
(
2011
).
Mind the gap! Detection of gaps and locomotor strategies of electrolocating Gnathonemus petersii
.
Proceedings of the 104th Annual Meeting of the Deutsche Zoologische Gesellschaft (DZG)
. p
26
.
Bennett
M. V. L.
(
1971
).
Electric organs
. In
Fish Physiology
, Vol.
5
(ed.
Hoar
W. S.
,
Randall
D. J.
), pp.
347
491
.
London
:
Academic Press
.
Budelli
R.
,
Caputi
A.
,
Gomez
L.
,
Rother
D.
,
Grant
K.
(
2002
).
The electric image in Gnathonemus petersii
.
J. Physiol. Paris
96
,
421
429
.
Caputi
A. A.
(
2011
).
Encyclopedia of Fish Physiology
(ed.
Farrell
A.
).
Amsterdam
:
Elsevier
.
Caputi
A. A.
,
Budelli
R.
(
2006
).
Peripheral electrosensory imaging by weakly electric fish
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
192
,
587
600
.
Caputi
A.
,
Budelli
R.
,
Grant
K.
,
Bell
C. C.
(
1998
).
The electric image in weakly electric fish: physical images of resistive objects in Gnathonemus petersii
.
J. Exp. Biol.
201
,
2115
2128
.
Caputi
A. A.
,
Aguilera
P. A.
,
Castelló
M. E.
(
2003
).
Probability and amplitude of novelty responses as a function of the change in contrast of the reafferent image in G. carapo
.
J. Exp. Biol.
206
,
999
1010
.
Caputi
A. A.
,
Castelló
M. E.
,
Aguilera
P. A.
,
Pereira
A. C.
,
Nogueira
J.
,
Rodríguez-Cattaneo
A.
,
Lezcano
C.
(
2008
).
Active electroreception in Gymnotus omari: imaging, object discrimination, and early processing of actively generated signals
.
J. Physiol. Paris
102
,
256
271
.
Caputi
A.
,
Aguilera
P.
,
Pereira
A. C.
(
2011
).
Active electric imaging: body-object interplay and object’s ‘electric texture’
.
PLoS ONE
6
,
e2279
.
Castelló
M. E.
,
Aguilera
P. A.
,
Trujillo-Cenóz
O.
,
Caputi
A. A.
(
2000
).
Electroreception in Gymnotus carapo: pre-receptor processing and the distribution of electroreceptor types
.
J. Exp. Biol.
203
,
3279
3287
.
Chen
L.
,
House
J. H.
,
Krahe
R.
,
Nelson
M. E.
(
2005
).
Modeling signal and background components of electrosensory scenes
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
191
,
331
345
.
Engelmann
J.
,
Baceló
J.
,
Metzen
M.
,
Pusch
R.
,
Bouton
B.
,
Migliaro
A.
,
Caputi
A.
,
Budelli
R.
,
Grant
K.
,
von der Emde
G.
(
2008
).
Electric imaging through active electrolocation: implication for the analysis of complex scenes
.
Biol. Cybern.
98
,
519
539
.
Gómez
L.
,
Budelli
R.
,
Grant
K.
,
Caputi
A. A.
(
2004
).
Pre-receptor profile of sensory images and primary afferent neuronal representation in the mormyrid electrosensory system
.
J. Exp. Biol.
207
,
2443
2453
.
Lissmann
H. W.
,
Machin
K. E.
(
1958
).
The mechanism of object location in Gymnarchus niloticus and similar fish
.
J. Exp. Biol.
35
,
451
486
.
Migliaro
A.
,
Caputi
A. A.
,
Budelli
R.
(
2005
).
Theoretical analysis of pre-receptor image conditioning in weakly electric fish
.
PLoS Comput. Biol.
1
,
e:16
.
Nelson
M. E.
(
2005
).
Target detection, image analysis, and modeling in electroreception
. In
Handbook of Auditory Research
, Vol.
21
(ed.
Bullock
T. H.
,
Hopkins
C. D.
,
Popper
A. N.
,
Fay
R.
), pp.
290
317
.
Springer
.
Nelson
M. E.
,
MacIver
M. A.
(
1999
).
Prey capture in the weakly electric fish Apteronotus albifrons: sensory acquisition strategies and electrosensory consequences
.
J. Exp. Biol.
202
,
1195
1203
.
Nelson
M. E.
,
MacIver
M. A.
(
2006
).
Sensory acquisition in active sensing system
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
192
,
573
586
.
Pereira
A. C.
,
Caputi
A. A.
(
2010
).
Imaging in electrosensory systems
.
Interdiscip. Sci.
2
,
291
307
.
Pereira
A. C.
,
Centurión
V.
,
Caputi
A. A.
(
2005
).
Contextual effects of small environments on the electric images of objects and their brain evoked responses in weakly electric fish
.
J. Exp. Biol.
208
,
961
972
.
Richer de Forges
M.
,
Crampton
W. G. R.
,
Albert
J. S.
(
2009
).
A new species of Gymnotus (Gymnotiformes, Gymnotidae) from Uruguay: description of a model species in neurophysiological research
.
Copeia
2009
,
538
544
.
Rother
D.
,
Migliaro
A.
,
Canetti
R.
,
Gómez
L.
,
Caputi
A.
,
Budelli
R.
(
2003
).
Electric images of two low resistance objects in weakly electric fish
.
Biosystems
71
,
169
177
.
Sanguinetti-Scheck
J. I.
,
Pedraja
E. F.
,
Cilleruelo
E.
,
Migliaro
A.
,
Aguilera
P.
,
Caputi
A. A.
,
Budelli
R.
(
2011
).
Fish geometry and electric organ discharge determine functional organization of the electrosensory epithelium
.
PLoS ONE
6
:
e27470
.
Sicardi
A.
,
Caputi
A. A.
,
Budelli
R.
(
2000
).
Physical basis of distance discrimination in weakly electric fish
.
Physica A
283
,
86
93
.
Snyder
J. B.
,
Nelson
M. E.
,
Burdick
J. W.
,
MacIver
M. A.
(
2007
).
Omnidirectional sensory and motor volumes in electric fish
.
PLoS Biol.
5
,
e301
.
von der Emde
G.
(
1990
).
Discrimination of objects through electrolocation in the weakly electric fish, Gnathonemus petersii
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
167
,
413
421
.
von der Emde
G.
,
Ronacher
B.
(
1994
).
Perception of electric properties of objects in electrolocating weakly electric fish: two-dimensional similarity scaling reveals a City-Block metric
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
175
,
801
812
.