SUMMARY
Visually guided animals depend heavily on the quality of visual signals in order to obtain functionally relevant information about their environment. To support visual information processing, nature has evolved a large variety of physiological adaptations and behavioral strategies such as compensatory head movements. During self-movement, head rotations compensate for changes in body attitude in order to stabilize gaze. However, how walking animals cope with uneven structured substrates, which may affect body and gaze orientation, is still unknown. We used stereo high-speed video to analyze compensatory head movements of blowflies walking freely on differently structured substrates. We found that even a pronounced asperity of the ground structure, with bumps of almost the size of the animal, was largely compensated by the walking apparatus of the blowfly, which leads to body roll and pitch movements only marginally larger than those on flat substrate. Pitch and roll fluctuations of the head were smaller compared with body fluctuations on all tested substrates, emphasizing the significance of gaze stabilization during walking on structured substrates. Furthermore, we found no impairment in head and body stabilization during walks in darkness, which indicates that the control system mediating compensatory head movements works well without any visual input. Interestingly, blowflies changed their walking style in the dark and seemed to use their forelegs as tactile probes.
INTRODUCTION
Locomotion in a three-dimensional (3-D) world causes complex retinal image shifts. These may be strongly affected by involuntary eye displacements induced by the properties of the locomotor apparatus. For instance, leg and body kinematics of humans even during walking on flat ground would cause periodic vertical head and eye displacements and, accordingly, vertical retinal image shifts, if the head and eyes were tightly coupled to the body (Grossman et al., 1988; Pozzo et al., 1990; Hirasaki et al., 1999). Such vertical image shifts would complicate object fixation and extraction of pattern details of the surrounding environment. Another example is the wing stroke of flying animals, which also produces involuntary head and eye displacements (van Hateren and Schilstra, 1999a). Moreover, for kinematic reasons, many fast-flying animals have to perform banked turns by rolling their body to the side while rotating around their transverse body axis (pitching) towards the desired direction. Flying blowflies may reach roll angles of 90 deg during large banked turns (Wagner, 1986; van Hateren and Schilstra, 1999a; van Hateren and Schilstra, 1999b; Geurten et al., 2010). If their head and eyes were tightly coupled to their body, the orientation of their visual system would rotate in space as well, complicating the processing of information about the external world.
To reduce these negative consequences of unintended locomotion-induced eye displacements, animals perform compensatory head and eye movements. These are mostly reflexive and countervail changes of the body position [head bobbing in birds (Necker, 2007)] and orientation in space. Thereby they stabilize gaze to enable, for instance, precise object fixation during locomotion (Dunlap and Mowrer, 1930; Land, 1973; Pozzo et al., 1990; Miles, 1997; van Hateren and Schilstra, 1999b).
Compensatory head and eye movements appear to be multimodally controlled. The visual system is thought to register unintended whole-field image shifts and initiates movements to reduce the retinal image slip (Wallmann and Letelier, 1993; Hengstenberg, 1993; Boeddeker and Hemmi, 2010). Concurrently, diverse mechanosensory systems sense self-movements – changes in body attitude and head orientation – to induce corrective head movements, usually in a different dynamic range than the visual system [primates (Waespe and Henn, 1987); birds (Zeigler and Bischof, 1993); insects (Hengstenberg, 1993)].
Walking animals have a special problem in keeping their gaze stable, because under natural conditions they frequently have to walk on structured and bumpy substrates. Kinematics of the walking apparatus and direct contact with the ground and its asperities may cause strong unintended body movements, which would degrade the stability of gaze. As insects cannot move their compound eyes independently of their head, they perform exclusively compensatory head movements. Tethered and freely walking blowflies were shown to perform compensatory head movements on flat ground to stabilize gaze (Horn and Lang, 1978; Hengstenberg, 1986; Blaj and van Hateren, 2004). However, it is still unclear to what extent walking insects are able to hold their gaze stable when walking on structured ground. Furthermore, because of the complex multimodal control of head movements, it is unknown to what extent the visual system is involved in controlling these movements.
To tackle these knowledge gaps of the control and operating range of gaze stabilization in insects, we analyzed compensatory head movements of freely walking blowflies. The blowfly Calliphora vicina Robineau-Desvoidy 1830 has served for many years as a model system for analyzing compensatory head movements and visual information processing (for reviews, see Land, 1999; Egelhaaf, 2006; Borst et al., 2010). Walking is an essential behavioral mode of blowflies, because they perform near-field food search behaviors as well as oviposition on ground (Dethier, 1976). To analyze compensatory head movements, we recorded head and body movements during straight walks on differently structured substrates (‘walking bars’). The surface of these walking bars varied from flat to coarsely structured, with bumps of up to nearly the size of the animal. To assess the role of the visual system for the control of compensatory head movements, we compared rotational head stability of flies walking in the dark and under bright lighting conditions.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Flies and preparation
We used female blowflies C. vicina from stocks reared in our laboratory. The flies were caught out of the stock cage 1 day post eclosion. To prevent them from flying, we briefly anaesthetized them with CO2 and placed a drop of beeswax on each wing base. We painted three marker points on their head and six points on their thorax using white, nontoxic acrylic paint (Hooby Line, C. Kreul, Hallerndorf, Germany), to allow automatic tracking of head as well as body position and orientation (Fig. 1A). Prior to final marker placement, we cut the longest hairs at the intended locations to optimize marker bonding to the cuticle. Before the first experiment, flies had at least 24 h in isolation to adapt to the wing fixation and marker placements.
Experimental setup
Filming setup
We recorded walking blowflies at 500 frames s–1 with two orthogonally arranged and synchronized infrared (IR)-sensitive high-speed cameras (CR 600, Optronis GmbH, Kehl, Germany), both equipped with DG MACRO 24–70 mm lenses (SIGMA GmbH, Roedermark, Germany). One camera was positioned 50 cm above the walking bar (top view). The other camera was positioned 38 cm in front of the bar behind a hole (4.2 cm diameter) in a white screen, so that the recorded flies walked directly towards it (frontal view). The walking bar was placed on a custom-made light table, which illuminated the scenery from below. In addition, we placed two LED panels (Marathon multiLED, VIDEAL, Niederoenz, Germany) 60 cm above the walking bar (Fig. 2A). A mirror (10×5 cm) was fixed to the right side of the walking bar at an angle of 45 deg, which provided a side view of the walking blowflies from the top camera. To avoid a potential bias on head or body orientation caused by the unilaterally positioned mirror, we placed a second, identical mirror along the left side of the walking bar.
For recordings in the dark, we replaced the multiLED light sources with two custom-made IR LED panels, each consisting of 70 single IR LEDs (GaAIAs Double Hetero, Vishay Electronic GmbH, Selb, Germany; peak emission at λ=890 nm). The IR LED panels emitted long-wave light far above the perceivable wavelengths of the blowfly’s photoreceptors [R1–R6 sensitive to light up to 600 nm; R7 and R8 sensitive to light up to 500 and 600 nm, respectively (Hardie, 1979)], but within the sensitivity range of our IR-sensitive high-speed cameras (sensitive up to 1000 nm). We placed the IR LED panels approximately 17 cm to the left and right above the walking bar. All potential additional light sources (e.g. the indication LEDs of the cameras), which might be used by the flies for orientation, were covered with black insulating tape. Only an indirect red light source (PF 712E, Philips Deutschland GmbH, Hamburg, Germany) was used to enable the experimenter to handle the blowflies in the experimental setup. To assess what perceivable light intensities were present in the walking setup, we measured the luminance on the walking bar with a luminance meter (Konica Minolta Sensing LS-100, Osaka, Japan). Note that although the luminance meter was adapted to the sensitivity of the human eye, it provides a rough estimate of the light perceived by the fly because of the similar spectral sensitivities of human vision and fly photoreceptors (Riggs et al., 1949; Hardie, 1979). In the dark condition, the luminance amounted to 0.01 cd m–2, i.e. more than five orders of magnitude below the luminance of the bright condition (4820 cd m–2).
Structured walking bars
The walking bars were made of gypsum, which was poured into milled aluminum molds under vacuum to prevent air pockets. We designed four mold forms with a custom script written in MATLAB (The MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) that was capable of producing computer-aided design files. All bars had a length of 12 cm and were 1.6 cm wide (Fig. 2B). The walking bars were narrow enough to force the flies to walk straight, but wide enough to prevent them from simultaneously grabbing the left and right edges of the bar. One bar had a flat surface and served as the control condition to estimate head and body movements induced exclusively by the walking apparatus. The other three walking bars had surfaces consisting of regularly placed convex hemispheres (‘bumps’). The diameters of the bumps defined the substrates’ structure. We term the flat surface ‘0 mm substrate’ and the others ‘3 mm substrate’, ‘6 mm substrate’ and ‘8 mm substrate’ (Fig. 2B, left to right). Each walking bar was mounted on a 10 cm high stand (Fig. 2A).
Experimental procedure
Flies were transferred from the adapting cage using transparent plastic vessels (2×6 cm, diameter×length) and released at the far end (as seen from the frontal camera) of the walking bar. Eighty percent of the released flies started walking right away. After the tested fly reached the opposite end of the walking bar we caught it with the vessel, examined the recorded walk and saved it on hard disk in case the walk satisfied certain criteria: (1) the fly had to walk continuously without interruption while traversing the depth of field segment of the frontal camera; (2) the fly had to walk on top of the substrate, so all six legs were placed on top of the walking bar and not on the sides at all times; (3) the fly performed at least three complete stride cycles of its tripod gait (Strauss and Heisenberg, 1990) in the depth of field area of the frontal camera, which covered approximately 3.5–4 cm; and (4) head and body markers could be seen during the whole walk. We recorded three walks of 10 flies on each of the four substrates. To obtain more reliable individual data for each substrate condition, we averaged the respective data from the three walks for each individual fly. The substrate types were employed in pseudorandom order.
The experiments under IR illumination were conducted in the same way as described above, except that we accepted also walks with only two complete tripod stride cycles. This change was necessary to obtain a sufficient amount of data, because in darkness, flies were less motivated to start walking and, when walking, the distances covered tended to be shorter.
Video analysis
To reduce jitter in the pixel coordinate data of the maker points, we filtered them (Gaussian-like filter: window size 6 ms corresponding to three data points, sigma=2 ms). The positions of the manually applied markers differed minimally between animals. Hence, level orientations of the head and body with respect to pitch, yaw and roll calculated for different individuals were afflicted with an offset and could only be pooled after a correction. We corrected for these systematic differences for each individual by adding to each calculated head angle the difference between calculated orientation and actual head and body orientation in the video image. Therefore, we selected frames in which the head and body were held in normal (i.e. not rolled, pitched or yawed) orientation. The correction angles ranged from –2 to 3 deg. These differences affect only the absolute orientations, not their relative changes. The 3-D trajectory of the fly’s head was determined by stereo triangulation of the positions of the head marker located directly anterior to the ocelli in the corresponding 2-D images from both cameras (Fig. 1A). To define the 3-D space filmed by both cameras, the cameras had to be calibrated [using J. Y. Bouguet’s MATLAB camera calibration toolbox; www.vision.caltech.edu/bouguetj/calib_doc/ (Bouguet and Perona, 1998)]. By reconstruction of points in space of a known distance, we estimated the reconstruction error as, on average, 0.03 mm.
Head rotations around more than one axis led the head to turn out of the projection plane of the frontal and/or top camera. Consequently, distortions could have emerged and affected the calculated head orientation. To assess this potential error of the 2-D method, we introduced a partly transparent 3-D head model into ivTrace, which was positioned virtually over the frontal and top image. We manually adjusted the model’s position and orientation framewise, so that it matched the position and orientation of the fly’s head in the images (Fig. 1B). After this time-consuming routine, the orientation data could be extracted from the 3-D model trace. We call this technique the ‘3-D method’. To estimate the error introduced by this procedure, the experimenter repeated the 3-D method for a given segment (30 frames) of the fly’s trajectory on five consecutive days. The mean standard deviation between the 3-D adjustments was 1.2 deg.
Because the 3-D method is assumed to yield rotation angles closer to the real ones at the expense of analysis time, we determined its benefit against the rotation angles determined with 2-D method. We calculated the mean difference angle ΔΦ and the correlation coefficient r between both time-dependent rotation data sets. The results were as follows: pitch, ΔΦ=0.26 deg, r=0.94; roll, ΔΦ=0.74 deg, r=0.91; and yaw, ΔΦ=2.58 deg, r=0.75. The relatively large ΔΦ value and low correlation of the yaw data indicate a discrepancy for head yaw data obtained with the 2-D and 3-D methods. Therefore, we used the automatic 2-D method to obtain positional as well as pitch and roll data of the flies’ head and thorax. However, head yaw angles were determined with the more time-consuming 3-D method. Jitter in the head yaw data was reduced by filtering with a Butterworth filter (second degree; relative cut-off frequency 0.1 Hz). We verified that this cut-off frequency had no functionally distorting effect on the yaw data. The stride cycles were determined manually by noting the touchdown time point of the left foreleg (McNeill, 2003), and were used to calculate the average stride frequency and the stride-related head and body rotations.
Statistical tests
Under bright light conditions, each tested fly walked three times on each of the four substrates. To compare the paired data obtained on the different substrates, we used multivariate ANOVA (MANOVA) for repeated measurements. Data were tested for normality with the Lilliefors test. Single variables, as roll or vertical translational fluctuations, were tested with an ANOVA for repeated measurements. All data sets have been tested with the Box test or the Mauchly test for homogeneity of covariance or variance, respectively. For heterogeneous covariances or variances, we give the Greenhouse–Geisser corrected MANOVA or ANOVA results. The Bonferroni’s correction for multiple comparisons (BC) was applied as post hoc test. Changes of body posture between the light conditions were analyzed with a t-test. The error values are given as standard deviation (s.d.). Statistical tests were performed with IBM SPSS Statistics (IBM Deutschland GmbH, Ehningen, Germany) and the statistics toolbox of MATLAB.
RESULTS
Blowfly compensatory head and body movements when walking on differently structured substrates
We recorded 10 flies (N=individual flies) walking three times (n=total recorded walks) on the four different substrates (0, 3, 6 and 8 mm bumps). Within the field of view of the cameras, flies walked on average a distance of 3.64±0.12 cm, which corresponds to approximately four times their body length (approximately 9 mm).
The time course of body and head locomotion parameters is depicted in Fig. 3 for a single run on the 0 mm substrate. The fly had an average walking speed of 17.5 cm s–1 (Fig. 3B) and walked a distance of 4.6 cm. The head and body rotated slightly around the three rotation axes. Pitch and roll rotations of the head were smaller than the body rotations (Fig. 3A), revealing that the head was held more stable than the body (supplementary material Movie 1). Head and body rotations around the yaw axis (Fig. 3A) drifted during this walk to slightly negative values, indicating slightly rightward turning of the head and body. The fluctuations (s.d.) of body roll orientation were, at 5.8 deg, more than three times larger than the corresponding head roll fluctuations of 1.7 deg (Fig. 3A).
Besides rotations, we also analyzed head translations (Fig. 3B). Similar to head orientation and rotation velocities, head position and forward velocity fluctuate during walking. The head of the walking fly experienced a maximal sideward shift of 2.2 mm at velocities ≤4.5 cm s–1 and a maximal vertical shift of 0.6 mm with velocities of up to 5.1 cm s–1. During this walk, forward velocities never decreased below 13 cm s–1 and reached peak values exceeding 22 cm s–1 (Fig. 3B).
Relationship between stride cycle and head and body rotations
Rotational and translational head and body fluctuations contain a strong periodic component that may be caused by a coupling between head and body rotations and the stride cycle. The length of a full stride cycle (i.e. all six legs performed a step in a tripod gait) was determined from the time when the left foreleg touches the ground. The duration of a stride cycle as averaged across all flies walking on flat ground was 67.4±0.5 ms. To analyze the coupling between stride cycle and rotational fluctuations, we averaged the head and body rotation data within a time window of 34 ms (half the mean stride cycle length) before and after a completed stride cycle. The respective mean rotation values were subtracted to allow averaging over different walks. In this way, we averaged for each walk across three to seven stride cycles (mean completed stride cycles: 4.8±0.2) and then across the three walks performed by each fly per substrate. Note that non-periodic rotational head and body fluctuations are averaged out by this method. To determine how the different substrates influence the stride-induced head and body rotations, we averaged the data of all walking flies separately for the different substrates (Fig. 4).
In general, the influence of the stride cycle on rotations was equally strong for head and body yaw, whereas roll and pitch rotations of the body were affected more strongly than the corresponding head rotations. On the 0 mm substrate, the body roll rotations coupled to the stride cycle had amplitudes of approximately 8 deg; the corresponding head roll rotations had amplitudes of less than half that value, approximately 3 deg (Fig. 4).
The amplitudes of rotations coupled to the stride cycle became smaller with increased substrate granularity of the ground (Fig. 4). For instance, pitch fluctuations of head and body had amplitudes of 2.5±0.9 and 4.4±1.2 deg, respectively, on 0 mm substrate, but 2±0.9 and 3±1.2 deg on 8 mm substrate. At first sight, this result might seem counterintuitive, because head and body rotations might be assumed to increase with increasing substrate granularity. However, note that the fluctuations coupled to the stride cycle do not represent the total head rotations (non-periodic rotational head and body fluctuations are averaged out). With increasing substrate granularity, the total rotational fluctuations became slightly larger (see next section). In other words, the effect of the substrate granularity on head and body rotations increased, whereas the effect of the walking apparatus on head and body rotations decreased on more coarsely structured substrates.
Structure dependence of rotation fluctuations
In general, head and body stabilization were diversely affected during walking on differently structured substrates (MANOVA, F1,9=26.5, P=0.001). Although head fluctuations were approximately 0.8 deg smaller than body fluctuations around the pitch and approximately 1.8 deg around the roll axis, indicating compensatory head movements (Fig. 5), head yaw fluctuations largely overlap with those of the body (Fig. 3A, Fig. 5) and were, on average, 0.31 deg larger on all substrate types.
Head and body fluctuations significantly increased with substrate granularity (MANOVA, F3,27=12.3, P<0.001; Fig. 5). Hence, the tested surface structures degraded the rotational stabilization of the fly’s body and that of the fly’s head, which was especially prominent for rotations around the pitch axis (Fig. 5A). Pitch fluctuations increased monotonically with substrate granularity from 2.4 to 4.2 deg for the head and from 3.2 to 4.7 deg for the body. In contrast, roll fluctuations stayed more or less constant on the 0, 3 and 6 mm substrates and only increased noticeably on the 8 mm substrate (ANOVA, body, F3,27=3.04, P=0.087; head, F3,27=3.22, P=0.089; Fig. 5A). Accordingly, compensation around the roll axis seemed to be the most robust during walking on structured substrates.
To assess rotational head and body velocities during walking, we calculated the time derivative of all recorded head and body orientation data. We took the absolute values to calculate the mean rotational velocities. Head rotations were significantly slower than body rotations around all rotation axes and on all substrates (MANOVA, F1,27=57.2, P<0.001; Fig. 5B). Pitch and roll head rotations were approximately 25% slower than those of the body, whereas head yaw turns were approximately 11% slower. Moreover, yaw rotations were on average the slowest turns whereas roll turns were the fastest, with velocities of up to >1000 deg s–1 in exceptional cases. These results are confirmed by the probability densities of the rotational velocities (Fig. 5C). Low rotational head velocities (up to 200 deg s–1) occurred more often than low rotational body velocities. The effect of substrate granularity on rotational velocities was significant (MANOVA, F3,81=5.05, P<0.001) but less systematic than on the rotational fluctuations.
Structure dependence of translation fluctuations
To assess how well the flies’ head is stabilized in space during walks on structured ground, we determined head translations from individual head trajectories (Fig. 6). The translational forward head velocity or walking speed was significantly affected by the substrate granularity (ANOVA, F3,27=10.66, P<0.001; Fig. 6B). Flies walked slightly faster on the 3 mm (BC, P=0.044) and 6 mm (BC, P=0.12) substrates than on flat ground. In fact, walking speed was fastest on the 3 mm substrate and slowest on the 8 mm substrate. The fluctuations of head sideward translations were similar for all substrates and ranged from 0.82 to 1.1 mm (ANOVA, F3,27=2.38, P=0.09; Fig. 6A). The head of a blowfly has a typical width of approximately 3.5 mm. Thus, the recorded head sideward translations did not exceed half head width. Vertical head translations (Fig. 6A) were significantly affected by substrate structure (ANOVA, F3,27=18.58, P<0.001) and increased monotonically with substrate granularity. At 0.44±0.12 mm, they were more than twice as large on the 8 mm substrate compared with on flat ground (0.2±0.03 mm). Nevertheless, in general, vertical translations were small during walking, even on the 8 mm substrate, given the fact that the corresponding bumps had nearly the size of the fly’s body.
Translational mean velocities were calculated as the derivative of the head position over time. We found a weak effect of substrate granularity on sideward head velocities (ANOVA, F3,27=3.94, P=0.019; Fig. 6B). Vertical head translation velocities also increased significantly with substrate granularity (ANOVA, F3,27=5.17, P=0.006; Fig. 6B).
Benchmark of compensation
To assess to what extent head and body are stabilized by compensatory mechanisms, we need to know how much they would fluctuate without such compensatory mechanisms. Because uncompensated fluctuations cannot be determined directly, we roughly estimated their potential size by comparing the granularity of the surface structure (i.e. height of substrate bumps) and the observed fluctuations in vertical head translations. We assumed that the vertical translations of the body are completely transferred to the head because head movements mediated by the fly’s neck motor system are mainly rotational and not translational (Sandeman, 1980a). Furthermore, we subtracted the vertical translational fluctuations recorded on the flat surface from the fluctuations determined for the structured substrates to assess the vertical translation values due to substrate granularity. The calculated vertical head translations induced by the substrate were 0.07 mm for the 3 mm substrate, 0.15 mm for the 6 mm substrate and 0.23 mm for the 8 mm substrate. These substrate-induced fluctuations amount, on average, to only 5.2% of the respective height of the hemispheres, which were used to characterize the different walking substrates. In other words, the walking apparatus of the tested blowflies compensates for most of the granularity of the walking surface. This good compensation is likely to be both, controlled via sensory systems and a consequence of leg properties and step positions on the substrates. More elaborate analyses would require sophisticated models of the blowflies’ pedal system and of the structured substrates. The generation of such models is far beyond the scope of the present study.
Relationship between stride frequency and walking speed
The mean stride cycle frequency over all conditions was 14.83±2.24 Hz. We found a strong positive correlation between walking speed and stride frequency for all substrate types (r0mm=0.89, r3mm=0.89, r6mm=0.84 and r8mm=0.82; Fig. 7). The average stride length is represented by the slopes of the respective regression fits (0 mm: 0.77 cm; 3 mm: 0.68 cm; 6 mm: 0.82 cm; 8 mm: 0.61 cm), which differed significantly for the tested substrate conditions (ANOVA, F3,27=4.91, P=0.008; Fig. 7). Post hoc tests revealed that this significant variance was restricted to the 6 and 8 mm conditions (BC, 6 mm vs 8 mm, P=0.03), indicating that, on average, the tested flies made longer strides on the 6 mm substrate than on the 8 mm substrate. However, there was no systematic change in average stride length, as the calculated values did not monotonically increase or decrease with changes of the substrate granularity.
Influence of the visual system on the control of head stabilization when walking on differently structured substrates
To estimate the influence of the visual system on the compensatory head movements during walking on structured substrates, we eliminated the visual input of the flies by testing them in the dark. The only light sources were IR LED panels, which emitted long-wave light far above the perceivable wavelengths of the blowfly’s photoreceptors (Hardie, 1979). Because under bright light conditions the most pronounced difference in rotational fluctuations had been found between walks on the 0 and 8 mm substrates, we recorded walks of three flies on only these substrates in the dark. In general, flies were less motivated to start walking in the dark than under bright conditions. In addition, on average, their walks were 1.7 cm shorter (BC, P<0.001) and less directional compared with bright light conditions.
Another obvious difference between walks in light and in the dark was an altered head and body posture. In the dark, blowflies shifted their body towards the mid and hind legs, pitched their body and head upwards and performed extensive movements with their forelegs without using them directly for walking (Fig. 8). On average, the body was pitched upwards by approximately 15.4 deg (P=0.003) and the head by approximately 10.7 deg (P=0.011) during walks in the dark compared with walks under bright light conditions on flat substrate. As a consequence, the forelegs had virtually no load to carry but acted as tactile probes to detect obstacles and aided leg positioning. This behavior was only observed in the dark.
Because of the upward-pitched body and head of flies walking in the dark, their head covered the body markers for the frontal camera. Therefore, it was not possible to analyze body roll rotations for walks in the dark. The fluctuations of the head and the two remaining body rotations are depicted in Fig. 9A. For roll, pitch and yaw, the head fluctuations increased on the 8 mm substrate. The average rotational fluctuations were similar under both lighting conditions (MANOVA, F1,31=0.204, P=0.65).
The altered walking style of blowflies in the dark was accompanied by a reduction in average walking speed compared with walks in bright light (ANOVA, F1,11=8.12, P=0.016; Fig. 9B). Flies tested in the dark walked approximately 3.4 cm s–1 slower on the flat substrate than flies in light. On the 8 mm substrate, flies in the dark walked at speeds of 5.6 cm s–1, only half as fast as in bright light.
DISCUSSION
Walking animals have direct mechanical contact with their substrate. Asperities in the surface structure of the substrate as well as the physical properties of the animal’s walking apparatus should directly affect its head and body orientation, unless the animal somehow compensates for the unintended changes. Compensatory head movements might be important from a functional point of view as they may stabilize the orientation of gaze as a frame of reference for visual information processing. In this study, we addressed this issue for walking blowflies, which possess highly mobile heads and have been employed for many years successfully as a model system for analyzing visual information processing (Land, 1999; Egelhaaf, 2006; Borst et al., 2010) and compensatory head movements (Hengstenberg, 1993).
We found that body and head, and thereby gaze, orientation of freely walking blowflies are effectively stabilized, with residual rotational fluctuations ranging between 2 and 5 deg around all axes. Surprisingly, even on coarsely structured ground with surface bumps as large as the animal, gaze stability was only marginally reduced for pitch and yaw and stayed constant for roll. For all tested surface conditions, head movements compensated for 25% of the rotational fluctuations of body pitch and 46% of the rotational fluctuations of body roll. Rotational head velocities were also reduced compared with body velocities by 26% for pitch, 22% for roll and 11% for yaw.
Head and body rotations of walking blowflies were found to be coupled, to some extent, to the stride cycle. This coupling was less prominent on strongly structured substrates. Stride cycle coupled rotations of the body (but not of the head) were recorded in an earlier study on freely walking blowflies (Horn and Mittag, 1980) and found to be in a range similar to those observed in the present study. Comparable observations have been made in humans, where vertical and horizontal head rotations that are coupled to stride frequency during walking are compensated, to some extent, to improve gaze stability (Grossman et al., 1988; Hirasaki et al., 1999).
What are the mechanisms underlying gaze stabilization on structured ground? The body fluctuations of blowflies originating from asperities of the walking substrate are compensated to a large extent by the pedal system. This conclusion is based on the finding that the translational and rotational body fluctuations increase only marginally as the bumps of the structured ground increase to nearly the animal’s body size. On top of the already excellent body stabilization by the pedal system, the neck system of the blowfly performs additional compensatory head movements that stabilize the gaze even further (Figs 5, 6).
How is head and body stabilization in blowflies walking on structured substrates controlled? Because rotational head stability did not decrease in the dark (Fig. 9), we conclude that head and thus gaze stability in walking blowflies is not controlled visually but mainly by mechanosensory systems. This finding is surprising, as not only mechanosensory systems, but also the visual system, have previously been shown to induce compensatory head movements in tethered flight and walking (Hengstenberg, 1991; Hengstenberg, 1993). Based on older evidence of direct and indirect projections from wide-field motion-sensitive neurons of the third visual neuropile to neck motorneurons (Strausfeld and Seyan, 1982; Milde et al., 1987), more recent studies were able to show in which way the output from these neurons is used by the neck motor system and that this optomotor transformation depends on the behavioral state of the animal (Huston and Krapp, 2008; Schwyn et al., 2011; Rosner et al., 2010). Nonetheless, despite this evidence for compensatory optomotor head responses and their likely neural substrate, our data suggest that they do not play any obvious role in walking flies. Because substrate structure is not directly sensed by the visual system but by mechanosensory systems, it is apparently adequate to rely only on mechanosensory cues for controling compensatory body and head movements during walking on structured substrates.
The effective body stabilizing behavior by the pedal system is most likely controlled by mechanosensors in the legs’ proprioceptive system. These have been shown in different insect species to be involved in the control of body orientation with respect to gravity (Horn, 1982) and in maintaining the distance between the body and the substrate (Kemmerling and Varju, 1981).
Head movements that compensate for unintended body movements that are induced by the ground structure and the walking apparatus are most probably controlled by the following systems: (1) the proprioceptive system in the forelegs and middle legs, which is also able to modulate compensatory head movements (Horn, 1982); (2) the haltere system, which detects rotations of the body and can induce adequate compensatory head movements (Sandeman, 1980a; Sandeman, 1980b; Nalbach and Hengstenberg, 1994; Huston and Krapp, 2009); and (3) the prosternal organ on the ventral side of the fly’s neck, which senses head rotations and posture and initiates compensatory head movements (Horn, 1982; Strausfeld and Seyan, 1982; Hengstenberg, 1993; Paulk and Gilbert, 2006; Sandeman, 1980a).
At first sight, our results seem to differ from earlier findings on head and body movements of free-walking blowflies (Blaj and van Hateren, 2004), where virtually no head and body rotations were observed during straight walking phases, but were found to be mainly due to yaw saccades. These differences can be explained by different experimental paradigms. On the one hand, Blaj and van Hateren (Blaj and van Hateren, 2004) were not able to record leg movements and, thus, did not have access to the walking cycle as a reference. On the other hand, the blowflies in their experiments walked freely on a plain surface and revealed a saccadic walking mode by changing frequently their direction (Blaj and van Hateren, 2004). During the straight intersaccadic segments, the flies walked with velocities between 0 and 4 cm s–1, i.e. considerably slower than in our experiments. Based on the relationship between walking speed and stride frequency as observed in the present study, the stride duration at walking velocities as small as those observed by Blaj and van Hateren (Blaj and van Hateren, 2004) is predicted to be well above 100 ms. This suggests that for intersaccadic intervals of, on average, less than 100 ms, as estimated on the basis of the data of Blaj and van Hateren (Blaj and van Hateren, 2004), not even one complete stride cycle takes place between consecutive saccades under these experimental conditions. In addition, the walking pattern might deviate from a regular tripod gait, as has been reported for slowly walking Drosophila (Strauss and Heisenberg, 1990). We assume that the lower rotational head and body fluctuations recorded by Blaj and van Hateren (Blaj and van Hateren, 2004) might be due to the specific features of the intersaccadic stride pattern at slow walking speeds.
To compare the stabilization performance of the head during walking and during flight, we took flight data of van Hateren and Schilstra (van Hateren and Schilstra, 1999a; van Hateren and Schilstra, 1999b) and analyzed the rotational velocities during 81 intersaccadic flight sections which were at least 60 ms long and, thus, were at least about as long as the average length of one stride cycle, i.e. 67.5 ms. The head stability in intersaccadic flight is considerably better than during straight walking on flat ground. Yaw fluctuations are six times smaller and pitch and roll rotations are approximately three times smaller (mean head fluctuations in intersaccadic flight: yaw, 0.65±0.3 deg; pitch, 0.86±0.1 deg; roll, 0.84±0.4 deg). Moreover, mean rotational head velocities are approximately 2.5 times slower in straight flight compared with straight walking on flat ground (mean head rotation velocities in intersaccadic flight: yaw, 61.8±8 deg s–1; pitch, 78.85±7 deg s–1; roll, 93.4±21 deg s–1). Obviously, the walking apparatus, even on a flat substrate, causes larger head fluctuations than the flight apparatus during the straight intersaccadic intervals. This difference should be caused mainly by the extent of body fluctuations both types of locomotion produce by interacting with the particular medium. The impact of this interaction during walking on a generally solid medium is stronger and more direct than the impact during flight through less solid, viscous air and, therefore, may produce more rotational body fluctuations.
To what extent do the head fluctuations during walking affect visual information processing? The first processing stages in insect motion vision can be explained by the model of elementary motion detection (Egelhaaf and Reichardt, 1987; Borst and Egelhaaf, 1989; Egelhaaf and Borst, 1989; Hassenstein and Reichardt, 1956), in which the visual input from neighboring photoreceptors or ommatidia is processed in a directionally selective way. The interommatidial angles in the blowfly’s compound eye range between 1.2 deg in the frontal visual field and 3 deg in the dorsal pole region (Petrowitz et al., 2000). Hence, the rotational head fluctuations of approximately 4 deg induce rotational image slip (optic flow) that extends across one to three neighboring ommatidia, and thus should be detectable by the visual system of the fly.
Elegant studies have shown that flying as well as walking insects perform saccadic gaze strategies to support spatial information acquisition from the optic flow generated by their self-motion (Schilstra and van Hateren, 1998; Blaj and van Hateren, 2004; Boeddeker et al., 2010). To enable the fly’s visual system to extract spatial information effectively from the optic flow generated during walking, the rotational optic flow components should be considerably smaller than the translational components. Translational optic flow is strong for close objects and fast movement speeds and weak for more distant objects and slow movements. Because flies move slower during walking than during flight, the spatial range in which translational flow dominates and the fly is able to obtain spatial information is smaller during walking than during flight. Furthermore, the rotational head fluctuations during straight walking cause additional rotational optic flow, which further reduces the distance range in which translational flow prevails. Consequently, the range in which walking flies can extract distance information is expected to be significantly reduced compared with flight.
How far-reaching is distance estimation by optic flow information during walking? One possible approach to investigate this issue may be to record the activity of optic-flow-processing neurons during stimulation with reconstructed optic flow as perceived by freely walking blowflies, as has already been done for flying flies (Kern et al., 2006; Karmeier et al., 2006). Because behavioral activity and, in particular, locomotion can affect the response properties of these neurons to some extent (Jung et al., 2011; Chiappe et al., 2010; Maimon et al., 2010; Rosner et al., 2010), it would be desirable to make neuronal recordings in the behaving fly as has recently been successfully accomplished in the much larger locust (Fotowat et al., 2011). With this new technique, it might be possible to estimate what spatial information can be extracted from optic-flow-processing neurons during free walking.
Acknowledgements
We thank R. Kern and N. Boedekker for critical comments on the manuscript, E. Braun and J. P. Lindemann for fundamental help in establishing the head and body tracking technique, and T. Siesenop for his help with data acquisition. Furthermore, we thank D. Schwyn, H. Krapp (Imperial College, London) and the Natural History Museum, London, for kindly providing the 3-D model of the head of the blowfly Calliphora vicina, as well as M. Botsch (Computer Graphics and Geometry Processing, Bielefeld University) for reducing the polygon complexity of the 3-D model.
FOOTNOTES
FUNDING
This study was supported by the Human Frontier Science Program (HFSP) [RPG16/2008] and the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG).