Trait polymorphisms are widespread in nature, and explaining their stable co-existence is a central problem in ecology and evolution. Alternative reproductive tactics, in which individuals of one or more sex exhibit discrete, discontinuous traits in response to reproductive competition, represent a special case of trait polymorphism in which the traits are often complex, behavioural, and dynamic. Thus, studying how alternative reproductive tactics are maintained may provide general insights into how complex trait polymorphisms are maintained in populations. We construct a spatially explicit individual-based model inspired from extensively collected empirical data to address the mechanisms behind the co-existence of three behavioural alternative reproductive tactics in males of a tree cricket (Oecanthus henryi). Our results show that the co-existence of these tactics over ecological time scales is facilitated by the spatial structure of the landscape they inhabit, which serves to equalise the otherwise unequal mating benefits of the three tactics. We also show that this co-existence is unlikely if spatial aspects of the system are not considered. Our findings highlight the importance of spatial dynamics in understanding ecological and evolutionary processes and underscore the power of integrative approaches that combine models with empirical data.

The persistence of polymorphisms in populations has been a central theme in evolutionary biology. Alternative reproductive tactics (ARTs) are a class of polymorphisms that evolve in response to competition for mates and manifest as discrete morphological, physiological, and/or behavioural traits that can be expressed by individuals of either sex (Oliveira et al., 2008). For example, male guppies (poecilid fish) are known to either court or forcefully copulate with females (Bisazza and Pilastro, 1997). Likewise, female digger wasps (Sphex ichneumoneus) either build their nests to lay eggs or parasitise other females' nests (Brockmann, et al., 1979). Male field crickets either call to attract females or behave as satellites (Cade, 1981).

Traditionally, the persistence of ARTs has been understood with reference to their mode of expression. ‘Genetically determined’ ARTs are entirely controlled by a few loci and are thought to be maintained in populations through mechanisms such as neutrality (Shuster and Wade, 1991), spatiotemporally varying selection pressures (Mazer and Damuth, 2001), negative frequency-dependent selection (Gross, 1996) and heterozygote dominance (Krüger et al., 2001). On the other hand, ‘condition-dependent’ ARTs are conditionally expressed depending on an organism's intrinsic state or extrinsic environment and can persist as long as different tactics are optimal in different conditions and all these different conditions frequently occur in nature (Oliveira et al., 2008). However, the classification of ARTs into genetic and conditional is controversial, as it is increasingly clear that their phenotypic expression is regulated by a combination of genetic and environmental (intrinsic or extrinsic) factors (Brockmann, 2001). Understanding how the demographic and physical environment affect the fitness of ARTs independent of their mode of expression may yield more insights into coexistence of ARTs and contribute to a general theoretical framework for their persistence.

Trait frequency and population density are important demographic factors that affect the fitness of alternative tactics and may determine whether a polymorphism can be maintained in a population (Gross, 1996; Kokko and Rankin, 2006). Negative frequency-dependent selection, wherein the fitness of an ART decreases as its frequency of expression in the population increases, is thought to be a common mechanism for the coexistence of ARTs (and polymorphisms in general), especially in evolutionary game theory (Wolf and Waltz, 1993). This phenomenon can lead to the maintenance of ARTs at a constant (Gross, 1996; Wolf and Waltz, 1993) or oscillating frequency (Takahashi and Kawata, 2013) based on system specifics.

Population density can also affect the fitness of ARTs and determine which tactic (if any) has the highest fitness in the population. For example, in Sancassania berlesei, a species of acarid mites, males can either fight or scramble to gain mates. Artificial selection experiments show that the fitness of fighter morphs in this species decreases with an increase in population density (Michalczyk et al., 2018), possibly due to a higher propensity for harmful interactions with conspecifics. In field populations of the European field cricket (Gryllus campestris), silent males are more successful at mate-finding than calling males at high population densities, with the reverse holding true at low population densities (Hissmann, 1990) due to a change in encounter probabilities. Furthermore, both theoretical (Eadie and Fryxell, 1992) and empirical (Takahashi and Kawata, 2013) studies have shown that population density can have an amplifying effect on the strength of frequency-dependent selection and thus facilitate coexistence.

Spatial structure in the environment can also impact the fitness of alternative tactics and change the potential for the maintenance of polymorphisms. For example, fighter males of the mite Rhizoglyphus echinopus show lower fitness in complex 3D habitats, possibly due to the limited movement ability of the fighter males and the spatial distribution of females (Tomkins et al., 2011). Heterogeneity brought about by a spatially structured landscape provides an opportunity for multiple phenotypes to coexist via local adaptation (Gray et al., 2008; Korona et al., 1994; Stein et al., 2014; Zahnd et al., 2021). Computational and analytical models in the context of migration and cooperation show that movement and spatial structure can promote trait polymorphism and even oscillatory states (Guttal and Couzin, 2010; Joshi et al., 2017). In spatial models of competitive Lotka-Volterra communities, incorporating ‘steric’ structures that obstruct dispersal within the landscape can lead to more localised interspecific interactions in space, which in turn can facilitate ecological coexistence (Ursell, 2021). In nature, habitats are rarely isotropic and steric obstructions to movement abound, meaning that incorporating such heterogeneity is vital to making inferences about real-life populations in the field.

The reproductive life-history of sexual organisms often involves signal-receiver dynamics where individuals emit a signal to improve detectability or attractiveness to members of the opposite sex. In organisms that show acoustic signal-receiver dynamics, males can adopt multiple ARTs such as calling and satellite behaviours. Steric structures in the habitat can change the effective distance up to which a signal can be transmitted (Endler, 1992; Jain and Balakrishnan, 2012). Steric structures can also serve as microhabitats and affect the spatial distribution and movement patterns of signallers and receivers. This is especially relevant for species that exhibit ARTs: different tactics may employ diverse signalling and movement behaviours, and steric structures could differentially affect the fitness of different ARTs, with potential implications for the maintenance of polymorphisms over evolutionary time. The effect of spatial structure on the coexistence of such behavioural ARTs has, however, received relatively little attention.

In the tree cricket Oecanthus henryi, as in many cricket species, males call by stridulating with their forewings to attract mates (Walker, 1957). Alternatively, males can choose to remain silent (Torsekar and Balakrishnan, 2020). A third ART (‘baffling’) exists in which males construct an acoustic baffle (amplifier) using a leaf from their host plant (Hyptis suaveolens) and call through this structure (Deb et al., 2020). Baffling amplifies the male call, increasing the distance over which the male can be perceived by phonotactic females (Prozesky-Schulze et al., 1975) and increasing the attractiveness of a male to phonotactic females (Deb et al., 2020). Given these obvious mating advantages, one may expect that all males should baffle. Despite this, only a fraction of males are bafflers in natural populations of O. henryi (Deb, 2017). We explore this conundrum using an IbM based on empirical data. Specifically, we investigate whether mechanisms such as negative-frequency dependence, population density and spatial structure could (i) prevent baffling from always being advantageous over other tactics, and (ii) facilitate the coexistence of baffling, calling (without baffling), and silent mating tactics in O. henryi.

Individual-based models (IbMs) are a powerful tool to study the effect of spatial structure as they explicitly model the local interactions that provide a mechanistic basis for observable patterns at the population level. IbMs further allow us to capture emergent properties resulting from non-linear interactions which may not be feasible using analytical models. IbMs of calling and satellite cricket males show that their fitness is likely strongly affected by population density but that frequency-dependence plays a relatively minor role (Rowell and Cade, 1993; Walker and Cade, 2003). Similar models have also shown that both calling and satellite tactics can coexist despite high levels of parasitism by an acoustically orienting parasitoid (Rotenberry and Zuk, 2016). These studies do not, however, incorporate the spatial structure of the habitat in their simulations.

In the homogenous habitat (Fig. 1A), the average mating success of baffling decreased with increasing proportion of bafflers in the population, implying negative frequency-dependent fitness benefits of baffling (Fig. 2A,C). The negative frequency dependence of baffling is strengthened as population density increases as indicated by the negative interaction of population density with the proportion of bafflers in the population (Fig. 2D). In the homogenous habitat, despite the strong negative frequency-dependent fitness effects on baffling, the average mating success of the baffling tactic was greater than the other ARTs (silent and calling) for all values of population density and proportion of bafflers that we tested in the simulations (Fig. 2A). Further, the disparity between the mating success of baffling and the other ARTs increased with population density (Fig. 3A,B).

Fig. 1.

An illustration of the simulation arena. (A) A homogenous simulation arena. (B) A heterogenous simulation arena with squares indicating bushes. The circles indicate the radial distance at which sound pressure level (SPL) of a male caller/baffler (coloured dots at the centre of circles) drops to 45 dB (hearing threshold of females). The number of individuals depicted and the arena dimensions are for illustrative purposes only. The number of individuals in the simulations was fixed to 500 and the arena size was varied depending on population density.

Fig. 1.

An illustration of the simulation arena. (A) A homogenous simulation arena. (B) A heterogenous simulation arena with squares indicating bushes. The circles indicate the radial distance at which sound pressure level (SPL) of a male caller/baffler (coloured dots at the centre of circles) drops to 45 dB (hearing threshold of females). The number of individuals depicted and the arena dimensions are for illustrative purposes only. The number of individuals in the simulations was fixed to 500 and the arena size was varied depending on population density.

Fig. 2.

Mating success of ARTs is altered by spatial structure. (A) Average mating success of the ARTs decreases with the proportion of bafflers in the homogenous landscape, with bafflers showing maximum success, for various population densities (d). (B) Average mating success of the ARTs is altered dramatically in a structured landscape with bush density ρ=1 bush /m2, with nearly all strategies having similar success across baffler proportions and population densities. (C) The strength of effect of proportion of bafflers on mating success of the baffling tactic across the homogenous habitat and structured habitats having varying bush densities ρ. The negative frequency dependence of bafflers weakens in structured habitats. (D) Interaction effects of proportion of bafflers and population density on the mating success of the baffling tactic across the homogenous habitat and structured habitats having varying bush densities ρ. Error bars indicate 95% CIs. Panels A and B depict results for only a partial set of population densities and habitat type/bush densities (ρ). The complete results are presented in Fig. S1.

Fig. 2.

Mating success of ARTs is altered by spatial structure. (A) Average mating success of the ARTs decreases with the proportion of bafflers in the homogenous landscape, with bafflers showing maximum success, for various population densities (d). (B) Average mating success of the ARTs is altered dramatically in a structured landscape with bush density ρ=1 bush /m2, with nearly all strategies having similar success across baffler proportions and population densities. (C) The strength of effect of proportion of bafflers on mating success of the baffling tactic across the homogenous habitat and structured habitats having varying bush densities ρ. The negative frequency dependence of bafflers weakens in structured habitats. (D) Interaction effects of proportion of bafflers and population density on the mating success of the baffling tactic across the homogenous habitat and structured habitats having varying bush densities ρ. Error bars indicate 95% CIs. Panels A and B depict results for only a partial set of population densities and habitat type/bush densities (ρ). The complete results are presented in Fig. S1.

Fig. 3.

Disparity in mating success of tactics decreases in structured habitats compared to homogenous habitats. (A) Difference in mating success between calling and baffling tactic (values outside brackets in each cell) is positive for structured habitats (all rows above the ‘Homogenous’ row), indicating that callers perform better than bafflers in structured habitats. The values inside the bracket provide mating success of the baffling tactic to allow comparison of absolute mating success values. (B) Difference in mating success between silent and baffling tactic (values outside brackets in each cell) is negative for both homogenous and structured habitats, indicating that bafflers always gain more matings than silent individuals. However, for all population densities, this difference reduces as the habitat becomes more structured, indicating that even silent individuals do relatively better in structured habitats than in homogenous habitats. Here too, values within brackets indicate mating success of the baffling tactic to allow comparison of absolute mating successes of the three tactics.

Fig. 3.

Disparity in mating success of tactics decreases in structured habitats compared to homogenous habitats. (A) Difference in mating success between calling and baffling tactic (values outside brackets in each cell) is positive for structured habitats (all rows above the ‘Homogenous’ row), indicating that callers perform better than bafflers in structured habitats. The values inside the bracket provide mating success of the baffling tactic to allow comparison of absolute mating success values. (B) Difference in mating success between silent and baffling tactic (values outside brackets in each cell) is negative for both homogenous and structured habitats, indicating that bafflers always gain more matings than silent individuals. However, for all population densities, this difference reduces as the habitat becomes more structured, indicating that even silent individuals do relatively better in structured habitats than in homogenous habitats. Here too, values within brackets indicate mating success of the baffling tactic to allow comparison of absolute mating successes of the three tactics.

When spatial structure in the form of bushes was introduced into the simulation landscape (Fig. 1B), we observed a weakening of the frequency dependence of the baffling tactic and the disparity between the mating success of baffling and the other tactics diminished considerably (compare Fig. 2A and B). The weakening of the strength of frequency dependence was independent of bush density in the structured habitat (Fig. 2B,C). However, increase in population density strengthened the frequency dependence of baffling at low bush densities (Fig. 2D).

While baffling was clearly superior in homogenous habitats, in spatially structured habitats, the calling tactic showed a 2-14% increase in mating success relative to that of bafflers (Fig. 3A). The advantage of calling over baffling increased with population density and bush density. The difference between the silent and baffling tactics also reduced with the introduction of bushes. Increasing bush density and decreasing population density together decreased the disparity in mating success of the silent and baffling tactics (Fig. 3B). Furthermore, movement of callers and silent males reduced the disparity in mating success of the ARTs in the structured habitat (Fig. 4B) while it showed no such effect in the homogenous habitat (Fig. 4A). Sensitivity analyses revealed that the results remained qualitatively unchanged for various movement related (Fig. S4-6; S8-12) and demographic (Fig. S7 and S13) parameter values.

Fig. 4.

Habitat dependent effects of male movement on mating success of ARTs. (A) Mating success of ARTs in homogenous habitats when males were not allowed to move (left panel) and when silent and calling males were allowed to move (right panel); these suggest a negligible impact of movement on the mating success in homogenous habitats. (B) Mating success of ARTs in structured habitats (bush density ρ=1) when males were not allowed to move (left panel) and when silent and calling males were allowed to move (right panel); these suggest that, unlike the homogenous habitat scenario shown in A, movement affects the relative mating success of callers and bafflers in structured habitats. For both panels A and B, population density d=0.5. Error bars indicate 95% CIs.

Fig. 4.

Habitat dependent effects of male movement on mating success of ARTs. (A) Mating success of ARTs in homogenous habitats when males were not allowed to move (left panel) and when silent and calling males were allowed to move (right panel); these suggest a negligible impact of movement on the mating success in homogenous habitats. (B) Mating success of ARTs in structured habitats (bush density ρ=1) when males were not allowed to move (left panel) and when silent and calling males were allowed to move (right panel); these suggest that, unlike the homogenous habitat scenario shown in A, movement affects the relative mating success of callers and bafflers in structured habitats. For both panels A and B, population density d=0.5. Error bars indicate 95% CIs.

Demographic factors can affect the fitness outcomes of ARTs. In the present study, we examined the effect of the proportion of bafflers, population density and habitat structure on the mating success of baffling males in the presence of other ARTs (calling and silent) expressed by males of a tree cricket by employing IbMs.

Does baffling show negative-frequency dependence?

In homogenous habitats, baffling showed clear negative frequency-dependence in mating success, and the strength of negative frequency-dependence increased with an increase in population density (Fig. 2A). The negative frequency-dependence of mating success of bafflers can be attributed to increased competition for mates among bafflers as their frequency in the population increased. This is because, increasing the number of baffling males will increase competition for a fixed number of females, resulting in lowered per-capita mating success of bafflers. The increased strength of frequency-dependence with an increase in population density can be understood as an increase in the potential for competition: when population density is high, females are more likely to hear two or more male calls simultaneously and execute choice in phonotaxis. These results are similar to the negative frequency-dependence of mating success of calling males observed in caller-satellite systems (Rotenberry and Zuk, 2016; Rowell and Cade, 1993).

Interestingly, the introduction of spatial structure ameliorated negative frequency-dependence of baffling and led to more uniform mating success. This can be understood through the following reasoning: since movement across bushes is rare, most phonotaxis in our model happens within the same bush. The spatial scale over which bafflers directly compete for females is thus constrained by bush size, whereas in the homogenous case, it is constrained by the size of the overlapping active sound fields of the calling males (i.e. the distance up to which their call transmits before the amplitude falls below the behavioural threshold required for female phonotaxis). Thus, in our simulations, baffling males effectively compete mostly with other baffling males that co-occur on the same bush, whereas in the homogenous cases, they compete with a larger number of baffling males that are present within their active acoustic spaces.

Is baffling the superior tactic in structured habitats?

In the homogenous case, despite the occurrence of negative frequency-dependence, the mating success of the baffling tactic was considerably higher than those of the other ARTs for all values of baffling proportion and population density we examined in our simulations (Fig. 2A), in line with a previous simulation study (Deb et al., 2020). This is because bafflers enjoy a large increase in their call amplitude, which confers a twofold benefit to mating success by both increasing the distance from which bafflers can be detected and increasing their attractiveness when competing with other calling males that are close by. These effects together lead to bafflers having a much higher mating success than callers and silent males.

Introducing spatial structure decreased the absolute mating success of all three tactics compared to the homogenous case (compare Fig. 2A and B). More importantly, spatial structure disproportionally decreases the mating success of bafflers, reducing the disparity in mating success between tactics and resulting in calling being the tactic with the greatest mating success (Fig. 3A and B). The limited success of baffling can once again be understood by recognizing that bushes are small, and across-bush flights are rare. Since across-bush movement is rare, most mating events are the result of phonotaxis that occurs towards a male in the same bush as the female. The advantages of baffling are limited in this case, since bafflers can primarily attract females that co-occur on the same bush. Given the low expected number of animals on a bush in structured habitats, the chance of a baffler co-occurring with a female on a bush is low, which limits the success of bafflers. Further, since females always move towards a sound that is sufficiently loud [in our simulations, >60 dB sound pressure level (SPL)], the greater amplitude of bafflers is only advantageous in scenarios in which females are choosing between two or more alternative sources of sound, a scenario that is rare unless the global population density is high (Deb and Balakrishnan, 2014) and the habitat is unstructured.

How does movement affect mate-encounter rate in structured versus homogenous habitats?

Individuals using tactics involving external structures such as leaves (in tree crickets) and burrows (in mole crickets) to increase the amplitude of their calls (Brandt et al., 2023) are often rendered immobile because these external structures are immovable. In our simulations too, bafflers were constrained to be stationary for the duration of each simulation run. In contrast, non-baffling callers, as well as silent males, moved around the landscape albeit with low likelihood. To test whether movement conferred any advantage to calling and silent males in homogenous landscapes, we compared the results of our simulations where callers and silent males were allowed to move to those where they were not. The comparisons showed that movement did not provide any effective advantage to callers and silent males (Fig. 4A). Our results are in agreement with simulations investigating optimal mate search patterns in homogenous habitats for mating systems that involve sexual signalling mates, which show that signallers should move more slowly or less diffusively than the receivers to maximise their mate-encounter rate (Mizumoto and Dobata, 2018).

However, similar comparisons using structured habitats showed that callers and silent males showed higher mating success when they were allowed to move compared to when they were not allowed to move (Fig. 4B). Advantage due to movement may explain why in spatially structured habitats, calling males, despite their lower call amplitude showed higher mating success than stationary bafflers (Fig. 2B, middle and right panel). Furthermore, the mating success of silent males in structured habitats (Fig. 2B) also increased compared to the negligible mating success they enjoyed in homogenous habitats (Fig. 2A). Simulations with only silent males and females showed that mating success was higher in structured versus the homogenous habitat, suggesting that spatial structuring increases mate encounter rate (Fig. S2). The higher random mate-encounter rate of silent males in structured habitats partially explains the reduction in the difference between the mating success of bafflers and silent males in structured habitats compared to the homogenous habitat. Random mate encounter may have increased relevance to mating success in spatially structured habitats as females showed increased tendency to perform random movement relative to phonotaxis in structured habitats (Fig. S3). The findings suggest that movement may facilitate mate-finding for both signalling and non-signalling males in structured landscapes, highlighting the importance of incorporating movement and spatial structure in future studies aiming to infer optimal mate-finding strategies in signal-receiver systems.

The importance of habitat spatial structure for O. henryi

A recent study investigating the acoustic efficiency of different calling strategies in 3D landscapes using finite element analysis showed that baffling is more efficient in increasing the volume of the sound field than unaided calling for male Oecanthus species, which mainly call from vegetation (Brandt et al., 2023). Despite the increased acoustic efficiency of baffling, however, only around 11% of O. henryi males baffle on a given night in natural field conditions (Deb, 2017). Deb et al. (2020) showed that baffling may be condition-dependent with smaller, lower intensity calling males opting to baffle as they stand to obtain a larger increase in mating success from baffling as compared to larger, louder males (Deb et al., 2020). Our results propose an additional explanation for the low percentage of bafflers in the field: calling may be more effective than baffling in structured landscapes as baffling highly constrains male movement. Additionally, it is evident from Fig. 3A that the advantage of calling over baffling is marginal at low population and bush densities but considerable at higher population and bush density. Thus, fluctuating population and bush densities, which can be expected in a natural habitat, may affect the difference in mating success of these ARTs and facilitate their co-existence. Incorporating more ecological realism, such as saturating benefits (Deb et al., 2020) and energetic costs associated with loud signalling may further equalise the pay-offs of these ARTs, potentially explaining their coexistence. Predation risk, another relevant ecological factor, was found to be to too low to have meaningful effects on the coexistence of ARTs in O. henryi (Torsekar et al., 2019).

Conclusion and future directions

Our study shows that incorporating spatial structure can affect the success of ARTs in a population. Firstly, while our finding corroborates previous modelling studies that demonstrate negative-frequency dependence of ARTs in crickets in unstructured landscapes, our study goes further to show that the negative frequency dependence breaks down in structured landscapes. Secondly, incorporating spatial structure reduces the disparity in mating success between ARTs. Post facto analysis of our findings revealed that movement played a pivotal role in reducing the disparity and the same is only observable in structured landscapes. Furthermore, in our model, introducing spatial structure changed the hierarchy of mating success of male ARTs. In structured habitats, calling was marginally more successful than baffling at low population densities and this difference in mating success increased with population density. Thus, our study shows that spatial structure along with population density plays a role in altering the hierarchy in mating success of ARTs in structured habitats. Summarily, by employing individual based modelling which allowed us to incorporate heterogeneity in the habitat as well as among individuals in a populations, we provide a first proof of concept for the role of spatial structure and movement ecology in facilitating the coexistence of signalling ARTs.

Analytic studies do propose cyclical dominance of strategies as a mechanism for the coexistence of three or more alternative strategies in nature (Szolnoki et al., 2014) with empirical support stemming from studies on biological systems ranging from side-blotched lizards (Sinervo and Lively, 1996) to bacteria (Kirkup and Riley, 2004). However, empirical insights suggest that cyclic dominance is quite rare in nature (Park et al., 2020), meaning the generality of cyclic dominance as a mechanism for coexistence is limited. Analytic studies of ecological coexistence in non-spatial models also suggest that the coexistence of multiple strategies is unstable (Alonzo and Calsbeek, 2010), unlikely (Han et al., 2012) or require special circumstances (Roy and Chattopadhyay, 2007). On the other hand, our study finds support in numerous other studies that show that habitat structuring can sustain the coexistence of competing species/phenotypes by promoting diversifying selection (Gray et al., 2008), demographic stochasticity (Calsbeek et al., 2002) or limiting interaction between competing individuals (Ursell, 2021).

We employed individual-based modelling in our study as it is a useful tool to model systems where relative importance of different factors and the nature of their interaction in determining the system dynamics is a priori unknown. IbMs can serve as a guide to building more sophisticated analytical models. For example, it would be interesting to see which factors of our model (such as caller:silent ratio, behavioural differences in mated versus virgin females, etc.) can be safely neglected in analytical approximations without affecting the qualitative behaviour of the system. Such analytical models, with their reduced complexity, may allow us to model long term evolutionary dynamics and thus predict persistence of ARTs.

Although our study does not allow for trait evolution or demonstrate the coexistence of competing phenotypes over evolutionary time, it does show that incorporating habitat structure contributes to equalizing the mating success of competing ARTs. There is a need for future studies to perform simulations over evolutionary timescales to examine the persistence or lack thereof of ARTs. It is imperative that these studies should consider spatial structure along with other important factors such as individual lifespan under natural conditions and energetic costs of signalling among others while examining the persistence of ARTs. Furthermore, mesocosm experiments that simulate homogenous and structured environments can be employed to test the efficacy of signal-based ARTs in obtaining mates. Individual behavioural tracking in these mesocosm experiments can provide longitudinal data on the per-capita expression rates of the ARTs along with their corresponding mating successes. Comparing the mating success of ARTs in both homogenous and heterogenous mesocosms will help test the hypothesis whether habitat structuring indeed contributes to the reduction in disparity of mating success associated with the ARTs. Furthermore, experiments that incorporate increasing levels of heterogeneity can be used to test whether the stabilizing effect of habitat structure on the fitness of ARTs increases with habitat heterogeneity. Such studies can inform conservation decisions concerned with the modification of landscapes housing organisms that display ARTs.

We use a spatially explicit, individual-based model of O. henryi in which we specify sex and tactic-specific rules for each individual. The values of the parameters used in the model are primarily inspired and abstracted from the observations made on O. henryi. Description of model parameters, their values as well as their empirical sources are stated in the following sections and summarised in Table 1. For instances where we used parameterised distributions, the goodness of fit of distributions to the empirical data are summarised in Table 2. Here, we summarise the broad outline of the model and the details are given in the sub-headings that follow.

Table 1.

List of all static model parameters and their empirical sources

List of all static model parameters and their empirical sources
List of all static model parameters and their empirical sources
Table 2.

Summary of parameter distributions for male and female movement, male calling effort and bush widths (i.e. bush sizes)

Summary of parameter distributions for male and female movement, male calling effort and bush widths (i.e. bush sizes)
Summary of parameter distributions for male and female movement, male calling effort and bush widths (i.e. bush sizes)

Model overview

Our model consists of individual crickets positioned in a two-dimensional (2D) square arena with reflective boundaries. Each simulation consists of 500 individuals (1:1 sex ratio) and is run for 72 time-steps, where each time step corresponds to 10 min of real time and the total simulation time captures one night (12 h). Each male is assigned one of three tactics (silent, calling, baffling) and this tactic is fixed for the entire duration of a simulation. The males and females move and interact according to rules that reflect empirical findings (described later). We vary the proportion of bafflers and the spatial structure. We also vary population density (d) by varying the size of the square arena. We then evaluate the average mating success of each tactic employed by individual males. The details of the model are described below.

Spatial structure

Individuals inhabit a large 2D, square arena, whose size is determined according to the specified population density d (number of individuals per square metre). We consider a range of densities from 0.05 to 1 individual per sq. m (i.e. d={ 0.05, 0.25, 0.5, 0.75, 1 }). The range of population densities chosen in our model was inspired by inter-individual distances measured in the field (Deb and Balakrishnan, 2014). Tree crickets are generally found on trees or bushes, and the species we study (O. henryi) is found mainly on Hyptis suaveolens bushes. To check for the effect of spatial structure in the form of bushes, we modelled two distinct kinds of habitats (as illustrated in Fig. 1).

Homogenous habitat

As a model for unstructured habitats, we construct a homogenous habitat where we do not account for the presence of bushes and the individuals are free to move throughout the landscape (Fig. 1A). To initiate the simulation, individuals are positioned throughout the landscape according to a random uniform distribution. The homogenous habitat acts as a null model to compare with the effects of structured habitats.

Structured habitat

As an approximation of the ecological reality of the habitat we incorporate bushes in our landscape. Bushes are abstracted as closed squares within the landscape (Fig. 1B). Individuals can either move within the same bush or instantaneously move from one bush to another but cannot exist outside of a bush at any given time step. The presence of bushes therefore brings about heterogeneity in the landscape. The position of the centres of the bushes was determined by a uniform random distribution whose limits were determined by the edges of the landscape. The number of bushes is determined by the spatial density of bushes (number of bushes per square metre), denoted by ρ. In structured habitats, the expected number of animals on a bush is given by d/ρ where d is population density and ρ is bush density. Thus, for a bush density value of 1, the number of individuals on a bush can range from 0.05 to 1 for population densities ranging from 0.05 to 1. We tested for the influence of the spatial density of bushes by varying ρ from 0.5 to 2 bushes per sq. m. In the field, bushes are present at a mean density of about 1.6 bushes per sq. m, but this number is subject to substantial seasonal variation (Torsekar et al., 2019).

Acoustic properties of the landscape

Since male tree crickets emit acoustic signals to attract mates, the attenuation properties of sound within the landscape are important for their reproductive ecology. In our model, we assume that the sound pressure level decays according to the laws of spherical spreading. Our assumption of spherical spreading of sound reflects the fact that the presence of bushes in the field causes negligible excess attenuation (Deb and Balakrishnan, 2014). The intensity of sound is measured in terms of SPL. According to the laws of spherical spreading, i.e. the transmission loss (L) of a signal in dB SPL at a distance d metres away from the source is assumed to be given by:

The constant 0.2 reflects the fact that the empirical measurements of sound amplitudes that we used to parameterise the simulation are measured from a distance of 0.2 m (20 cm) away from the source. The reflective boundary conditions on animal movement ensure that individuals on one end of the landscape do not listen to individuals signalling from the opposite end, thus preventing possible boundary effects due to unphysical acoustic transmission.

Individual attributes

Males can perform one of three tactics, and the tactic assigned to them is fixed for the duration of the simulation run: (1) Bafflers can call but their location is fixed for the duration of the simulation. (2) Callers can both call and move randomly through the landscape. (3) Silent males execute random movements but do not emit any calls.

To check for the effect of proportion of bafflers on the mating success of the ARTs, we varied the proportion of bafflers in the population. The remaining males are equally partitioned into the caller and silent ARTs, in accordance with empirical findings that show that a male is equally likely to call or remain silent on a night (Sadiq et al., 2023 preprint).

Since the ARTs differ with respect to both signalling and movement, we elucidate them separately.

Signalling

Silent males do not emit any calls. Callers and bafflers were modelled as point emitters of spherically symmetric sound. They have an associated call effort, defined as the proportion of time steps (out of 72) for which they emit a call. Based on empirical work (Torsekar and Balakrishnan, 2020), the call effort of each signalling male is drawn from a truncated normal distribution with a mean 0.5 and s.d. 0.26. Since call effort must be a number within the interval (0,1), we resampled if the drawn call effort was outside this interval. Once the call effort is determined for each individual, the specific time steps in which a male calls are randomly assigned. In addition, we assume that a male will not call when it is mating. Each signalling male also has an associated amplitude for its call. The SPL of a caller is drawn from an N(60.8 dB, 3.6 dB) distribution following empirical data (Deb et al., 2020). If the male is a baffler, it receives an additional amplitude boost due to its baffle relative to the non-baffling caller. The excess amplitude due to the baffle is also drawn from an N(10.1, 1.3) distribution (Deb et al., 2020).

Movement

In our simulations, bafflers are not allowed to move since baffling requires males to make a hole and call from a leaf. Silent males and callers can move, but we assume that callers do not move while emitting a call. The extent and likelihood of movement differ based on whether the movement is within a bush or across bushes. Males can perform only one of the two movements (within or across bush movement) in a time step. The likelihood of movement within a bush is denoted by πm. In this case, the distance (in cm) a male moves is drawn from an LN(2.59, 0.88) distribution, i.e. a log-normal distribution with mean 2.59 and standard deviation 0.88. The parameters for the log-normal distribution are derived from empirical data (Sadiq et al., 2023 preprint). If a male does not move within a bush, it may instead move across bushes with probability πm. Therefore, the net likelihood of across-bush movement is given by [1-πmm′ (i.e. probability of not moving within a bush multiplied by the probability of moving across bushes) and is lower than the likelihood of movement within a bush. Our assumption of high within-bush-movement and low across-bush-movement is motivated by the high site fidelity previously observed in males of O. henryi (Deb and Balakrishnan, 2014) within nights and is common to other taxa as well (Kleyla and Dodson, 1978; Pittman et al., 2008). For movement within the bush, the direction is chosen randomly. When individuals execute movement across bushes, they move to a randomly chosen bush whose centre is within a distance λm from the centre of the bush the male currently occupies. λm is drawn from a LN(1.93, 0.75) distribution each time the male moves across bushes (Torsekar and Balakrishnan, 2020). The males assume a random position within this new bush. In homogenous habitats, males move according to within-bush parameters. While it is interesting to assume that silent males behave as satellites and show directed movement towards calling males, we assumed randomness in the directionality of movement for both calling and silent males for the sake of simplicity.

Females

Females are silent and perform two types of movement: 1) directed movement in response to male signals, termed as phonotaxis; 2) movement in a random direction when not within the sound field of calling males. Laboratory experiments indicate that mating status (virgin or mated) can affect female movement (Modak et al., 2021). In line with previous studies (Rowell and Cade, 1993), we therefore implemented different relative probabilities of phonotaxis and random movement for females in our simulations based on their mating status.

Movement

Females show a high likelihood of movement within a bush, and we denote this likelihood by πf. In this case, the distance (in cm) moved by the female in a timestep is drawn from a LN(2.95, 1.03) distribution. If a female does not move within a bush, it may instead move across bushes with probability πf′. Therefore, the net likelihood of across-bush movement is given by [1-πff, i.e. probability of not moving within a bush multiplied by probability of moving across bushes and is lower than the likelihood of movement within a bush. When individuals execute movement across bushes, they move to a bush whose centre is within a distance λf from the centre of the bush the female currently occupies. λf is drawn from a LN(2.1, 0.82) distribution each time the female moves across bushes, in accordance with empirical data (Torsekar and Balakrishnan, 2020). The females assume a random position within this new bush. In homogenous habitats, females move according to within-bush parameters. The direction of movement is random unless the females perform phonotaxis.

Phonotaxis

At every time step, the probability that a female samples her acoustic landscape is dependent on her mating status and is given by a value of 1 for virgin females and a value of 0.35 for mated females (Modak et al., 2021). Once the female samples her landscape, the probability that a female responds to a signal of perceived intensity I is given by:

45 dB is the minimum SPL below which females are guaranteed to not respond phonotactically. 60 dB is the minimum threshold above which females are guaranteed to respond phonotactically. The implementation of this threshold response is supported by empirical data (Deb and Balakrishnan, 2014).

If a female is simultaneously exposed to two or more sounds whose perceived intensity is greater than 45 dB, it preferentially moves towards the loudest one. In line with laboratory experiments, females were additionally assumed to have an amplitude resolution of 3 dB meaning that they do not distinguish between two sounds whose perceived intensities differed by less than 3 dB (Deb et al., 2020). If exposed to two or more sounds whose perceived intensities are less than 3 dB apart, they choose one at random and move towards it.

Since females show high bush fidelity (see the Movement section under Females), they first listen for sound sources within their own bush, and only listen for sounds from neighbouring bushes if there are no sufficiently loud sounds being emitted from their own bush. For calculating the perceived SPL of sound(s) for a listener positioned on a different bush, we sum the SPL of each individual present in the bush (after making the appropriate conversions from dB SPL to Pa for addition) to obtain the total intensity of sound emanating from the bush. Thus, we implicitly assume that males call in phase such that calls constructively interfere with each other. The perceived SPL is then calculated after accounting for the transmission loss due to the distance between the centres of the two bushes. Individuals from bushes that are greater than 10 m away from the focal bush are considered inaudible to the female since sounds from 10 m away are sub-threshold due to transmission loss.

Mating

From empirical observations and experiments, we know that females do not reject males upon encounter, and typically mate with ∼90% probability (Modak et al., 2021). Consequently, we assumed in our model that two individuals of the opposite sex mate if they are within 5 cm (equivalent to ∼5 body lengths) of each other. If there are multiple males within 5 cm of a single female, she chooses a male randomly for mating. While mating, neither individual can move or engage in any other matings and males cannot emit calls. A mating event lasts for six time steps, which corresponds to a typical mating period (i.e. 1 h in real-time). At the end of the mating period, we re-position the female to a random location within the landscape and assign it a ‘mated’ status.

The model was written in Python 3.9.7 (Van Rossum and Drake, 1995), and utilised the Python packages NumPy 1.20.3 (Harris et al., 2020), SciPy 1.7.1 (Virtanen et al., 2020), random 3.9.7, and multiprocessing 0.70.14. (McKerns et al., 2011).

Quantitative inferences

We employed linear regressions to quantify the effect of proportion of bafflers, population density and spatial structure. The mating success of a tactic in a simulation run was measured as the average number of mates obtained by a male performing that tactic, which implies that it is possible for males to have a mating success greater than 1. Since our simulations were stochastic, there was significant variation between individual runs of a simulation for any fixed set of parameters. We thus averaged the mating success of each tactic over 100 independent runs for each combination of these three variables: proportion of bafflers, population density, and habitat type. To check for the effect of the proportion of bafflers and population density on the mating success of baffling, we performed a linear regression analysis, with the mating success of bafflers as the response variable and the proportion of bafflers, population density and (proportion of bafflers)*(population density) set as fixed factors. The linear model was employed separately for the homogenous landscape as well as the structured landscapes with different bush densities. To compare the mating success of the three ARTs, we constructed a linear model with mating success as the response variable, and the ARTs and proportion of bafflers in the population as fixed factors. The linear model was applied separately to simulation results from each combination of population density and habitat type. We then compared the intercepts for each of the ARTs to check for differences in mating success of the ARTs. Regression analyses and plotting were done in R 4.1.1 (R core team, 2013). Plotting used the ggplot2 (Wickham, 2016) package.

We clarify that the purpose of the statistical analyses is not to check the statistical significance as done in an empirical study; this is because by increasing the number of replications within our simulations, we can obtain an arbitrarily high degree of statistical significance (e.g. measured via P-values). Instead, our focus here is to quantify the relative strengths of mating success as a function of conditions such as population density and habitat structure.

Effect of movement on mating success

To assess the role of movement on the mating success of ARTs in structured and homogenous habitats, we performed simulations where all males, irrespective of their tactic, remained stationary for the duration of the simulation. We used the population density d=1 for such simulations. For structured habitats, we used a bush density of ρ=1. We then compared the results of these simulations to those where calling and silent males were allowed to move for each type of habitat.

Sensitivity analysis

We varied some of the parameters to check if our model is sensitive to these parameters. The results of the sensitivity analysis are presented as supplementary information (Figs S4-S13).

We acknowledge infrastructure support from DST-FIST and DBT-IISc partnership program Phase II, Government of India. We thank the BEAGLE cluster facility at the Indian Institute of Science, Bengaluru for providing computational resources. We thank the anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments that have helped improve the manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: M.A.S., A.S.B., V.G., R.B.; Methodology: M.A.S., A.S.B., V.G., R.B.; Software: M.A.S., A.S.B.; Validation: M.A.S., A.S.B.; Formal analysis: M.A.S., A.S.B.; Investigation: M.A.S., A.S.B.; Data curation: M.A.S., A.S.B.; Writing - original draft: M.A.S., A.S.B.; Writing - review & editing: M.A.S., A.S.B., V.G., R.B.; Visualization: M.A.S., A.S.B.; Supervision: V.G., R.B.

Funding

M.A.S. is supported by the Prime Minister's Research Fellowship from the Ministry of Education, Government of India. A.S.B. was supported by a Kishore Vaigyanik Protsahan Yojana (KVPY) fellowship (ID: SX-1711025) from the Department of Science and Technology, Ministry of Science and Technology, India. Open Access funding provided by Indian Institute of Science, Bengaluru. Deposited in PMC for immediate release.

Data availability

All data are provided in the site mentioned in the Open research statement. All relevant data can be found within the article and its supplementary information.

Open research statement

All data and codes generated in the production of this manuscript can be found on a public GitHub repository at: https://github.com/ThePandalorian/Sadiq_et_al_2023_oecanthus_ART_IbM

Alonzo
,
S. H.
and
Calsbeek
,
R.
(
2010
).
The unstable dynamics of multiple alternative reproductive tactics
.
J. Evol. Biol.
23
,
2614
-
2624
.
Bisazza
,
A.
and
Pilastro
,
A.
(
1997
).
Small male mating advantage and reversed size dimorphism in poeciliid fishes
.
J. Fish Biol.
50
,
397
-
406
.
Brandt
,
E. E.
,
Duke
,
S.
,
Wang
,
H.
and
Mhatre
,
N.
(
2023
).
The ground offers acoustic efficiency gains for crickets and other calling animals
.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
120
,
e2302814120
.
Brockmann
,
H. J.
(
2001
).
The evolution of alternative strategies and tactics
.
Adv. Study Behav.
30
,
1
-
51
.
Brockmann
,
H. J.
,
Grafen
,
A.
and
Dawkins
,
R.
(
1979
).
Evolutionarily stable nesting Stategy in digger wasps
.
J. Theor. Biol
77
,
473
-
496
.
Cade
,
W. H.
(
1981
).
Alternative male strategies: genetic differences in crickets
.
Science
212
,
563
-
564
.
Calsbeek
,
R.
,
Alonzo
,
S. H.
,
Zamudio
,
K.
and
Sinervo
,
B.
(
2002
).
Sexual selection and alternative mating behaviours generate demographic stochasticity in small populations
.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B: Biol. Sci.
269
,
157
-
164
.
Deb
,
R.
(
2017
).
Mate choice, mate sampling and baffling behaviour in the tree cricket Oecanthus henryi
.
PhD thesis
,
Indian Institute of Science
,
Bengaluru
. .
Deb
,
R.
and
Balakrishnan
,
R.
(
2014
).
The opportunity for sampling: The ecological context of female mate choice
.
Behav. Ecol.
25
,
967
-
974
.
Deb
,
R.
,
Modak
,
S.
and
Balakrishnan
,
R.
(
2020
).
Baffling: a condition-dependent alternative mate attraction strategy using self-made tools in tree crickets
.
Proc. R. Soc. B
287
,
20202229
.
Eadie
,
J. M.
and
Fryxell
,
J. M.
(
1992
).
Density dependence, frequency dependence, and alternative nesting strategies in goldeneyes
.
Am. Nat.
140
,
621
-
641
.
Endler
,
J. A.
(
1992
).
Signals, signal conditions, and the direction of evolution
.
Am. Nat.
139
,
S125
-
S153
.
Gray
,
S. M.
,
Dill
,
L. M.
,
Tantu
,
F. Y.
,
Loew
,
E. R.
,
Herder
,
F.
and
McKinnon
,
J. S.
(
2008
).
Environment-contingent sexual selection in a colour polymorphic fish
.
Proc. R. Soc. B
275
,
1785
-
1791
.
Gross
,
M. R.
(
1996
).
Tactics: diversity within sexes
.
Trends Ecol. Evol.
11
,
92
-
98
.
Guttal
,
V.
and
Couzin
,
I. D.
(
2010
).
Social interactions, information use, and the evolution of collective migration
.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA
107
,
16172
-
16177
.
Han
,
T. A.
,
Traulsen
,
A.
and
Gokhale
,
C. S.
(
2012
).
On equilibrium properties of evolutionary multi-player games with random payoff matrices
.
Theor. Popul. Biol.
81
,
264
-
272
.
Harris
,
C. R.
,
Millman
,
K. J.
,
van der Walt
,
S. J.
,
Gommers
,
R.
,
Virtanen
,
P.
,
Cournapeau
,
D.
,
Wieser
,
E.
,
Taylor
,
J.
,
Berg
,
S.
,
Smith
,
N. J.
et al. 
(
2020
).
Array programming with NumPy
.
Nature
585
,
357
-
362
.
Hissmann
,
K.
(
1990
).
Strategies of mate finding in the European field cricket (Gryllus campestris) at different population densities: a field study
.
Ecol. Entomol.
15
,
281
-
291
.
Jain
,
M.
and
Balakrishnan
,
R.
(
2012
).
Does acoustic adaptation drive vertical stratification? A test in a tropical cricket assemblage
.
Behav. Ecol.
23
,
343
-
354
.
Joshi
,
J.
,
Couzin
,
I. D.
,
Levin
,
S. A.
and
Guttal
,
V.
(
2017
).
Mobility can promote the evolution of cooperation via emergent self-assortment dynamics
.
PLoS Comput. Biol.
13
,
e1005732
.
Kirkup
,
B. C.
and
Riley
,
M. A.
(
2004
).
Antibiotic-mediated antagonism leads to a bacterial game of rock–paper–scissors in vivo
.
Nature
428
,
412
-
414
.
Kleyla
,
P. C.
and
Dodson
,
G.
(
1978
).
Calling behavior and spatial distribution of two species of mole crickets in the field
.
Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am.
71
,
602
-
604
.
Kokko
,
H.
and
Rankin
,
D. J.
(
2006
).
Lonely hearts or sex in the city? Density-dependent effects in mating systems
.
Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci.
361
,
319
-
334
.
Korona
,
R.
,
Nakatsu
,
C. H.
,
Forney
,
L. J.
and
Lenski
,
R. E.
(
1994
).
Evidence for multiple adaptive peaks from populations of bacteria evolving in a structured habitat
.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
91
,
9037
-
9041
.
Krüger
,
O.
,
Lindström
,
J.
and
Amos
,
W.
(
2001
).
Maladaptive mate choice maintained by heterozygote advantage
.
Evolution
55
,
1207
-
1214
.
Mazer
,
S. J.
and
Damuth
,
J.
(
2001
).
Nature and causes of variation
. In:
Evolutionary Ecology: Concepts and Case Studies
(ed.
C. W.
Fox
,
D. A.
Roff
and
D. J.
Fairbairn
), pp.
3
-
15
.
Oxford
:
Oxford University Press
.
McKerns
,
M.
,
Strand
,
L.
,
Sullivan
,
T.
,
Fang
,
A.
and
Aivazis
,
M.
(
2011
).
Building a Framework for Predictive Science
. In:
Proceedings of the 10th Python in Science Conference
.
Michalczyk
,
L.
,
Dudziak
,
M.
,
Radwan
,
J.
and
Tomkins
,
J. L.
(
2018
).
Fitness consequences of threshold trait expression subject to environmental cues
.
Proc. R. Soc. B
285
,
20180783
.
Mizumoto
,
N.
and
Dobata
,
S.
(
2018
).
The optimal movement patterns for mating encounters with sexually asymmetric detection ranges
.
Sci. Rep.
8
,
3356
.
Modak
,
S.
,
Brown
,
W. D.
and
Balakrishnan
,
R.
(
2021
).
Decoupling of female phonotaxis and mating propensity in a tree cricket
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
75
.
Oliveira
,
R. F.
,
Taborsky
,
M.
and
Brockmann
,
H. J.
(Eds.). (
2008
).
Alternative Reproductive Tactics: An Integrative Approach
.
Cambridge University Press
.
Park
,
H. J.
,
Pichugin
,
Y.
and
Traulsen
,
A.
(
2020
).
Why is cyclic dominance so rare?
Elife
9
,
e57857
.
Pittman
,
S. E.
,
Jendrek
,
A. L.
,
Price
,
S. J.
and
Dorcas
,
M. E.
(
2008
).
Habitat selection and site fidelity of Cope's Gray Treefrog (Hyla chrysoscelis) at the aquatic-terrestrial ecotone
.
J. Herpetol.
42
,
378
-
385
.
Prozesky-Schulze
,
L.
,
Prozesky
,
O. P. M.
,
Anderson
,
F.
and
Van Der Merwe
,
G. J. J.
(
1975
).
Use of a self-made sound baffle by a tree cricket
.
Nature
255
,
142
-
143
.
R Core Team
,
R.
(
2013
).
R: A language and environment for statistical computing
.
Rotenberry
,
J. T.
and
Zuk
,
M.
(
2016
).
Alternative reproductive tactics in context: how demography, ecology, and behavior affect male mating success
.
Am. Nat.
188
,
582
-
588
.
Rowell
,
G. A.
and
Cade
,
W. H.
(
1993
).
Simulation of alternative male reproductive behavior: calling and satellite behavior in field crickets
.
Ecol. Model.
65
,
265
-
280
.
Roy
,
S.
and
Chattopadhyay
,
J.
(
2007
).
Towards a resolution of ‘the paradox of the plankton’: a brief overview of the proposed mechanisms
.
Ecol. Complex
4
,
26
-
33
.
Sadiq
,
M. A.
,
Torsekar
,
V. R.
and
Balakrishnan
,
R.
(
2023
).
To call or not to call: Persistence of flexible alternative reproductive tactics in a tree cricket
.
bioRxiv, 2023-04
.
Shuster
,
S. M.
and
Wade
,
M. J.
(
1991
).
Equal mating success among male reproductive strategies in a marine isopod
.
Nature
350
,
608
-
610
.
Sinervo
,
B.
and
Lively
,
C. M.
(
1996
).
The rock–paper–scissors game and the evolution of alternative male strategies
.
Nature
380
,
240
-
243
.
Stein
,
A.
,
Gerstner
,
K.
and
Kreft
,
H.
(
2014
).
Environmental heterogeneity as a universal driver of species richness across taxa, biomes and spatial scales
.
Ecol. Lett.
17
,
866
-
880
.
Szolnoki
,
A.
,
Mobilia
,
M.
,
Jiang
,
L. L.
,
Szczesny
,
B.
,
Rucklidge
,
A. M.
and
Perc
,
M.
(
2014
).
Cyclic dominance in evolutionary games: a review
.
J. R. Soc. Interface
11
,
20140735
.
Takahashi
,
Y.
and
Kawata
,
M.
(
2013
).
A comprehensive test for negative frequency-dependent selection
.
Popul. Ecol.
55
,
499
-
509
.
Tomkins
,
J. L.
,
Hazel
,
W. N.
,
Penrose
,
M. A.
,
Radwan
,
J. W.
and
LeBas
,
N. R.
(
2011
).
Habitat complexity drives experimental evolution of a conditionally expressed secondary sexual trait
.
Curr. Biol.
21
,
569
-
573
.
Torsekar
,
V. R.
and
Balakrishnan
,
R.
(
2020
).
Sex differences in alternative reproductive tactics in response to predation risk in tree crickets
.
Funct. Ecol.
34
,
2326
-
2337
.
Torsekar
,
V. R.
,
Isvaran
,
K.
and
Balakrishnan
,
R.
(
2019
).
Is the predation risk of mate-searching different between the sexes?
Evol. Ecol.
33
,
329
-
343
.
Ursell
,
T.
(
2021
).
Structured environments foster competitor coexistence by manipulating interspecies interfaces
.
PLoS Comput. Biol.
17
,
1
-
23
.
Van Rossum
,
G.
and
Drake
,
F. L.
(
1995
).
Python Reference Manual
, vol.
111
, pp.
1
-
52
.
Amsterdam
:
Centrum voor Wiskunde en Informatica
.
Virtanen
,
P.
,
Gommers
,
R.
,
Oliphant
,
T. E.
,
Haberland
,
M.
,
Reddy
,
T.
,
Cournapeau
,
D.
,
Vázquez-Baeza
,
Y.
,
Peterson
,
P.
,
Weckesser
,
W.
,
Bright
,
J.
et al. 
(
2020
).
SciPy 1.0: fundamental algorithms for scientific computing in Python
.
Nat. Methods
17
,
261
-
272
.
Walker
,
T. J.
(
1957
).
Specificity in the response of female tree crickets (orthoptera, gryllidae, oecanthinae) to calling songs of the Males1
.
Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am.
50
,
626
-
636
.
Walker
,
S. E.
and
Cade
,
W. H.
(
2003
).
A simulation model of the effects of frequency dependence, density dependence and parasitoid flies on the fitness of male field crickets
.
Ecol. Model.
169
,
119
-
130
.
Wickham
,
H.
(
2016
).
Programming with ggplot2. Ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis
, pp.
241
-
253
.
Springer, Cham
.
Wolf
,
L. L.
and
Waltz
,
E. C.
(
1993
).
Alternative mating tactics in male white-faced dragonflies: experimental evidence for a behavioural assessment ESS
.
Anim. Behav.
46
,
325
-
334
.
Zahnd
,
S.
,
Fontcuberta
,
A.
,
Koken
,
M.
,
Cardinaux
,
A.
and
Chapuisat
,
M.
(
2021
).
Fine-scale habitat heterogeneity favours the coexistence of supergene-controlled social forms in Formica selysi
.
BMC Ecol. Evol.
21
,
24
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

Supplementary information